Linguistics Linguistics
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > Linguistics    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 
Webpage

Linguistics : Publications since January 2023

List all publications in the database.    :recent first  alphabetical  combined listing:
%% Andrews, Edna   
@article{fds372813,
   Author = {Eierud, C and Michael, A and Banks, D and Andrews,
             E},
   Title = {Resting-state functional connectivity in lifelong
             musicians},
   Journal = {Psychoradiology},
   Volume = {3},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/psyrad/kkad003},
   Abstract = {Background: It has been postulated that musicianship can
             lead to enhanced brain and cognitive reserve, but the neural
             mechanisms of this effect have been poorly understood.
             Lifelong professional musicianship in conjunction with novel
             brain imaging techniques offers a unique opportunity to
             examine brain network differences between musicians and
             matched controls. Objective: In this study we aim to
             investigate how resting-state functional networks (FNs)
             manifest in lifelong active musicians. We will evaluate the
             FNs of lifelong musicians and matched healthy controls using
             resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging.
             Methods: We derive FNs using the data-driven independent
             component analysis approach and analyze the functional
             network connectivity (FNC) between the default mode (DMN),
             sensory-motor (SMN), visual (VSN), and auditory (AUN)
             networks. We examine whether the linear regressions between
             FNC and age are different between the musicians and the
             control group. Results: The age trajectory of average FNC
             across all six pairs of FNs shows significant differences
             between musicians and controls. Musicians show an increase
             in average FNC with age while controls show a decrease (P =
             0.013). When we evaluated each pair of FN, we note that in
             musicians FNC values increased with age in DMN-AUN, DMN-VSN,
             and SMN-VSN and in controls FNC values decreased with age in
             DMN-AUN, DMN-SMN, AUN-SMN, and SMN-VSN. Conclusion: This
             result provides early evidence that lifelong musicianship
             may contribute to enhanced brain and cognitive reserve.
             Results of this study are preliminary and need to be
             replicated with a larger number of participants.},
   Doi = {10.1093/psyrad/kkad003},
   Key = {fds372813}
}


%% Baran, Dominika M   
@article{fds369809,
   Author = {N/A},
   Title = {Anti-genderism in Global Nationalist Movements},
   Journal = {Gender and Language},
   Number = {Special issue},
   Publisher = {Equinox Publishing},
   Editor = {Tebaldi, C and Baran, D},
   Year = {2023},
   Key = {fds369809}
}

@article{fds369810,
   Author = {Baran, D},
   Title = {Defending Christianity from the “rainbow plague”:
             Historicized narratives of nationhood in rightwing
             antigenderist discourses in Poland},
   Journal = {Gender and Language},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {1},
   Publisher = {Equinox Publishing},
   Year = {2023},
   Key = {fds369810}
}

@article{fds369811,
   Author = {Baran, D},
   Title = {American immigrants and English},
   Booktitle = {In The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of World
             Englishes},
   Year = {2023},
   Key = {fds369811}
}


%% Bergelson, Elika   
@article{fds376090,
   Author = {Lavechin, M and Metais, M and Titeux, H and Boissonnet, A and Copet, J and Riviere, M and Bergelson, E and Cristia, A and Dupoux, E and Bredin,
             H},
   Title = {Brouhaha: Multi-Task Training for Voice Activity Detection,
             Speech-to-Noise Ratio, and C50 Room Acoustics
             Estimation},
   Journal = {2023 IEEE Automatic Speech Recognition and Understanding
             Workshop, ASRU 2023},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9798350306897},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ASRU57964.2023.10389718},
   Abstract = {Most automatic speech processing systems register degraded
             performance when applied to noisy or reverberant speech. But
             how can one tell whether speech is noisy or reverberant? We
             propose Brouhaha, a neural network jointly trained to
             extract speech/non-speech segments, speech-to-noise ratios,
             and C50 room acoustics from single-channel recordings.
             Brouhaha is trained using a data-driven approach in which
             noisy and reverberant audio segments are synthesized. We
             first evaluate its performance and demonstrate that the
             proposed multi-task regime is beneficial. We then present
             two scenarios illustrating how Brouhaha can be used on
             naturally noisy and reverberant data: 1) to investigate the
             errors made by a speaker diarization model (pyannote.audio);
             and 2) to assess the reliability of an automatic speech
             recognition model (Whisper from OpenAI). Both our pipeline
             and a pretrained model are open source and shared with the
             speech community.},
   Doi = {10.1109/ASRU57964.2023.10389718},
   Key = {fds376090}
}

@article{fds367924,
   Author = {Bulgarelli, F and Bergelson, E},
   Title = {Talker variability is not always the right noise: 14 month
             olds struggle to learn dissimilar word-object pairs under
             talker variability conditions.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {227},
   Pages = {105575},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105575},
   Abstract = {Seminal work by Stager & Werker (1997) finds that
             14-month-olds can rapidly learn two word-object pairings if
             the words are distinct (e.g. "neem" and "lif") but not
             similar (e.g. the minimal pair "bih" and "dih"). More
             recently, studies have found that adding talker variability
             during exposure to new word-object pairs lets 14-month-olds
             succeed on the more challenging minimal pair task,
             presumably due to talker variability highlighting the
             "relevant" consistencies between the similar words (Rost &
             McMurray, 2009; Galle et al., 2015; Hohle et al., 2020). It
             remains an open question, however, whether talker
             variability would be similarly useful for learning new
             word-object pairings when the words themselves are already
             distinct, or whether instead this extra variability may
             extinguish learning due to increased task demands. We find
             evidence for the latter. Namely, in our sample of 54
             English-learning 14-month-olds, training infants on two
             word-object pairings (e.g. "neem" with a dog toy and "lof"
             with a kitchen tool) only led them to notice when the words
             and objects were switched if they were trained with
             single-speaker identical word tokens. When the training
             featured talker variability (from one or multiple talkers)
             infants failed to learn the pairings. We suggest that when
             talker variability is not necessary to highlight the
             invariant differences between similar words, it may actually
             increase task difficulty, making it harder for infants to
             determine what to attend to in the earliest phases of word
             learning.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105575},
   Key = {fds367924}
}

@article{fds370372,
   Author = {Liu, J and Hilton, CB and Bergelson, E and Mehr, SA},
   Title = {Language experience predicts music processing in a
             half-million speakers of fifty-four languages.},
   Journal = {Current biology : CB},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {1916-1925.e4},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2023.03.067},
   Abstract = {Tonal languages differ from other languages in their use of
             pitch (tones) to distinguish words. Lifelong experience
             speaking and hearing tonal languages has been argued to
             shape auditory processing in ways that generalize beyond the
             perception of linguistic pitch to the perception of pitch in
             other domains like music. We conducted a meta-analysis of
             prior studies testing this idea, finding moderate evidence
             supporting it. But prior studies were limited by mostly
             small sample sizes representing a small number of languages
             and countries, making it challenging to disentangle the
             effects of linguistic experience from variability in music
             training, cultural differences, and other potential
             confounds. To address these issues, we used web-based
             citizen science to assess music perception skill on a global
             scale in 34,034 native speakers of 19 tonal languages (e.g.,
             Mandarin, Yoruba). We compared their performance to 459,066
             native speakers of other languages, including 6
             pitch-accented (e.g., Japanese) and 29 non-tonal languages
             (e.g., Hungarian). Whether or not participants had taken
             music lessons, native speakers of all 19 tonal languages had
             an improved ability to discriminate musical melodies on
             average, relative to speakers of non-tonal languages. But
             this improvement came with a trade-off: tonal language
             speakers were also worse at processing the musical beat. The
             results, which held across native speakers of many diverse
             languages and were robust to geographic and demographic
             variation, demonstrate that linguistic experience shapes
             music perception, with implications for relations between
             music, language, and culture in the human
             mind.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2023.03.067},
   Key = {fds370372}
}

@article{fds373687,
   Author = {Meylan, SC and Foushee, R and Wong, NH and Bergelson, E and Levy,
             RP},
   Title = {How adults understand what young children
             say.},
   Journal = {Nature human behaviour},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {2111-2125},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01698-3},
   Abstract = {Children's early speech often bears little resemblance to
             that of adults, and yet parents and other caregivers are
             able to interpret that speech and react accordingly. Here we
             investigate how adult listeners' inferences reflect
             sophisticated beliefs about what children are trying to
             communicate, as well as how children are likely to pronounce
             words. Using a Bayesian framework for modelling spoken word
             recognition, we find that computational models can replicate
             adult interpretations of children's speech only when they
             include strong, context-specific prior expectations about
             the messages that children will want to communicate. This
             points to a critical role of adult cognitive processes in
             supporting early communication and reveals how children can
             actively prompt adults to take actions on their behalf even
             when they have only a nascent understanding of the adult
             language. We discuss the wide-ranging implications of the
             powerful listening capabilities of adults for theories of
             first language acquisition.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41562-023-01698-3},
   Key = {fds373687}
}

@article{fds374572,
   Author = {Bergelson, E and Soderstrom, M and Schwarz, I-C and Rowland, CF and Ramírez-Esparza, N and R Hamrick and L and Marklund, E and Kalashnikova, M and Guez, A and Casillas, M and Benetti, L and Alphen,
             PV and Cristia, A},
   Title = {Everyday language input and production in 1,001 children
             from six continents.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {120},
   Number = {52},
   Pages = {e2300671120},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2300671120},
   Abstract = {Language is a universal human ability, acquired readily by
             young children, who otherwise struggle with many basics of
             survival. And yet, language ability is variable across
             individuals. Naturalistic and experimental observations
             suggest that children's linguistic skills vary with factors
             like socioeconomic status and children's gender. But which
             factors really influence children's day-to-day language use?
             Here, we leverage speech technology in a big-data approach
             to report on a unique cross-cultural and diverse data set:
             >2,500 d-long, child-centered audio-recordings of 1,001 2-
             to 48-mo-olds from 12 countries spanning six continents
             across urban, farmer-forager, and subsistence-farming
             contexts. As expected, age and language-relevant clinical
             risks and diagnoses predicted how much speech (and
             speech-like vocalization) children produced. Critically, so
             too did adult talk in children's environments: Children who
             heard more talk from adults produced more speech. In
             contrast to previous conclusions based on more limited
             sampling methods and a different set of language proxies,
             socioeconomic status (operationalized as maternal education)
             was not significantly associated with children's productions
             over the first 4 y of life, and neither were gender or
             multilingualism. These findings from large-scale
             naturalistic data advance our understanding of which factors
             are robust predictors of variability in the speech behaviors
             of young learners in a wide range of everyday
             contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.2300671120},
   Key = {fds374572}
}

@article{fds375505,
   Author = {Campbell, E and Casillas, R and Bergelson, E},
   Title = {The role of vision in the acquisition of words: Vocabulary
             development in blind toddlers.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Pages = {e13475},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13475},
   Abstract = {What is vision's role in driving early word production? To
             answer this, we assessed parent-report vocabulary
             questionnaires administered to congenitally blind children
             (N = 40, Mean age = 24 months [R: 7-57 months]) and
             compared the size and contents of their productive
             vocabulary to those of a large normative sample of sighted
             children (N = 6574). We found that on average, blind
             children showed a roughly half-year vocabulary delay
             relative to sighted children, amid considerable variability.
             However, the content of blind and sighted children's
             vocabulary was statistically indistinguishable in word
             length, part of speech, semantic category, concreteness,
             interactiveness, and perceptual modality. At a finer-grained
             level, we also found that words' perceptual properties
             intersect with children's perceptual abilities. Our findings
             suggest that while an absence of visual input may initially
             make vocabulary development more difficult, the content of
             the early productive vocabulary is largely resilient to
             differences in perceptual access. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS:
             Infants and toddlers born blind (with no other diagnoses)
             show a 7.5 month productive vocabulary delay on average,
             with wide variability. Across the studied age range
             (7-57 months), vocabulary delays widened with age. Blind
             and sighted children's early vocabularies contain similar
             distributions of word lengths, parts of speech, semantic
             categories, and perceptual modalities. Blind children (but
             not sighted children) were more likely to say visual words
             which could also be experienced through other
             senses.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.13475},
   Key = {fds375505}
}

@article{fds375235,
   Author = {Laing, C and Bergelson, E},
   Title = {Analyzing the effect of sibling number on input and output
             in the first 18 months.},
   Journal = {Infancy : the official journal of the International Society
             on Infant Studies},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {175-195},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/infa.12578},
   Abstract = {Prior research suggests that across a wide range of
             cognitive, educational, and health-based measures,
             first-born children outperform their later-born peers.
             Expanding on this literature using naturalistic
             home-recorded data and parental vocabulary reports, we find
             that early language outcomes vary by number of siblings in a
             sample of 43 English-learning U.S. children from mid-to-high
             socioeconomic status homes. More specifically, we find that
             children in our sample with two or more-but not one-older
             siblings had smaller productive vocabularies at 18 months,
             and heard less input from caregivers across several measures
             than their peers with less than two siblings. We discuss
             implications regarding what infants experience and learn
             across a range of family sizes in infancy.},
   Doi = {10.1111/infa.12578},
   Key = {fds375235}
}

@article{fds376037,
   Author = {Moore, C and Bergelson, E},
   Title = {Wordform variability in infants' language environment and
             its effects on early word learning.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {245},
   Pages = {105694},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105694},
   Abstract = {Most research regarding early word learning in English tends
             to make the simplifying assumption that there exists a
             one-to-one mapping between concrete objects and their
             labels. In the current work, we provide evidence that runs
             counter to this assumption, aligning English with more
             morphologically-rich languages. We suggest that even in a
             morphologically-poor language like English, real world
             language input to infants does not provide tidy 1-to-1
             mappings. Instead, infants encounter many variant wordforms
             for familiar nouns (e.g. dog∼doggy∼dogs). We explore
             this wordform variability in 44 English-learning infants'
             naturalistic environments using a longitudinal corpus of
             infant-available speech. We look at both the frequency and
             composition of wordform variability. We find two broad
             categories of variability: referent-changing alterations,
             where words were pluralized or compounded (e.g.
             coat∼raincoats); and wordplay, where words changed form
             without a notable change in referent (e.g. bird∼birdie).
             We further find that wordplay occurs with a limited number
             of lemmas that are usually early-learned, high-frequency,
             and shorter. When looking at all wordform variability, we
             find that individual words with higher levels of wordform
             variability are learned earlier than words with fewer
             wordforms, over and above the effect of frequency.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105694},
   Key = {fds376037}
}


%% Mazuka, Reiko   
@article{fds371461,
   Author = {Choi, Y and Nam, M and Yamane, N and Mazuka, R},
   Title = {Lack of early sensitivity and gradual emergence of native
             phoneme categories: A pattern from underrepresented language
             learners.},
   Journal = {Developmental Science},
   Pages = {e13422},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13422},
   Abstract = {Perceptual narrowing of speech perception supposes that
             young infants can discriminate most speech sounds early in
             life. During the second half of the first year, infants'
             phonetic sensitivity is attuned to their native phonology.
             However, supporting evidence for this pattern comes
             primarily from learners from a limited number of regions and
             languages. Very little evidence has accumulated on infants
             learning languages spoken in Asia, which accounts for most
             of the world's population. The present study examined the
             developmental trajectory of Korean-learning infants'
             sensitivity to a native stop contrast during the first year
             of life. The Korean language utilizes unusual voiceless
             three-way stop categories, requiring target categories to be
             derived from tight phonetic space. Further, two of these
             categories-lenis and aspirated-have undergone a diachronic
             change in recent decades as the primary acoustic cue for
             distinction has shifted among modern speakers. Consequently,
             the input distributions of these categories are mixed across
             speakers and speech styles, requiring learners to build
             flexible representations of target categories along these
             variations. The results showed that among the three age
             groups-4-6 months, 7-9 months, and 10-12 months-we tested,
             only 10-12-month-olds showed weak sensitivity to the two
             categories, suggesting that robust discrimination is not in
             place by the end of the first year. The study adds scarcely
             represented data, lending additional support for the lack of
             early sensitivity and prolonged emergence of native
             phonology that are inconsistent with learners of predominant
             studies and calls for more diverse samples to verify the
             generality of the typical perceptual narrowing pattern.
             RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We investigated Korean-learning
             infants' developmental trajectory of native phoneme
             categories and whether they show the typical perceptual
             narrowing pattern. Robust discrimination did not appear
             until 12 months, suggesting that Korean infants' native
             phonology is not stabilized by the end of the first year.
             The prolonged emergence of sensitivity could be due to
             restricted phonetic space and input variations but suggests
             the possibility of a different developmental trajectory. The
             current study contributes scarcely represented
             Korean-learning infants' phonetic discrimination data to the
             speech development field.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.13422},
   Key = {fds371461}
}


%% Neander, Karen   
@article{fds219963,
   Author = {K.L. Neander},
   Title = {"Toward an Informational Teleosemantics"},
   Booktitle = {Millikan and Her Critics},
   Editor = {Justine Kingsbury},
   Keywords = {Teleolosemantics, functions, information, representation,
             content, distal content, Millikan, Papineau.},
   Abstract = {This paper argues that there are response functions. Systems
             can have the function to produce one thing in response to
             another. This has consequences for the kind of
             teleosemantics that can be offered. Contrary to claims made
             by Millikan and Papineau, sensory representations can have
             contents that are determined by the functions of sensory
             systems to respond to stimuli in certain ways. This paper
             further explores these implications and offers a
             teleosemantic and yet informational theory for sensory
             representations. It further offers a solution to the problem
             of distal content.},
   Key = {fds219963}
}


%% Rubin, David C.   
@article{fds371560,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Hoyle, RH and Munkholm Møller and D and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Digital daydreaming: Introducing the spontaneous smartphone
             checking scale},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {147-160},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.4034},
   Abstract = {Smartphones are a ubiquitous part of many people's lives,
             but little is known about their impact on everyday thought
             processes. Here we introduce the spontaneous smartphone
             checking scale (SSCS)—which measures the tendency to
             direct attention toward one's smartphone, unpreceded by
             external prompts (e.g., notifications, or alerts) and with
             no specific conscious goal in mind, as a parallel to
             mind-wandering directed toward internal thoughts. The SSCS
             showed good psychometric properties and construct validity.
             It separated from measures of daydreaming and mind-wandering
             by not loading on dimensions related to self-consciousness,
             reflection, and rumination, but instead loading highly on a
             factor associated with other aspects of digital
             communication and concerns about public appearance on social
             media. This suggests that spontaneous smartphone checking
             serves different mental and social functions than internally
             generated spontaneous thought processes. We discuss possible
             long-term effects of spontaneous smartphone checking taking
             up time for internally generated spontaneous
             thoughts.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.4034},
   Key = {fds371560}
}

@article{fds371743,
   Author = {Shan, Y and Yan, S and Jia, Y and Hu, Y and Rubin, DC and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {The Properties of Involuntary and Voluntary Autobiographical
             Memories in Chinese Patients with Depression and Healthy
             Individuals},
   Journal = {Cognitive Therapy and Research},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-023-10353-0},
   Abstract = {Background: Research on depression has largely focused on
             negative intrusive memories with little research on general
             involuntary memories as they occur in everyday life. In
             addition, all studies have been conducted on Western
             participants, and there are no studies on general
             involuntary memory in Eastern patients with depression.
             Methods: Thirty Chinese patients with depression and 30
             healthy controls completed a memory diary in which they
             recorded a total of 10 involuntary and 10 voluntary
             memories. They were requested to fill out corresponding
             questionnaires of involuntary and voluntary memories as
             well. Results: Both patients with depression and healthy
             controls reported involuntary memories that had a more
             negative impact, were more specific, and were associated
             with more maladaptive emotion regulation when compared to
             voluntary memories. For both retrieval modes, patients with
             depression reported more negative and fewer positive
             memories, more negative and less positive mood impact, more
             avoidance, rumination, worry, negative interpretation, and
             less positive interpretation in response to the memories.
             Patients with depression rated their memories as more
             central, less specific, and rehearsed more frequently.
             Negative mood impact and maladaptive emotion regulation
             associated with involuntary memories were amplified in
             depression. Conclusions: These findings support the view
             that general involuntary memories could be a potential
             target to promote the treatment for depression.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10608-023-10353-0},
   Key = {fds371743}
}

@article{fds370410,
   Author = {Allé, MC and Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory and the self on the psychosis
             continuum: investigating their relationship with positive-
             and negative-like symptoms.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {518-529},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2173236},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memory is severely impaired in
             schizophrenia, but previous work has largely treated both as
             unitary concepts. Here, we examined how various dimensions
             of autobiographical memory relate to different aspects of
             psychosis. Participants were recruited from the general
             population (Study 1, N = 264) and a university subject
             pool (Study 2, N = 305). We examined different measures
             of autobiographical memory and self (i.e., involuntary
             memory, autobiographical recollection, self-knowledge and
             self-awareness), at the trait level in Study 1 and both
             trait and state levels in Study 2, as a function of
             positive-and negative-like symptoms of psychosis. Across
             both studies, positive and negative dimensions of psychosis
             were found to be related to an increase in involuntary
             memories (i.e., the spontaneous recall of personal
             memories), and to lower self-concept clarity and insight.
             Positive and negative dimensions of psychosis correlated
             differently with autobiographical recollection
             characteristics, measured at both trait (Studies 1 and 2)
             and state levels (Study 2). Positive-like symptoms (in
             particular hallucination-proneness) showed a stronger and
             more consistent pattern of correlations than negative-like
             symptoms. These findings call for a dimensional approach to
             the relationship between autobiographical memory and
             psychosis symptoms in clinical and non-clinical individuals,
             to better understand the breakdown of autobiographical
             memory in the psychopathology of psychosis.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2173236},
   Key = {fds370410}
}

@article{fds370313,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Bell, CF},
   Title = {Tonic immobility (freezing) during sexual and physical
             assaults produces stronger memory effects than other
             characteristics of the assaults.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {678-688},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2188642},
   Abstract = {Tonic immobility (TI) is a phylogenetically conserved,
             passive, obligatory defense mechanism commonly engaged
             during sexual and physical assaults. During TI, people
             become immobile while remaining conscious and later
             reexperience intrusive memories of both their assault and of
             its accompanying immobility. Here we show that this
             well-studied biological process has powerful effects on
             memory and other processes. Participants had experienced
             either a serious sexual (<i>n</i> = 234) or physical
             (<i>n</i> = 137) assault. For both the assault and its
             accompanying immobility, the standard measure of the
             peritraumatic severity of TI correlated between .40 and .65
             with post-assault effects on memory, including memory of the
             assault and memory of the immobility, the two memory-based
             self-concept measures of self-blame and event centrality,
             and post-assault anxiety and depression. The correlations
             with TI were much higher than other peritraumatic
             characteristics commonly used to predict and describe
             posttraumatic effects in assaults and other traumas. The
             results suggest that TI should be considered for a broader,
             more biologically based and ecologically valid understanding
             of the effects of trauma on memory and memory-based
             reactions.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2188642},
   Key = {fds370313}
}

@article{fds372264,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Bell, CF},
   Title = {Using shame to extend Martin Conway's self-memory
             system.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Pages = {1-12},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2241673},
   Abstract = {We extend Conway's self-memory system by adding theory and
             data from shame, an emotion that disrupts the internalised
             ideals of society needed for a positive self-concept. The
             event that caused 273 undergraduates their greatest amount
             of shame was analyzed; 66% were not very negative except for
             producing shame. Ratings of post-event effects, including
             two measure of self (self-perceived weakness, and centrality
             to identity) and four clinical symptoms (intrusions,
             avoidance, anxiety, and depression), were attributed
             separately to the remembered event, behaviour during the
             event, and shame from the event. The effects of shame were
             generally as large as the those of the event and larger than
             those of the behaviour, demonstrating the importance of
             shame's effects. The Tonic Immobility Scale (TIS), which
             measures tonic immobility (i.e., freezing), was obtained for
             the event that produced the most tonic immobility but that
             was not the event that caused the most shame. The post-event
             symptoms measured on the event that caused the most shame
             and the TIS correlated highly, suggesting that shame and
             tonic immobility may belong to a cluster of phylogenetically
             conserved submissive defensive mechanisms that could account
             for effects currently attributed to goals in self-memory
             systems.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2241673},
   Key = {fds372264}
}

@article{fds371260,
   Author = {Gehrt, TB and Nielsen, NP and Hoyle, RH and Rubin, DC and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {Narrative identity does not predict well-being when
             controlling for emotional valence.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1051-1061},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2218632},
   Abstract = {Narrative identity refers to a person's internalized and
             evolving life story. It is a rapidly growing research field,
             motivated by studies showing a unique association with
             well-being. Here we show that this association disappears
             when controlling for the emotional valence of the stories
             told and individuals' general experience of autobiographical
             memory. Participants (<i>N </i>= 235) wrote their life
             story and completed questionnaires on their general
             experience of autobiographical memory and several dimensions
             of well-being and affect. Participants' life stories were
             coded for standard narrative identity variables, including
             agency and communion. When controlling for emotional valence
             of the life story, the general experience of
             autobiographical memory was a significant predictor of most
             well-being measures, whereas agency was a predictor of one
             variable only and communion of none. These findings
             contradict the claim of an incremental association between
             narrative identity and well-being, and have important
             theoretical and practical implications for narrative
             identity as an outcome measure in interventions.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2218632},
   Key = {fds371260}
}


%% Tomasello, Michael   
@article{fds362755,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Gerdemann, SC and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Evidence for a developmental shift in the motivation
             underlying helping in early childhood.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e13253},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13253},
   Abstract = {We investigated children's positive emotions as an indicator
             of their underlying prosocial motivation. In Study 1, 2-,
             and 5-year-old children (N = 64) could either help an
             individual or watch as another person provided help.
             Following the helping event and using depth sensor imaging,
             we measured children's positive emotions through changes in
             postural elevation. For 2-year-olds, helping the individual
             and watching another person help was equally rewarding;
             5-year-olds showed greater postural elevation after actively
             helping. In Study 2, 5-year-olds' (N = 59) positive
             emotions following helping were greater when an audience was
             watching. Together, these results suggest that 2-year-old
             children have an intrinsic concern that individuals be
             helped whereas 5-year-old children have an additional,
             strategic motivation to improve their reputation by
             helping.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.13253},
   Key = {fds362755}
}

@article{fds365125,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Social cognition and metacognition in great apes: a
             theory.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {25-35},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01662-0},
   Abstract = {Twenty-five years ago, at the founding of this journal,
             there existed only a few conflicting findings about great
             apes' social-cognitive skills (theory of mind). In the 2 ½
             decades since, we have discovered that great apes understand
             the goals, intentions, perceptions, and knowledge of others,
             and they use this knowledge to their advantage in
             competitive interactions. Twenty-five years ago there
             existed basically no studies on great apes' metacognitive
             skills. In the 2 ½ decades since, we have discovered that
             great apes monitor their uncertainty and base their
             decisions on that, or else decide to gather more information
             to make better decisions. The current paper reviews the past
             25 years of research on great ape social cognition and
             metacognition and proposes a theory about how the two are
             evolutionarily related.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-022-01662-0},
   Key = {fds365125}
}

@article{fds371813,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Thielhelm, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Five-year-old children show cooperative preferences for
             faces with white sclera.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {225},
   Pages = {105532},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105532},
   Abstract = {The cooperative eye hypothesis posits that human eye
             morphology evolved to facilitate cooperation. Although it is
             known that young children prefer stimuli with eyes that
             contain white sclera, it is unknown whether white sclera
             influences children's perception of a partner's
             cooperativeness specifically. In the current studies, we
             used an online methodology to present 5-year-old children
             with moving three-dimensional face models in which facial
             morphology was manipulated. Children found "alien" faces
             with human eyes more cooperative than faces with dark sclera
             (Study 2) but not faces with enlarged irises (Study 1). For
             more human-like faces (Study 3), children found human eyes
             more cooperative than either enlarged irises or dark sclera
             and found faces with enlarged irises cuter (but not more
             cooperative) than eyes with dark sclera. Together, these
             results provide strong support for the cooperative eye
             hypothesis.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105532},
   Key = {fds371813}
}

@misc{fds371506,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Having Intentions, Understanding Intentions, and
             Understanding Communicative Intentions},
   Pages = {63-75},
   Booktitle = {Developing Theories of Intention: Social Understanding and
             Self-Control},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780805831412},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003417927-5},
   Abstract = {This chapter looks at a major cause and a major consequence
             of the 9-month social-cognitive revolution; and both of
             these also concern infant intentionality. It argues that
             young children’s understanding of other persons as
             intentional agents results in large part from newly emerging
             forms of intentionality in their own sensory-motor actions.
             The chapter explores young children’s understanding of a
             special type of intention that emerges directly on the heels
             of the 9-month revolution, namely, communicative intentions.
             Intentional agents have goals and make active choices among
             behavioral means for attaining those goals. Important,
             intentional agents also make active choices about what they
             pay attention to in pursuing those goals. ntentional agents
             have goals and make active choices among behavioral means
             for attaining those goals. Important, intentional agents
             also make active choices about what they pay attention to in
             pursuing those goals.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781003417927-5},
   Key = {fds371506}
}

@article{fds367773,
   Author = {Colle, L and Grosse, G and Behne, T and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Just teasing! - Infants' and toddlers' understanding of
             teasing interactions and its effect on social
             bonding.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {231},
   Pages = {105314},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105314},
   Abstract = {The current study investigates infants' and toddlers'
             understanding of teasing interactions and its effect on
             subsequent social interactions. Teasing is a special kind of
             social interaction due to its dual nature: It consists of a
             slightly provocative contingent action accompanied by
             positive ostensive emotional cues. Teasing thus presents an
             especially interesting test case to inform us about young
             children's abilities to deal with complex social intentions.
             In a first experiment, we looked at 9-, 12-, and
             18-month-old infants' ability to understand and
             differentiate a teasing intention from a trying intention
             and a refuse intention. We found that by 12 months of age,
             infants react differently (gaze, reach) and by 18 months
             they smile more in reaction to the Tease condition. In the
             second experiment, we tested 13-, 20- and 30-month-old
             children in closely matched purely playful and teasing
             situations. We also investigated potential social effects of
             teasing interactions on a subsequent affiliation sequence.
             Twenty- and 30-month-old children smile more in the Teasing
             than in the Play condition. For the 30-month-old toddlers,
             additionally, number of laughs is much higher in the Tease
             than in the Play condition. No effect on affiliation could
             be found. Thus, from very early in development, infants and
             toddlers are able to differentiate teasing from
             superficially similar but serious behavior and from around
             18 months of age they enjoy it more. Infants and toddlers
             are able to process a complex social intention like teasing.
             Findings are discussed regarding infant and toddler
             intention understanding abilities.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105314},
   Key = {fds367773}
}

@article{fds368903,
   Author = {Schäfer, M and B M Haun and D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's consideration of collaboration and merit when
             making sharing decisions in private.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {228},
   Pages = {105609},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105609},
   Abstract = {Young children share equally when they acquire resources
             through collaboration with a partner, yet it is unclear
             whether they do so because in such contexts resources are
             encountered as common and distributed in front of the
             recipient or because collaboration promotes a sense of
             work-based fairness. In the current studies, 5- and
             8-year-old children from Germany (N = 193) acquired
             resources either by working individually alongside or by
             collaborating with a peer. After finding out that the
             partner's container was empty, they decided in private
             whether they wanted to donate some resources to the peer.
             When both partners had worked with equal efforts (Study 1),
             children shared more after collaboration than after
             individual work. When one partner had worked with much more
             effort than the other (Study 2), children shared more with a
             harder-working partner than with a less-working partner
             independently of whether they had collaborated or worked
             individually. Younger children were more generous than older
             children, in particular after collaboration. These findings
             support the view that collaboration promotes a genuine sense
             of fairness in young children, but they also indicate that
             merit-based notions of fairness in the context of work may
             develop independently of collaboration, at least by the
             beginning of middle childhood and in Western
             societies.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105609},
   Key = {fds368903}
}

@article{fds370629,
   Author = {Benozio, A and House, BR and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Apes reciprocate food positively and negatively.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {290},
   Number = {1998},
   Pages = {20222541},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.2541},
   Abstract = {Reciprocal food exchange is widespread in human societies
             but not among great apes, who may view food mainly as a
             target for competition. Understanding the similarities and
             differences between great apes' and humans' willingness to
             exchange food is important for our models regarding the
             origins of uniquely human forms of cooperation. Here, we
             demonstrate in-kind food exchanges in experimental settings
             with great apes for the first time. The initial sample
             consisted of 13 chimpanzees and 5 bonobos in the control
             phases, and the test phases included 10 chimpanzees and 2
             bonobos, compared with a sample of 48 human children aged 4
             years. First, we replicated prior findings showing no
             spontaneous food exchanges in great apes. Second, we
             discovered that when apes believe that conspecifics have
             'intentionally' transferred food to them, positive
             reciprocal food exchanges (food-for-food) are not only
             possible but reach the same levels as in young children
             (approx. 75-80%). Third, we found that great apes engage in
             negative reciprocal food exchanges (no-food for no-food) but
             to a lower extent than children. This provides evidence for
             reciprocal food exchange in great apes in experimental
             settings and suggests that while a potential mechanism of
             <i>fostering</i> cooperation (via positive reciprocal
             exchanges) may be shared across species, a stabilizing
             mechanism (via negative reciprocity) is not.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2022.2541},
   Key = {fds370629}
}

@article{fds373982,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A Shared Intentionality Account of Uniquely Human Social
             Bonding.},
   Journal = {Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the
             Association for Psychological Science},
   Pages = {17456916231201795},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/17456916231201795},
   Abstract = {Many mechanisms of social bonding are common to all
             primates, but humans seemingly have developed some that are
             unique to the species. These involve various kinds of
             interactive experiences-from taking a walk together to
             having a conversation-whose common feature is the triadic
             sharing of experience. Current theories of social bonding
             have no explanation for why humans should have these unique
             bonding mechanisms. Here we propose a shared intentionality
             account of uniquely human social bonding. Humans evolved to
             participate with others in unique forms of cooperative and
             communicative activities that both depend on and create
             shared experience. Sharing experience in these activities
             causes partners to feel closer because it allows them to
             assess their partner's cooperative competence and motivation
             toward them and because the shared representations created
             during such interactions make subsequent cooperative
             interactions easier and more effective.},
   Doi = {10.1177/17456916231201795},
   Key = {fds373982}
}

@article{fds370890,
   Author = {Vasil, J and Moore, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Thought and language: association of groupmindedness with
             young English-speaking children’s production of
             pronouns},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {516-538},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01427237231169398},
   Abstract = {Shared intentionality theory posits that at age 3, children
             expand their conception of plural agency to include 3- or
             more-person groups. We sought to determine whether this
             conceptual shift is detectable in children’s pronoun use.
             We report the results of a series of Bayesian hierarchical
             generative models fitted to 479 English-speaking
             children’s first-person plural, first-person singular,
             second-person, third-person plural, and third-person
             singular pronouns. As a proportion of pronouns, children
             used more first-person plural pronouns, only, after 3;0
             compared to before. Additionally, children used more 1pp.
             pronouns when their mothers used more 1pp. pronouns. As a
             proportion of total utterances, all pronoun classes were
             used more often as children aged. These findings suggest
             that a shift in children’s social conceptualizations at
             age 3 is reflected in their use of 1pp. pronouns.},
   Doi = {10.1177/01427237231169398},
   Key = {fds370890}
}

@article{fds374236,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Differences in the Social Motivations and Emotions of Humans
             and Other Great Apes.},
   Journal = {Human nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {588-604},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0},
   Abstract = {Humans share with other mammals and primates many social
             motivations and emotions, but they are also much more
             cooperative than even their closest primate relatives. Here
             I review recent comparative experiments and analyses that
             illustrate humans' species-typical social motivations and
             emotions for cooperation in comparison with those of other
             great apes. These may be classified most generally as (i)
             'you > me' (e.g., prosocial sympathy, informative and
             pedagogical motives in communication); (ii) 'you = me'
             (e.g., feelings of mutual respect, fairness, resentment);
             (iii) 'we > me' (e.g., feelings of obligation and guilt);
             and (iv) 'WE (in the group) > me' (e.g., in-group
             loyalty and conformity to norms, shame, and many in-group
             biases). The existence of these species-typical and
             species-universal motivations and emotions provides
             compelling evidence for the importance of cooperative
             activities in the human species.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0},
   Key = {fds374236}
}

@article{fds374400,
   Author = {Vasil, J and Price, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Thought and language: Effects of group-mindedness on young
             children's interpretation of exclusive we.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14049},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated whether age-related changes
             in the conceptualization of social groups influences
             interpretation of the pronoun we. Sixty-four 2- and
             4-year-olds (N = 29 female, 50 White-identifying) viewed
             scenarios in which it was ambiguous how many puppets
             performed an activity together. When asked who performed the
             activity, a speaker puppet responded, "We did!" In one
             condition, the speaker was near one and distant from another
             puppet, implying a dyadic interpretation of we. In another
             condition, the speaker was distant from both, thus pulling
             for a group interpretation. In the former condition, 2- and
             4-year-olds favored the dyadic interpretation. In the latter
             condition, only 4-year-olds favored the group
             interpretation. Age-related conceptual development "expands"
             the set of conceivable plural person referents.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.14049},
   Key = {fds374400}
}

@article{fds374171,
   Author = {Katz, T and Kushnir, T and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children are eager to take credit for prosocial acts, and
             cost affects this tendency.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {237},
   Pages = {105764},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105764},
   Abstract = {We report two experiments on children's tendency to enhance
             their reputations through communicative acts. In the
             experiments, 4-year-olds (N = 120) had the opportunity to
             inform a social partner that they had helped him in his
             absence. In a first experiment, we pitted a prosocial act
             ("Let's help clean up for Doggie!") against an instrumental
             act ("Let's move these out of our way"). Children in the
             prosocial condition were quicker to inform their partner of
             the act and more likely to protest when another individual
             was given credit for it. In a second experiment, we
             replicated the prosocial condition but with a new
             manipulation: high-cost versus low-cost helping. We
             manipulated both the language surrounding cost (i.e., "This
             will be pretty tough to clean up" vs. "It will be really
             easy to clean this up") and how difficult the task itself
             was. As predicted, children in the high-cost condition were
             quicker to inform their partner of the act and more likely
             to take back credit for it. These results suggest that even
             4-year-old children make active attempts to elicit positive
             reputational judgments for their prosocial acts, with cost
             as a moderating factor.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105764},
   Key = {fds374171}
}

@article{fds374401,
   Author = {Winter Née Grocke and P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {From what I want to do to what we decided to do:
             5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, honor their agreements
             with peers.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {239},
   Pages = {105811},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105811},
   Abstract = {Sometimes we have a personal preference but we agree with
             others to follow a different course of action. In this
             study, 3- and 5-year-old children (N = 160) expressed a
             preference for playing a game one way and were then
             confronted with peers who expressed a different preference.
             The experimenter then either got the participants to agree
             with the peers explicitly or just shrugged her shoulders and
             moved on. The children were then left alone to play the game
             unobserved. Only the older children stuck to their agreement
             to play the game as the peers wished. These results suggest
             that by 5 years of age children's sense of commitment to
             agreements is strong enough to override their personal
             preferences.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105811},
   Key = {fds374401}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Linguistics * Faculty * Librarian * Staff * Reload * Login