Linguistics Faculty Database
Linguistics
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > Linguistics > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of David C. Rubin    :chronological  alphabetical  by type listing:

%%    
@article{fds371260,
   Author = {Gehrt, TB and Nielsen, NP and Hoyle, RH and Rubin, DC and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {Narrative identity does not predict well-being when
             controlling for emotional valence.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1051-1061},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2218632},
   Abstract = {Narrative identity refers to a person's internalized and
             evolving life story. It is a rapidly growing research field,
             motivated by studies showing a unique association with
             well-being. Here we show that this association disappears
             when controlling for the emotional valence of the stories
             told and individuals' general experience of autobiographical
             memory. Participants (<i>N </i>= 235) wrote their life
             story and completed questionnaires on their general
             experience of autobiographical memory and several dimensions
             of well-being and affect. Participants' life stories were
             coded for standard narrative identity variables, including
             agency and communion. When controlling for emotional valence
             of the life story, the general experience of
             autobiographical memory was a significant predictor of most
             well-being measures, whereas agency was a predictor of one
             variable only and communion of none. These findings
             contradict the claim of an incremental association between
             narrative identity and well-being, and have important
             theoretical and practical implications for narrative
             identity as an outcome measure in interventions.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2218632},
   Key = {fds371260}
}

@article{fds372264,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Bell, CF},
   Title = {Using shame to extend Martin Conway's self-memory
             system.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Pages = {1-12},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2241673},
   Abstract = {We extend Conway's self-memory system by adding theory and
             data from shame, an emotion that disrupts the internalised
             ideals of society needed for a positive self-concept. The
             event that caused 273 undergraduates their greatest amount
             of shame was analyzed; 66% were not very negative except for
             producing shame. Ratings of post-event effects, including
             two measure of self (self-perceived weakness, and centrality
             to identity) and four clinical symptoms (intrusions,
             avoidance, anxiety, and depression), were attributed
             separately to the remembered event, behaviour during the
             event, and shame from the event. The effects of shame were
             generally as large as the those of the event and larger than
             those of the behaviour, demonstrating the importance of
             shame's effects. The Tonic Immobility Scale (TIS), which
             measures tonic immobility (i.e., freezing), was obtained for
             the event that produced the most tonic immobility but that
             was not the event that caused the most shame. The post-event
             symptoms measured on the event that caused the most shame
             and the TIS correlated highly, suggesting that shame and
             tonic immobility may belong to a cluster of phylogenetically
             conserved submissive defensive mechanisms that could account
             for effects currently attributed to goals in self-memory
             systems.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2241673},
   Key = {fds372264}
}

@article{fds370313,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Bell, CF},
   Title = {Tonic immobility (freezing) during sexual and physical
             assaults produces stronger memory effects than other
             characteristics of the assaults.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {678-688},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2188642},
   Abstract = {Tonic immobility (TI) is a phylogenetically conserved,
             passive, obligatory defense mechanism commonly engaged
             during sexual and physical assaults. During TI, people
             become immobile while remaining conscious and later
             reexperience intrusive memories of both their assault and of
             its accompanying immobility. Here we show that this
             well-studied biological process has powerful effects on
             memory and other processes. Participants had experienced
             either a serious sexual (<i>n</i> = 234) or physical
             (<i>n</i> = 137) assault. For both the assault and its
             accompanying immobility, the standard measure of the
             peritraumatic severity of TI correlated between .40 and .65
             with post-assault effects on memory, including memory of the
             assault and memory of the immobility, the two memory-based
             self-concept measures of self-blame and event centrality,
             and post-assault anxiety and depression. The correlations
             with TI were much higher than other peritraumatic
             characteristics commonly used to predict and describe
             posttraumatic effects in assaults and other traumas. The
             results suggest that TI should be considered for a broader,
             more biologically based and ecologically valid understanding
             of the effects of trauma on memory and memory-based
             reactions.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2188642},
   Key = {fds370313}
}

@article{fds370410,
   Author = {Allé, MC and Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory and the self on the psychosis
             continuum: investigating their relationship with positive-
             and negative-like symptoms.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {518-529},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2173236},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memory is severely impaired in
             schizophrenia, but previous work has largely treated both as
             unitary concepts. Here, we examined how various dimensions
             of autobiographical memory relate to different aspects of
             psychosis. Participants were recruited from the general
             population (Study 1, N = 264) and a university subject
             pool (Study 2, N = 305). We examined different measures
             of autobiographical memory and self (i.e., involuntary
             memory, autobiographical recollection, self-knowledge and
             self-awareness), at the trait level in Study 1 and both
             trait and state levels in Study 2, as a function of
             positive-and negative-like symptoms of psychosis. Across
             both studies, positive and negative dimensions of psychosis
             were found to be related to an increase in involuntary
             memories (i.e., the spontaneous recall of personal
             memories), and to lower self-concept clarity and insight.
             Positive and negative dimensions of psychosis correlated
             differently with autobiographical recollection
             characteristics, measured at both trait (Studies 1 and 2)
             and state levels (Study 2). Positive-like symptoms (in
             particular hallucination-proneness) showed a stronger and
             more consistent pattern of correlations than negative-like
             symptoms. These findings call for a dimensional approach to
             the relationship between autobiographical memory and
             psychosis symptoms in clinical and non-clinical individuals,
             to better understand the breakdown of autobiographical
             memory in the psychopathology of psychosis.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2023.2173236},
   Key = {fds370410}
}

@article{fds371560,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Hoyle, RH and Munkholm Møller and D and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Digital daydreaming: Introducing the spontaneous smartphone
             checking scale},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {147-160},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.4034},
   Abstract = {Smartphones are a ubiquitous part of many people's lives,
             but little is known about their impact on everyday thought
             processes. Here we introduce the spontaneous smartphone
             checking scale (SSCS)—which measures the tendency to
             direct attention toward one's smartphone, unpreceded by
             external prompts (e.g., notifications, or alerts) and with
             no specific conscious goal in mind, as a parallel to
             mind-wandering directed toward internal thoughts. The SSCS
             showed good psychometric properties and construct validity.
             It separated from measures of daydreaming and mind-wandering
             by not loading on dimensions related to self-consciousness,
             reflection, and rumination, but instead loading highly on a
             factor associated with other aspects of digital
             communication and concerns about public appearance on social
             media. This suggests that spontaneous smartphone checking
             serves different mental and social functions than internally
             generated spontaneous thought processes. We discuss possible
             long-term effects of spontaneous smartphone checking taking
             up time for internally generated spontaneous
             thoughts.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.4034},
   Key = {fds371560}
}

@article{fds371743,
   Author = {Shan, Y and Yan, S and Jia, Y and Hu, Y and Rubin, DC and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {The Properties of Involuntary and Voluntary Autobiographical
             Memories in Chinese Patients with Depression and Healthy
             Individuals},
   Journal = {Cognitive Therapy and Research},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-023-10353-0},
   Abstract = {Background: Research on depression has largely focused on
             negative intrusive memories with little research on general
             involuntary memories as they occur in everyday life. In
             addition, all studies have been conducted on Western
             participants, and there are no studies on general
             involuntary memory in Eastern patients with depression.
             Methods: Thirty Chinese patients with depression and 30
             healthy controls completed a memory diary in which they
             recorded a total of 10 involuntary and 10 voluntary
             memories. They were requested to fill out corresponding
             questionnaires of involuntary and voluntary memories as
             well. Results: Both patients with depression and healthy
             controls reported involuntary memories that had a more
             negative impact, were more specific, and were associated
             with more maladaptive emotion regulation when compared to
             voluntary memories. For both retrieval modes, patients with
             depression reported more negative and fewer positive
             memories, more negative and less positive mood impact, more
             avoidance, rumination, worry, negative interpretation, and
             less positive interpretation in response to the memories.
             Patients with depression rated their memories as more
             central, less specific, and rehearsed more frequently.
             Negative mood impact and maladaptive emotion regulation
             associated with involuntary memories were amplified in
             depression. Conclusions: These findings support the view
             that general involuntary memories could be a potential
             target to promote the treatment for depression.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10608-023-10353-0},
   Key = {fds371743}
}

@article{fds363909,
   Author = {McNally, RJ and Berntsen, D and Brewin, CR and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Are memories of sexual trauma fragmented? A post publication
             discussion among Richard J. McNally, Dorthe Berntsen, Chris
             R. Brewin and David C. Rubin.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {658-660},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2022.2061135},
   Abstract = {Following the publication of his article on whether memories
             of trauma in sexual assault victims are fragmented (McNally,
             2022), McNally moderated a discussion between Chris R.
             Brewin and David C. Rubin/Dorthe Berntsen whose perspectives
             on memory fragmentation were cited by McNally. The
             discussion clarified their contrasting viewpoints on this
             controversy.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2022.2061135},
   Key = {fds363909}
}

@article{fds355485,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A conceptual space for episodic and semantic
             memory.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {464-477},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-021-01148-3},
   Abstract = {I propose a model that places episodic, semantic, and other
             commonly studied forms of memory into the same conceptual
             space. The space is defined by three dimensions required for
             Tulving's episodic and semantic memory. An implicit-explicit
             dimension contrasts both episodic and semantic memory with
             common forms of implicit memory. A self-reference dimension
             contrasts episodes that occurred to one person with semantic
             knowledge. A scene dimension contrasts episodes that
             occurred in specific contexts with context-free semantic
             information. The three dimensions are evaluated against
             existing behavioral and neural evidence to evaluate both the
             model and the concepts underlying the study of human memory.
             Unlike a hierarchy, which has properties specific to each
             category, the dimensions have properties that extend
             throughout the conceptual space. Thus, the properties apply
             to all forms of existing and yet-to-be-discovered memory
             within the space. Empty locations in the proposed space are
             filled with existing phenomena that lack a clear place in
             current theories of memory, including reports of
             episodic-like memories for events reported to but not
             witnessed by a person, fictional narrative accounts, déjà
             vu, and implicit components contributing to personality, the
             self, and autobiographical memory.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-021-01148-3},
   Key = {fds355485}
}

@article{fds355486,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Properties of autobiographical memories are reliable and
             stable individual differences.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {210},
   Pages = {104583},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104583},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memory research typically focuses on
             individual memories with variability in individual
             participants' responses serving as error variance.
             Integrating individual-difference and experimental
             approaches demonstrated that properties of autobiographical
             memories are stable individual differences with stable
             patterns of correlations. In two sessions approximately one
             week apart, different cues were used to prompt seven
             autobiographical memories. Each memory was rated on 12
             properties including visual imagery, emotional intensity,
             narrative coherence, reliving, and past rehearsals. In two
             studies with samples from different populations (Ns of 200
             and 160), each property had a high reliability in both
             sessions (median α = .90), and the mean of each property
             averaged over seven memories correlated highly with itself
             over sessions (median r = .72). Multiple regressions
             predicting three properties from Session 1 with the
             remaining nine properties of Session 2 and exploratory
             factor analyses yielded solutions consistent with
             expectations from studies of individual memories. Moreover,
             the correlation matrices of the 12 properties across studies
             and sessions were extremely similar. Thus, separate
             sessions, cues, samples, and properties provided
             generalizable data about individual differences in
             autobiographical memory. Practical, theoretical, and
             methodological implications include that individual
             differences in memory affect: life stories and narrative
             structure internal to events, stable clinical syndromes and
             symptoms, experimental results previously attributed to the
             properties of individual memories, and the confidence people
             have in the accuracy of their autobiographical and episodic
             memories.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104583},
   Key = {fds355486}
}

@article{fds359270,
   Author = {Gehrt, TB and Nielsen, NP and Hoyle, RH and Rubin, DC and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {Individual Differences in Autobiographical Memory: The
             Autobiographical Recollection Test Predicts Ratings of
             Specific Memories Across Cueing Conditions},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {85-96},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2021.07.004},
   Abstract = {The Autobiographical Recollection Test (ART; Berntsen et
             al., 2019) measures individual differences in
             autobiographical memory. We here examined whether the ART
             correlates with characteristics of people's specific
             autobiographical memories. Participants (Ns ≥ 475)
             completed the ART and rated recollective qualities of
             autobiographical memories cued by words (Study 1), by
             positive and negative emotional valence (Study 2), and by
             future and past temporal direction (Study 3). Scores on the
             ART consistently correlated with recollective qualities of
             specific memories and future thoughts, both immediately and
             after a 1-week delay. The magnitude of these correlations
             was at the same level as the correlations between individual
             memory items, underscoring the ability of the ART, as a
             trait measure to predict ratings of individual memories. The
             findings support the construct validity of the ART and
             demonstrate that people's evaluation of their
             autobiographical memory, in general, is reliably related to
             how they remember specific events.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2021.07.004},
   Key = {fds359270}
}

@article{fds352998,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Self-Concept Focus: A Tendency to Perceive Autobiographical
             Events as Central to Identity},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {576-586},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.001},
   Abstract = {Self-Concept Focus is a 15-item measure of the disposition
             to make autobiographical memories central to one's
             self-concept and thus to rehearse them more frequently. In
             studies with 400 MTurk workers and 299 undergraduates,
             Self-Concept Focus had high reliability (α ∼.83), good
             test-retest stability (r = .66) that did not decline between
             7 and 54 days, good psychometric properties in a bifactor
             measurement model, and results that replicated across
             studies. A factor analysis of nine measures relevant to
             self-concept resulted in two factors. Self-Concept Focus,
             Reflection, Reappraisal and Private Self-Consciousness all
             loaded on one factor, suggesting an underlying dimension of
             elaborative rehearsal of memories and emotion regulation.
             Self-Concept Focus also correlated with PTSD symptoms for a
             single very negative event and thus with opportunities to
             modify such memory-based symptoms. Given its association
             with elaborative rehearsal and emotion regulation,
             Self-Concept Focus has potential applied relevance in
             clinical, forensic and consumer contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.001},
   Key = {fds352998}
}

@article{fds348066,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The ability to recall scenes is a stable individual
             difference: Evidence from autobiographical
             remembering.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {197},
   Pages = {104164},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104164},
   Abstract = {Four behavioral studies (ns ~ 200 to 400) extended neural
             studies of ventral stream damage and fMRI activation and
             behavioral studies of scene recall conducted on individual
             memories to individual differences in normal populations.
             Ratings of scene and contents were made on one set of
             autobiographical memories. Ratings of reliving, vividness,
             belief, emotional intensity, and temporal specificity were
             made on different memories. Thus, correlations between these
             ratings were due to variability in the participants, not the
             events remembered. Scene correlated more highly than
             contents with reliving, vividness, belief, and emotional
             intensity but not temporal specificity. Scene correlated
             more highly than other visual imagery tests with reliving,
             vividness, and belief. Scene correlated with individual
             differences tests of episodic memories and future events
             more highly than it did with tests of semantic memory and
             spatial navigation abilities. Moreover, scene had high
             test-retest correlations measured at periods of up to one
             month. The ability to recall scenes is a stable disposition,
             with both convergent and divergent validity, which predicts
             basic qualities of autobiographical memories. A Scene Recall
             Imagery Test is introduced.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104164},
   Key = {fds348066}
}

@article{fds348842,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Academic Forgetting},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {52-57},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.12.003},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.12.003},
   Key = {fds348842}
}

@article{fds345433,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Hoyle, RH and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The Autobiographical Recollection Test (ART): A Measure of
             Individual Differences in Autobiographical
             Memory.},
   Journal = {Journal of applied research in memory and
             cognition},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {305-318},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.06.005},
   Abstract = {We introduce the Autobiographical Recollection Test (ART) to
             examine individual differences in how well people think they
             remember personal events. The ART comprises seven
             theoretically motivated and empirically supported
             interrelated aspects of recollecting autobiographical
             memories: <i>reliving, vividness, visual imagery, scene,
             narrative coherence, life-story relevance,</i> and
             <i>rehearsal</i>. Desirable psychometric properties of the
             ART are established by confirmatory factor analyses
             demonstrating that items probing each of the seven
             components form well-defined, yet highly correlated, factors
             that are indicators of a single underlying second-order
             factor. The ART shows high test-retest reliability over
             delays averaging three weeks and correlates meaningfully
             with a test of different categories of memory. Overall, the
             findings document that autobiographical recollection is a
             dimension that varies among individuals. The ART forms a
             reliable and easily administered autobiographical memory
             test that will help to integrate autobiographical memory
             research with fields generally concerned with individual
             differences, such as health and personality
             psychology.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.06.005},
   Key = {fds345433}
}

@article{fds339651,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Deffler, SA and Umanath, S},
   Title = {Scenes enable a sense of reliving: Implications for
             autobiographical memory.},
   Volume = {183},
   Pages = {44-56},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.024},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memory has been defined by the
             phenomenological properties of reliving, vividness, and
             belief that an event occurred. Neuropsychological damage
             that results in the inability to recall the layout of a
             scene also results in amnesia suggesting a possible milder
             effect in people without such neurological damage. Based on
             this and other observations, we hypothesized that the degree
             to which the layout of a scene is recalled will correlate
             positively with ratings of reliving, vividness, and belief,
             and will explain more variance in multiple regressions than
             recalling the scene's contents. We also hypothesized that a
             lack of layout underlies nonspecific autobiographical
             memories which are common in aging, future events, and
             clinical disorders, whereas currently such memories are most
             commonly measured by reports of extended duration. We tested
             these theory-driven novel hypotheses in three studies to
             replicate our results. In each study, approximately 200
             participants rated the layout, content, and other properties
             of personal events. Correlational analyses in each study and
             a structural equation model for the combined studies provide
             strong support for the role of mental scene construction in
             an integrative neurocognitive approach to clarify cognitive
             theory and clinical phenomena.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.024},
   Key = {fds339651}
}

@article{fds337418,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D and Deffler, SA and Brodar,
             K},
   Title = {Self-narrative focus in autobiographical events: The effect
             of time, emotion, and individual differences.},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {63-75},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0850-4},
   Abstract = {Individuals may take a self-narrative focus on the meaning
             of personal events in their life story, rather than viewing
             the events in isolation. Using the Centrality of Event Scale
             (CES; Berntsen & Rubin in Behaviour Research and Therapy,
             44, 219-231, 2006) as our measure, we investigated
             self-narrative focus as an individual differences variable
             in addition to its established role as a measure of
             individual events. Three studies, with 169, 182, and 190
             participants had 11, 10, and 11 different events varied
             across the dimensions of remembered past versus imagined
             future, distance from the present, and valence. Imagined
             future events, events more distant from the present, and
             positive events all had increased self-narrative focus, in
             agreement with published theories and findings. Nonetheless,
             CES ratings for individual events correlated positively with
             each other within individuals (r ~ .30) and supported a
             single factor solution. These results are consistent with a
             stable individual differences tendency toward self-narrative
             focus that transcends single events. Thus, self-narrative
             focus is both a response whereby people relate individual
             events to their life story and identity and an individual
             differences variable that is stable over a range of events.
             The findings are discussed in relation to narrative measures
             of autobiographical reasoning.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-018-0850-4},
   Key = {fds337418}
}

@misc{fds347786,
   Author = {Rubin, D},
   Title = {Placing autobiographical memory in a general memory
             organization},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press.},
   Year = {2019},
   Key = {fds347786}
}

@article{fds337343,
   Author = {Gehrt, TB and Berntsen, D and Hoyle, RH and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Psychological and clinical correlates of the Centrality of
             Event Scale: A systematic review.},
   Volume = {65},
   Pages = {57-80},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.006},
   Abstract = {The Centrality of Event Scale (CES) was introduced to
             examine the extent to which a traumatic or stressful event
             is perceived as central to an individual's identity and life
             story, and how this relates to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
             (PTSD) symptoms. In addition, the CES has been examined in
             relation to a range of other conditions and dispositions. We
             present a systematic review of the correlates of the CES.
             Results from 92 publications resulted in 25 measurement
             categories in the six theoretical domains of trauma,
             negative affect and distress, autobiographical memory,
             personality, positive affect, and gender. The mean weighted
             correlations of the 25 measurement categories ranged from
             -.17 to .55, with standard errors from .01 to .02, allowing
             us to distinguish empirically among effects. Consistent with
             the theoretical motivation for the CES and predictions
             predating the review, the CES correlated positively with a
             range of measures, correlating most highly with measures
             related to trauma, PTSD, grief, and autobiographical memory.
             The findings show that the CES probes aspects of
             autobiographical memory of broad relevance to clinical
             disorders, and with specific implications for theories of
             PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.006},
   Key = {fds337343}
}

@article{fds335706,
   Author = {Hall, SA and Brodar, KE and LaBar, KS and Berntsen, D and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Neural responses to emotional involuntary memories in
             posttraumatic stress disorder: Differences in timing and
             activity.},
   Volume = {19},
   Pages = {793-804},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.05.009},
   Abstract = {Background:Involuntary memories are a hallmark symptom of
             posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but studies of the
             neural basis of involuntary memory retrieval in
             posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are sparse. The study
             of the neural correlates of involuntary memories of
             stressful events in PTSD focuses on the voluntary retrieval
             of memories that are sometimes recalled as intrusive
             involuntary memories, not on involuntary retrieval while
             being scanned. Involuntary memory retrieval in controls has
             been shown to elicit activity in the parahippocampal gyrus,
             precuneus, inferior parietal cortex, and posterior midline
             regions. However, it is unknown whether involuntary memories
             are supported by the same mechanisms in PTSD. Because
             previous work has shown that both behavioral and neural
             responsivity is slowed in PTSD, we examined the
             spatiotemporal dynamics of the neural activity underlying
             negative and neutral involuntary memory retrieval.
             Methods:Twenty-one individuals with PTSD and 21 non-PTSD,
             trauma-exposed controls performed an involuntary memory
             task, while undergoing a functional magnetic resonance
             imaging scan. Environmental sounds served as cues for
             well-associated pictures of negative and neutral scenes. We
             used a finite impulse response model to analyze temporal
             differences between groups in neural responses.
             Results:Compared with controls, participants with PTSD
             reported more involuntary memories, which were more
             emotional and more vivid, but which activated a similar
             network of regions. However, compared to controls,
             individuals with PTSD showed delayed neural responsivity in
             this network and increased vmPFC/ACC activity for
             negative > neutral stimuli. Conclusions:The similarity
             between PTSD and controls in neural substrates underlying
             involuntary memories suggests that, unlike voluntary
             memories, involuntary memories elicit similar activity in
             regions critical for memory retrieval. Further, the delayed
             neural responsivity for involuntary memories in PTSD
             suggests that factors affecting cognition in PTSD, like
             increased fatigue, or avoidance behaviors could do so by
             delaying activity in regions necessary for cognitive
             processing. Finally, compared to neutral memories, negative
             involuntary memories elicit hyperactivity in the vmPFC,
             whereas the vmPFC is typically shown to be hypoactive in
             PTSD during voluntary memory retrieval. These patterns
             suggest that considering both the temporal dynamics of
             cognitive processes as well as involuntary cognitive
             processes would improve existing neurobiological models of
             PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.nicl.2018.05.009},
   Key = {fds335706}
}

@article{fds332086,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {What psychology and cognitive neuroscience know about the
             communicative function of memory.},
   Journal = {The Behavioral and brain sciences},
   Volume = {41},
   Pages = {e30},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1700156x},
   Abstract = {Mahr & Csibra (M&C) include interesting ideas about the
             nature of memory from outside of the field of cognitive
             psychology and cognitive neuroscience. However, the target
             article's inaccurate claims about those fields limit its
             usefulness. I briefly review the most serious omissions and
             distortions of the literature by the target article,
             including its misrepresentation of event memory, and offer
             suggestions for forwarding the goal of understanding the
             communicative function of memory.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x1700156x},
   Key = {fds332086}
}

@article{fds328627,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Li, D and Hall, SA and Kragel, PA and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {Taking tests in the magnet: Brain mapping standardized
             tests.},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {5706-5725},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23761},
   Abstract = {Standardized psychometric tests are sophisticated,
             well-developed, and consequential instruments; test outcomes
             are taken as facts about people that impact their lives in
             important ways. As part of an initial demonstration that
             human brain mapping techniques can add converging
             neural-level evidence to understanding standardized tests,
             our participants completed items from standardized tests
             during an fMRI scan. We compared tests for diagnosing
             posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and the correlated
             measures of Neuroticism, Attachment, and Centrality of Event
             to a general-knowledge baseline test. Twenty-three
             trauma-exposed participants answered 20 items for each of
             our five tests in each of the three runs for a total of 60
             items per test. The tests engaged different neural
             processes; which test a participant was taking was
             accurately predicted from other participants' brain
             activity. The novelty of the application precluded specific
             anatomical predictions; however, the interpretation of
             activated regions using meta-analyses produced encouraging
             results. For instance, items on the Attachment test engaged
             regions shown to be more active for tasks involving
             judgments of others than judgments of the self. The results
             are an initial demonstration of a theoretically and
             practically important test-taking neuroimaging paradigm and
             suggest specific neural processes in answering PTSD-related
             tests. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5706-5725, 2017. © 2017 Wiley
             Periodicals, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1002/hbm.23761},
   Key = {fds328627}
}

@article{fds318745,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Siegler, IC and Beckham, JC and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Neuroticism Increases PTSD Symptom Severity by Amplifying
             the Emotionality, Rehearsal, and Centrality of Trauma
             Memories.},
   Journal = {J Pers},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {702-715},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12278},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: Although it is well established that neuroticism
             increases the risk of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD),
             little is known about the mechanisms that promote PTSD in
             individuals with elevated levels of neuroticism. Across two
             studies, we examined the cognitive-affective processes
             through which neuroticism leads to greater PTSD symptom
             severity. METHOD: Community-dwelling adults with trauma
             histories varying widely in severity (Study 1) and
             clinically diagnosed individuals exposed to DSM-IV-TR A1
             criterion traumas (Study 2) completed measures of
             neuroticism, negative affectivity, trauma memory
             characteristics, and PTSD symptom severity. RESULTS:
             Longitudinal data in Study 1 showed that individuals with
             higher scores on two measures of neuroticism assessed
             approximately three decades apart in young adulthood and
             midlife reported trauma memories accompanied by more intense
             physiological reactions, more frequent involuntary
             rehearsal, and greater perceived centrality to identity in
             older adulthood. These properties of trauma memories were in
             turn associated with more severe PTSD symptoms. Study 2
             replicated these findings using cross-sectional data from
             individuals with severe trauma histories and three
             additional measures of neuroticism. CONCLUSIONS: Results
             suggest that neuroticism leads to PTSD symptoms by
             magnifying the emotionality, availability, and centrality of
             trauma memories as proposed in mnemonic models of
             PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1111/jopy.12278},
   Key = {fds318745}
}

@article{fds330889,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Commentary-Pre- and Posttrauma Predictors of Posttraumatic
             Stress Disorder Symptom Severity: Reply to van der Velden
             and van der Knaap (2017).},
   Journal = {Clin Psychol Sci},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {146-149},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702616661057},
   Doi = {10.1177/2167702616661057},
   Key = {fds330889}
}

@misc{fds347787,
   Author = {Rubin, D and Talarico, JM},
   Title = {Ordinary memory processes shape flashbulb memories of
             extraordinary events: A review of 40 years of
             research.},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press.},
   Year = {2017},
   Key = {fds347787}
}

@article{fds318744,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D and Ogle, CM and Deffler, SA and Beckham,
             JC},
   Title = {Scientific evidence versus outdated beliefs: A response to
             Brewin (2016).},
   Journal = {J Abnorm Psychol},
   Volume = {125},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1018-1021},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000211},
   Abstract = {We find Brewin's (2016) critiques of the narratives, power,
             and coherence measures in Rubin et al. (2016) without merit;
             his suggestions for a "revised formulation" (p. 1015) of
             coherence are contradicted by data readily available in the
             target article but ignored. We place Brewin's commentary in
             a historical context and show that it reiterates views of
             trauma memory fragmentation that are unsupported by data. We
             evaluate an earlier review of fragmentation of trauma
             memories (Brewin, 2014), which Brewin uses to support his
             position in the commentary. We show that it is contradicted
             by more comprehensive reviews and fails to include several
             studies that met Brewin's inclusion criteria but provided no
             support for his position, including 3 studies by the present
             authors (Rubin, 2011; Rubin, Boals, & Berntsen, 2008; Rubin,
             Dennis, & Beckham, 2011). In short, the commentary's
             position does not stand against scientific evidence;
             attempts to rescue it through arguments unsupported by data
             advance neither science nor clinical practice. (PsycINFO
             Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/abn0000211},
   Key = {fds318744}
}

@article{fds314827,
   Author = {Deffler, SA and Fox, C and Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {All my children: The roles of semantic category and phonetic
             similarity in the misnaming of familiar individuals.},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {989-999},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/11918 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Despite knowing a familiar individual (such as a daughter)
             well, anecdotal evidence suggests that naming errors can
             occur among very familiar individuals. Here, we investigate
             the conditions surrounding these types of errors, or
             misnamings, in which a person (the misnamer) incorrectly
             calls a familiar individual (the misnamed) by someone else's
             name (the named). Across 5 studies including over 1,700
             participants, we investigated the prevalence of the
             phenomenon of misnaming, identified factors underlying why
             it may occur, and tested potential mechanisms. We included
             undergraduates and MTurk workers and asked questions of both
             the misnamed and the misnamer. We find that familiar
             individuals are often misnamed with the name of another
             member of the same semantic category; family members are
             misnamed with another family member's name and friends are
             misnamed with another friend's name. Phonetic similarity
             between names also leads to misnamings; however, the size of
             this effect was smaller than that of the semantic category
             effect. Overall, the misnaming of familiar individuals is
             driven by the relationship between the misnamer, misnamed,
             and named; phonetic similarity between the incorrect name
             used by the misnamer and the correct name also plays a role
             in misnaming.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-016-0613-z},
   Key = {fds314827}
}

@article{fds303802,
   Author = {Ford, JH and Rubin, DC and Giovanello, KS},
   Title = {The effects of song familiarity and age on phenomenological
             characteristics and neural recruitment during
             autobiographical memory retrieval.},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {199-210},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000152},
   Abstract = {Recent research suggests that emotional music clips can
             serve as a highly successful tool for eliciting rich
             autobiographical memories, and that the utility of these
             cues may be related to their subjective familiarity. The
             current study was designed to examine the effects of
             familiarity on phenomenological characteristics and neural
             recruitment during retrieval of autobiographical memories
             elicited by musical cues. Further, we were interested in
             understanding how these effects differ as a function of age.
             In an event-related functional neuroimaging study,
             participants retrieved autobiographical memories associated
             with age-specific popular musical clips. Participants rated
             song familiarity, as well as the temporal specificity and
             emotional valence of each memory. Song familiarity was
             associated with increased dmPFC activity and ratings of
             temporal specificity and positivity across participants. In
             addition, behavioral and neuroimaging findings suggest age
             differences in familiarity-related effects in which
             familiarity was more associated with enhancement of memory
             detail in young adults and affective positivity in older
             adults. These findings highlight important age-related
             shifts in how individuals retrieve autobiographical events
             and how personally-relevant musical cues may be used to
             facilitate memory retrieval.},
   Doi = {10.1037/pmu0000152},
   Key = {fds303802}
}

@article{fds303804,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Maladaptive trauma appraisals mediate the relation between
             attachment anxiety and PTSD symptom severity.},
   Journal = {Psychol Trauma},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {301-309},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {1942-9681},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12029 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: In a large sample of community-dwelling older
             adults with histories of exposure to a broad range of
             traumatic events, we examined the extent to which appraisals
             of traumatic events mediate the relations between insecure
             attachment styles and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
             symptom severity. METHOD: Participants completed an
             assessment of adult attachment, in addition to measures of
             PTSD symptom severity, event centrality, event severity, and
             ratings of the A1 PTSD diagnostic criterion for the
             potentially traumatic life event that bothered them most at
             the time of the study. RESULTS: Consistent with theoretical
             proposals and empirical studies indicating that individual
             differences in adult attachment systematically influence how
             individuals evaluate distressing events, individuals with
             higher attachment anxiety perceived their traumatic life
             events to be more central to their identity and more severe.
             Greater event centrality and event severity were each in
             turn related to higher PTSD symptom severity. In contrast,
             the relation between attachment avoidance and PTSD symptoms
             was not mediated by appraisals of event centrality or event
             severity. Furthermore, neither attachment anxiety nor
             attachment avoidance was related to participants' ratings of
             the A1 PTSD diagnostic criterion. CONCLUSION: Our findings
             suggest that attachment anxiety contributes to greater PTSD
             symptom severity through heightened perceptions of traumatic
             events as central to identity and severe. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/tra0000112},
   Key = {fds303804}
}

@article{fds314826,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Rice, HJ and Wooldridge, CL and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Visual imagery in autobiographical memory: The role of
             repeated retrieval in shifting perspective.},
   Volume = {42},
   Pages = {237-253},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {1053-8100},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12021 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Recent memories are generally recalled from a first-person
             perspective whereas older memories are often recalled from a
             third-person perspective. We investigated how repeated
             retrieval affects the availability of visual information,
             and whether it could explain the observed shift in
             perspective with time. In Experiment 1, participants
             performed mini-events and nominated memories of recent
             autobiographical events in response to cue words. Next, they
             described their memory for each event and rated its
             phenomenological characteristics. Over the following three
             weeks, they repeatedly retrieved half of the mini-event and
             cue-word memories. No instructions were given about how to
             retrieve the memories. In Experiment 2, participants were
             asked to adopt either a first- or third-person perspective
             during retrieval. One month later, participants retrieved
             all of the memories and again provided phenomenology
             ratings. When first-person visual details from the event
             were repeatedly retrieved, this information was retained
             better and the shift in perspective was slowed.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.018},
   Key = {fds314826}
}

@article{fds303801,
   Author = {Koppel, J and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Recent Advances in Understanding the Reminiscence Bump: The
             Importance of Cues in Guiding Recall from Autobiographical
             Memory.},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {135-149},
   Publisher = {Association for Psychological Science},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0963-7214},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12028 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The reminiscence bump is the increased proportion of
             autobiographical memories from youth and early adulthood
             observed in adults over 40. It is one of the most robust
             findings in autobiographical memory research. Although
             described as a single period of increased memories, a recent
             meta-analysis which reported the beginning and ending ages
             of the bump from individual studies found that different
             classes of cues produce distinct bumps that vary in size and
             temporal location. The bump obtained in response to cue
             words is both smaller and located earlier in the lifespan
             than the bump obtained when important memories are
             requested. The bump obtained in response to odor cues is
             even earlier. This variation in the size and location of the
             reminiscence bump argues for theories based primarily on
             retrieval rather than encoding and retention, which most
             current theories stress. Furthermore, it points to the need
             to develop theories of autobiographical memory that account
             for this flexibility in the memories retrieved.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0963721416631955},
   Key = {fds303801}
}

@article{fds312315,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Accounting for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Symptom
             Severity With Pre- and Posttrauma Measures: A Longitudinal
             Study of Older Adults.},
   Journal = {Clin Psychol Sci},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {272-286},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {2167-7026},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12025 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Using data from a longitudinal study of community-dwelling
             older adults, we analyzed the most extensive set of known
             correlates of PTSD symptoms obtained from a single sample to
             examine the measures' independent and combined utility in
             accounting for PTSD symptom severity. Fifteen measures
             identified as PTSD risk factors in published meta-analyses
             and 12 theoretically and empirically supported individual
             difference and health-related measures were included.
             Individual difference measures assessed after the trauma,
             including insecure attachment and factors related to the
             current trauma memory, such as self-rated severity, event
             centrality, frequency of involuntary recall, and physical
             reactions to the memory, accounted for symptom severity
             better than measures of pre-trauma factors. In an analysis
             restricted to prospective measures assessed before the
             trauma, the total variance explained decreased from 56% to
             16%. Results support a model of PTSD in which
             characteristics of the current trauma memory promote the
             development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms.},
   Doi = {10.1177/2167702615583227},
   Key = {fds312315}
}

@article{fds303800,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D and Hoyle, RH and Boals, A and Collie, CF and Clancy, CP and Hertzberg, MA},
   Title = {The stress response syndrome: The 17 PTSD symptoms as a
             single scale},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   Key = {fds303800}
}

@article{fds302198,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Deffler, SA and Ogle, CM and Dowell, NM and Graesser, AC and Beckham, JC},
   Title = {Participant, rater, and computer measures of coherence in
             posttraumatic stress disorder.},
   Journal = {J Abnorm Psychol},
   Volume = {125},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {11-25},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0021-843X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12022 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {We examined the coherence of trauma memories in a
             trauma-exposed community sample of 30 adults with and 30
             without posttraumatic stress disorder. The groups had
             similar categories of traumas and were matched on multiple
             factors that could affect the coherence of memories. We
             compared the transcribed oral trauma memories of
             participants with their most important and most positive
             memories. A comprehensive set of 28 measures of coherence
             including 3 ratings by the participants, 7 ratings by
             outside raters, and 18 computer-scored measures, provided a
             variety of approaches to defining and measuring coherence. A
             multivariate analysis of variance indicated differences in
             coherence among the trauma, important, and positive
             memories, but not between the diagnostic groups or their
             interaction with these memory types. Most differences were
             small in magnitude; in some cases, the trauma memories were
             more, rather than less, coherent than the control memories.
             Where differences existed, the results agreed with the
             existing literature, suggesting that factors other than the
             incoherence of trauma memories are most likely to be central
             to the maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder and thus
             its treatment.},
   Doi = {10.1037/abn0000126},
   Key = {fds302198}
}

@article{fds253653,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC and Salgado, S},
   Title = {The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and
             future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional
             distress, and age.},
   Volume = {36},
   Pages = {352-372},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {1053-8100},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12024 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {We introduce a new scale, the Involuntary Autobiographical
             Memory Inventory (IAMI), for measuring the frequency of
             involuntary autobiographical memories and involuntary future
             thoughts. Using the scale in relation to other psychometric
             and demographic measures provided three important, novel
             findings. First, the frequency of involuntary and voluntary
             memories and future thoughts are similarly related to
             general measures of emotional distress. This challenges the
             idea that the involuntary mode is uniquely associated with
             emotional distress. Second, the frequency of involuntary
             autobiographical remembering does not decline with age,
             whereas measures of daydreaming, suppression of unwanted
             thoughts and dissociative experiences all do. Thus,
             involuntary autobiographical remembering relates differently
             to aging than daydreaming and other forms of spontaneous and
             uncontrollable thoughts. Third, unlike involuntary
             autobiographical remembering, the frequency of future
             thoughts does decrease with age. This finding underscores
             the need for examining past and future mental time travel in
             relation to aging and life span development.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.007},
   Key = {fds253653}
}

@article{fds300083,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Pretraumatic Stress Reactions in Soldiers Deployed to
             Afghanistan.},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {663-674},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {2167-7026},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12023 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Posttraumatic Stress Disorder is a diagnosis related to the
             past. Pre-traumatic stress reactions, as measured by
             intrusive involuntary images of possible future stressful
             events and their associated avoidance and increased arousal,
             have been overlooked in the PTSD literature. Here we
             introduce a scale that measures pre-traumatic stress
             reactions providing a clear future-oriented parallel to the
             posttraumatic stress reactions described in the diagnostic
             criteria for PTSD. We apply this pre-traumatic stress
             reactions checklist (PreCL) to Danish soldiers before,
             during, and after deployment to Afghanistan. The PreCL has
             good internal consistency and is highly correlated with a
             standard measure of PTSD symptoms. The PreCL as answered
             before the soldiers' deployment significantly predicted
             level of PTSD symptoms during and after their deployment,
             while controlling for baseline PTSD symptoms and combat
             exposure measured during and after deployment. The findings
             have implications for the conceptualization of PTSD,
             screening, and treatment.},
   Doi = {10.1177/2167702614551766},
   Key = {fds300083}
}

@article{fds253654,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {The relation between insecure attachment and posttraumatic
             stress: Early life versus adulthood traumas.},
   Journal = {Psychol Trauma},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {324-332},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {1942-9681},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000015},
   Abstract = {The present study examined the relations between insecure
             attachment and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms
             among community-dwelling older adults with exposure to a
             broad range of traumatic events. Attachment anxiety and
             attachment avoidance predicted more severe symptoms of PTSD
             and explained unique variance in symptom severity when
             compared to other individual difference measures associated
             with an elevated risk of PTSD, including NEO neuroticism and
             event centrality. A significant interaction between the
             developmental timing of the trauma and attachment anxiety
             revealed that the relation between PTSD symptoms and
             attachment anxiety was stronger for individuals with current
             PTSD symptoms associated with early life traumas compared to
             individuals with PTSD symptoms linked to adulthood traumas.
             Analyses examining factors that account for the relation
             between insecure attachment and PTSD symptoms indicated that
             individuals with greater attachment anxiety reported
             stronger physical reactions to memories of their trauma and
             more frequent voluntary and involuntary rehearsal of their
             trauma memories. These phenomenological properties of trauma
             memories were in turn associated with greater PTSD symptom
             severity. Among older adults with early life traumas, only
             the frequency of involuntary recall partially accounted for
             the relation between attachment anxiety and PTSD symptoms.
             Our differential findings concerning early life versus
             adulthood trauma suggest that factors underlying the
             relation between attachment anxiety and PTSD symptoms vary
             according to the developmental timing of the traumatic
             exposure. Overall our results are consistent with attachment
             theory and with theoretical models of PTSD according to
             which PTSD symptoms are promoted by phenomenological
             properties of trauma memories.},
   Doi = {10.1037/tra0000015},
   Key = {fds253654}
}

@article{fds253657,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {One bump, two bumps, three bumps, four? Using retrieval cues
             to divide one autobiographical memory reminiscence bump into
             many},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {87-89},
   Publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {2211-3681},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10069 Duke open
             access},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.12.005},
   Key = {fds253657}
}

@article{fds253659,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Umanath, S},
   Title = {Event memory: A theory of memory for laboratory,
             autobiographical, and fictional events.},
   Volume = {122},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-23},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0033-295X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10068 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {An event memory is a mental construction of a scene recalled
             as a single occurrence. It therefore requires the
             hippocampus and ventral visual stream needed for all scene
             construction. The construction need not come with a sense of
             reliving or be made by a participant in the event, and it
             can be a summary of occurrences from more than one encoding.
             The mental construction, or physical rendering, of any scene
             must be done from a specific location and time; this
             introduces a "self" located in space and time, which is a
             necessary, but need not be a sufficient, condition for a
             sense of reliving. We base our theory on scene construction
             rather than reliving because this allows the integration of
             many literatures and because there is more accumulated
             knowledge about scene construction's phenomenology,
             behavior, and neural basis. Event memory differs from
             episodic memory in that it does not conflate the independent
             dimensions of whether or not a memory is relived, is about
             the self, is recalled voluntarily, or is based on a single
             encoding with whether it is recalled as a single occurrence
             of a scene. Thus, we argue that event memory provides a
             clearer contrast to semantic memory, which also can be about
             the self, be recalled voluntarily, and be from a unique
             encoding; allows for a more comprehensive dimensional
             account of the structure of explicit memory; and better
             accounts for laboratory and real-world behavioral and neural
             results, including those from neuropsychology and
             neuroimaging, than does episodic memory.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0037907},
   Key = {fds253659}
}

@misc{fds302142,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A basic systems account of trauma memories in PTSD: is more
             needed?},
   Pages = {41-64},
   Booktitle = {Clinical perspective on autobiographical memory: Theories
             and approaches},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Watson, LA and Berntsen, D},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   Key = {fds302142}
}

@article{fds253663,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Psychology. How quickly we forget.},
   Volume = {346},
   Number = {6213},
   Pages = {1058-1059},
   Publisher = {American Association for the Advancement of Science
             (AAAS)},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0036-8075},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12026 Duke open
             access},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.aaa2341},
   Key = {fds253663}
}

@article{fds253669,
   Author = {Hall, SA and Rubin, DC and Miles, A and Davis, SW and Wing, EA and Cabeza,
             R and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {The neural basis of involuntary episodic
             memories.},
   Journal = {J Cogn Neurosci},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {2385-2399},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12027 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Voluntary episodic memories require an intentional memory
             search, whereas involuntary episodic memories come to mind
             spontaneously without conscious effort. Cognitive
             neuroscience has largely focused on voluntary memory,
             leaving the neural mechanisms of involuntary memory largely
             unknown. We hypothesized that, because the main difference
             between voluntary and involuntary memory is the controlled
             retrieval processes required by the former, there would be
             greater frontal activity for voluntary than involuntary
             memories. Conversely, we predicted that other components of
             the episodic retrieval network would be similarly engaged in
             the two types of memory. During encoding, all participants
             heard sounds, half paired with pictures of complex scenes
             and half presented alone. During retrieval, paired and
             unpaired sounds were presented, panned to the left or to the
             right. Participants in the involuntary group were instructed
             to indicate the spatial location of the sound, whereas
             participants in the voluntary group were asked to
             additionally recall the pictures that had been paired with
             the sounds. All participants reported the incidence of their
             memories in a postscan session. Consistent with our
             predictions, voluntary memories elicited greater activity in
             dorsal frontal regions than involuntary memories, whereas
             other components of the retrieval network, including
             medial-temporal, ventral occipitotemporal, and ventral
             parietal regions were similarly engaged by both types of
             memories. These results clarify the distinct role of dorsal
             frontal and ventral occipitotemporal regions in predicting
             strategic retrieval and recalled information, respectively,
             and suggest that, although there are neural differences in
             retrieval, involuntary memories share neural components with
             established voluntary memory systems.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_00633},
   Key = {fds253669}
}

@article{fds253671,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Boals, A and Hoyle, RH},
   Title = {Narrative centrality and negative affectivity: independent
             and interactive contributors to stress reactions.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. General},
   Volume = {143},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {1159-1170},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0096-3445},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035140},
   Abstract = {Reactions to stressful negative events have long been
             studied using approaches based on either the narrative
             interpretation of the event or the traits of the individual.
             Here, we integrate these 2 approaches by using
             individual-differences measures of both the narrative
             interpretation of the stressful event as central to one's
             life and the personality characteristic of negative
             affectivity. We show that they each have independent
             contributions to stress reactions and that high levels on
             both produce greater than additive effects. The effects on
             posttraumatic stress symptoms are substantial for both
             undergraduates (Study 1, n = 2,296; Study 3, n = 488) and
             veterans (Study 2, n = 104), with mean levels for
             participants low on both measures near floor on
             posttraumatic stress symptoms and those high on both
             measures scoring at or above diagnostic thresholds. Study 3
             included 3 measures of narrative centrality and 3 of
             negative affectivity to demonstrate that the effects were
             not limited to a single measure. In Study 4 (n = 987),
             measures associated with symptoms of posttraumatic stress
             correlated substantially with either measures of narrative
             centrality or measures of negative affectivity. The concepts
             of narrative centrality and negative affectivity and the
             results are consistent with findings from clinical
             populations using similar measures and with current
             approaches to therapy. In broad nonclinical populations,
             such as those used here, the results suggest that we might
             be able to substantially increase our ability to account for
             the severity of stress response by including both
             concepts.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0035140},
   Key = {fds253671}
}

@article{fds253667,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Schema-driven construction of future autobiographical
             traumatic events: the future is much more troubling than the
             past.},
   Volume = {143},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {612-630},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0096-3445},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9754 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Research on future episodic thought has produced compelling
             theories and results in cognitive psychology, cognitive
             neuroscience, and clinical psychology. In experiments aimed
             to integrate these with basic concepts and methods from
             autobiographical memory research, 76 undergraduates
             remembered past and imagined future positive and negative
             events that had or would have a major impact on them.
             Correlations of the online ratings of visual and auditory
             imagery, emotion, and other measures demonstrated that
             individuals used the same processes to the same extent to
             remember past and construct future events. These measures
             predicted the theoretically important metacognitive judgment
             of past reliving and future "preliving" in similar ways. On
             standardized tests of reactions to traumatic events, scores
             for future negative events were much higher than scores for
             past negative events. The scores for future negative events
             were in the range that would qualify for a diagnosis of
             posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); the test was
             replicated (n = 52) to check for order effects. Consistent
             with earlier work, future events had less sensory vividness.
             Thus, the imagined symptoms of future events were unlikely
             to be caused by sensory vividness. In a second experiment,
             to confirm this, 63 undergraduates produced numerous added
             details between 2 constructions of the same negative future
             events; deficits in rated vividness were removed with no
             increase in the standardized tests of reactions to traumatic
             events. Neuroticism predicted individuals' reactions to
             negative past events but did not predict imagined reactions
             to future events. This set of novel methods and findings is
             interpreted in the contexts of the literatures of episodic
             future thought, autobiographical memory, PTSD, and classic
             schema theory.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0032638},
   Key = {fds253667}
}

@article{fds253677,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Changes in neuroticism following trauma exposure.},
   Journal = {J Pers},
   Volume = {82},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {93-102},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23550961},
   Abstract = {Using longitudinal data, the present study examined change
             in midlife neuroticism following trauma exposure. Our
             primary analyses included 670 participants (M(age) = 60.55;
             65.22% male, 99.70% Caucasian) who completed the NEO
             Personality Inventory at ages 42 and 50 and reported their
             lifetime exposure to traumatic events approximately 10 years
             later. No differences in pre- and post-trauma neuroticism
             scores were found among individuals who experienced all of
             their lifetime traumas in the interval between the
             personality assessments. Results were instead consistent
             with normative age-related declines in neuroticism
             throughout adulthood. Furthermore, longitudinal changes in
             neuroticism scores did not differ between individuals with
             and without histories of midlife trauma exposure.
             Examination of change in neuroticism following
             life-threatening traumas yielded a comparable pattern of
             results. Analysis of facet-level scores largely replicated
             findings from the domain scores. Overall, our findings
             suggest that neuroticism does not reliably change following
             exposure to traumatic events in middle adulthood.
             Supplemental analyses indicated that individuals exposed to
             life-threatening traumas in childhood or adolescence
             reported higher midlife neuroticism than individuals who
             experienced severe traumas in adulthood. Life-threatening
             traumatic events encountered early in life may have a more
             pronounced impact on adulthood personality than recent
             traumatic events.},
   Doi = {10.1111/jopy.12037},
   Key = {fds253677}
}

@article{fds253665,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Involuntary Memories and Dissociative Amnesia: Assessing Key
             Assumptions in PTSD Research.},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {174-186},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {2167-7026},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9758 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memories of trauma victims are often
             described as disturbed in two ways. First, the trauma is
             frequently re-experienced in the form of involuntary,
             intrusive recollections. Second, the trauma is difficult to
             recall voluntarily (strategically); important parts may be
             totally or partially inaccessible-a feature known as
             dissociative amnesia. These characteristics are often
             mentioned by PTSD researchers and are included as PTSD
             symptoms in the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association,
             2000). In contrast, we show that both involuntary and
             voluntary recall are enhanced by emotional stress during
             encoding. We also show that the PTSD symptom in the
             diagnosis addressing dissociative amnesia, trouble
             remembering important aspects of the trauma is less well
             correlated with the remaining PTSD symptoms than the
             conceptual reversal of having trouble forgetting important
             aspects of the trauma. Our findings contradict key
             assumptions that have shaped PTSD research over the last 40
             years.},
   Doi = {10.1177/2167702613496241},
   Key = {fds253665}
}

@article{fds253675,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Cumulative exposure to traumatic events in older
             adults.},
   Journal = {Aging Ment Health},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {316-325},
   Year = {2014},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24011223},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVES: The present study examined the impact of
             cumulative trauma exposure on current posttraumatic stress
             disorder (PTSD) symptom severity in a nonclinical sample of
             adults in their 60s. The predictive utility of cumulative
             trauma exposure was compared to other known predictors of
             PTSD, including trauma severity, personality traits, social
             support, and event centrality. METHOD: Community-dwelling
             adults (n = 2515) from the crest of the Baby Boom generation
             completed the Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire, the PTSD
             Checklist, the NEO Personality Inventory, the Centrality of
             Event Scale, and rated their current social support.
             RESULTS: Cumulative trauma exposure predicted greater PTSD
             symptom severity in hierarchical regression analyses
             consistent with a dose-response model. Neuroticism and event
             centrality also emerged as robust predictors of PTSD symptom
             severity. In contrast, the severity of individuals' single
             most distressing life event, as measured by self-report
             ratings of the A1 PTSD diagnostic criterion, did not add
             explanatory variance to the model. Analyses concerning event
             categories revealed that cumulative exposure to childhood
             violence and adulthood physical assaults were most strongly
             associated with PTSD symptom severity in older adulthood.
             Moreover, cumulative self-oriented events accounted for a
             larger percentage of variance in symptom severity compared
             to events directed at others. CONCLUSION: Our findings
             suggest that the cumulative impact of exposure to traumatic
             events throughout the life course contributes significantly
             to posttraumatic stress in older adulthood above and beyond
             other known predictors of PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1080/13607863.2013.832730},
   Key = {fds253675}
}

@article{fds253676,
   Author = {Ford, JH and Rubin, DC and Giovanello, KS},
   Title = {Effects of task instruction on autobiographical memory
             specificity in young and older adults.},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {722-736},
   Year = {2014},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9763 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Older adults tend to retrieve autobiographical information
             that is overly general (i.e., not restricted to a single
             event, termed the overgenerality effect) relative to young
             adults' specific memories. A vast majority of studies that
             have reported overgenerality effects explicitly instruct
             participants to retrieve specific memories, thereby
             requiring participants to maintain task goals, inhibit
             inappropriate responses, and control their memory search.
             Since these processes are impaired in healthy ageing, it is
             important to determine whether such task instructions
             influence the magnitude of the overgenerality effect in
             older adults. In the current study participants retrieved
             autobiographical memories during presentation of musical
             clips. Task instructions were manipulated to separate
             age-related differences in the specificity of underlying
             memory representations from age-related differences in
             following task instructions. Whereas young adults modulated
             memory specificity based on task demands, older adults did
             not. These findings suggest that reported rates of
             overgenerality in older adults' memories might include
             age-related differences in memory representation, as well as
             differences in task compliance. Such findings provide a
             better understanding of the underlying cognitive mechanisms
             involved in age-related changes in autobiographical memory
             and may also be valuable for future research examining
             effects of overgeneral memory on general
             well-being.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2013.820325},
   Key = {fds253676}
}

@article{fds253709,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {The impact of the developmental timing of trauma exposure on
             PTSD symptoms and psychosocial functioning among older
             adults.},
   Journal = {Dev Psychol},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {2191-2200},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23458662},
   Abstract = {The present study examined the impact of the developmental
             timing of trauma exposure on posttraumatic stress disorder
             (PTSD) symptoms and psychosocial functioning in a large
             sample of community-dwelling older adults (N = 1,995).
             Specifically, we investigated whether the negative
             consequences of exposure to traumatic events were greater
             for traumas experienced during childhood, adolescence, young
             adulthood, midlife, or older adulthood. Each of these
             developmental periods is characterized by age-related
             changes in cognitive and social processes that may influence
             psychological adjustment following trauma exposure. Results
             revealed that older adults who experienced their currently
             most distressing traumatic event during childhood exhibited
             more severe symptoms of PTSD and lower subjective happiness
             compared with older adults who experienced their most
             distressing trauma after the transition to adulthood.
             Similar findings emerged for measures of social support and
             coping ability. The differential effects of childhood
             compared with later life traumas were not fully explained by
             differences in cumulative trauma exposure or by differences
             in the objective and subjective characteristics of the
             events. Our findings demonstrate the enduring nature of
             traumatic events encountered early in the life course and
             underscore the importance of examining the developmental
             context of trauma exposure in investigations of the
             long-term consequences of traumatic experiences.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0031985},
   Key = {fds253709}
}

@article{fds253672,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Feeling, N},
   Title = {Measuring the Severity of Negative and Traumatic
             Events.},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {375-389},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {2167-7026},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9764 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {We devised three measures of the general severity of events,
             which raters applied to participants' narrative
             descriptions: 1) placing events on a standard normed scale
             of stressful events, 2) placing events into five bins based
             on their severity relative to all other events in the
             sample, and 3) an average of ratings of the events' effects
             on six distinct areas of the participants' lives. Protocols
             of negative events were obtained from two non-diagnosed
             undergraduate samples (n = 688 and 328), a clinically
             diagnosed undergraduate sample all of whom had traumas and
             half of whom met PTSD criteria (n = 30), and a clinically
             diagnosed community sample who met PTSD criteria (n = 75).
             The three measures of severity correlated highly in all four
             samples but failed to correlate with PTSD symptom severity
             in any sample. Theoretical implications for the role of
             trauma severity in PTSD are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1177/2167702613483112},
   Key = {fds253672}
}

@article{fds253673,
   Author = {Ogle, CM and Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D and Siegler,
             IC},
   Title = {The Frequency and Impact of Exposure to Potentially
             Traumatic Events Over the Life Course.},
   Journal = {Clin Psychol Sci},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {426-434},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {2167-7026},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9766 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {We examined the frequency and impact of exposure to
             potentially traumatic events among a nonclinical sample of
             older adults (n = 3,575), a population typically
             underrepresented in epidemiological research concerning the
             prevalence of traumatic events. Current PTSD symptom
             severity and the centrality of events to identity were
             assessed for events nominated as currently most distressing.
             Approximately 90% of participants experienced one or more
             potentially traumatic events. Events that occurred with
             greater frequency early in the life course were associated
             with more severe PTSD symptoms compared to events that
             occurred with greater frequency during later decades. Early
             life traumas, however, were not more central to identity.
             Results underscore the differential impact of traumatic
             events experienced throughout the life course. We conclude
             with suggestions for further research concerning mechanisms
             that promote the persistence of post-traumatic stress
             related to early life traumas and empirical evaluation of
             psychotherapeutic treatments for older adults with
             PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1177/2167702613485076},
   Key = {fds253673}
}

@article{fds253674,
   Author = {St, JPL and Kragel, PA and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Neural networks supporting autobiographical memory retrieval
             in posttraumatic stress disorder.},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {554-566},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {1530-7026},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000324557900010&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) affects the functional
             recruitment and connectivity between neural regions during
             autobiographical memory (AM) retrieval that overlap with
             default and control networks. Whether such univariate
             changes relate to potential differences in the contributions
             of the large-scale neural networks supporting cognition in
             PTSD is unknown. In the present functional MRI study, we
             employed independent-component analysis to examine the
             influence of the engagement of neural networks during the
             recall of personal memories in a PTSD group (15
             participants) as compared to non-trauma-exposed healthy
             controls (14 participants). We found that the PTSD group
             recruited similar neural networks when compared to the
             controls during AM recall, including default-network
             subsystems and control networks, but group differences
             emerged in the spatial and temporal characteristics of these
             networks. First, we found spatial differences in the
             contributions of the anterior and posterior midline across
             the networks, and of the amygdala in particular, for the
             medial temporal subsystem of the default network. Second, we
             found temporal differences within the medial prefrontal
             subsystem of the default network, with less temporal
             coupling of this network during AM retrieval in PTSD
             relative to controls. These findings suggest that the
             spatial and temporal characteristics of the default and
             control networks potentially differ in a PTSD group versus
             healthy controls and contribute to altered recall of
             personal memory.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13415-013-0157-7},
   Key = {fds253674}
}

@article{fds253708,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Johannessen, KB and Thomsen, YD and Bertelsen, M and Hoyle, RH and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Peace and war: trajectories of posttraumatic stress disorder
             symptoms before, during, and after military deployment in
             Afghanistan.},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {1557-1565},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {0956-7976},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9771 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {In the study reported here, we examined posttraumatic stress
             disorder (PTSD) symptoms in 746 Danish soldiers measured on
             five occasions before, during, and after deployment to
             Afghanistan. Using latent class growth analysis, we
             identified six trajectories of change in PTSD symptoms. Two
             resilient trajectories had low levels across all five times,
             and a new-onset trajectory started low and showed a marked
             increase of PTSD symptoms. Three temporary-benefit
             trajectories, not previously described in the literature,
             showed decreases in PTSD symptoms during (or immediately
             after) deployment, followed by increases after return from
             deployment. Predeployment emotional problems and
             predeployment traumas, especially childhood adversities,
             were predictors for inclusion in the nonresilient
             trajectories, whereas deployment-related stress was not.
             These findings challenge standard views of PTSD in two ways.
             First, they show that factors other than immediately
             preceding stressors are critical for PTSD development, with
             childhood adversities being central. Second, they
             demonstrate that the development of PTSD symptoms shows
             heterogeneity, which indicates the need for multiple
             measurements to understand PTSD and identify people in need
             of treatment.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797612457389},
   Key = {fds253708}
}

@article{fds253710,
   Author = {St, JPL and Rubin, DC and Cabeza, R},
   Title = {Age-related effects on the neural correlates of
             autobiographical memory retrieval.},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1298-1310},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21190759},
   Abstract = {Older adults recall less episodically rich autobiographical
             memories (AM), however, the neural basis of this effect is
             not clear. Using functional MRI, we examined the effects of
             age during search and elaboration phases of AM retrieval.
             Our results suggest that the age-related attenuation in the
             episodic richness of AMs is associated with difficulty in
             the strategic retrieval processes underlying recovery of
             information during elaboration. First, age effects on AM
             activity were more pronounced during elaboration than
             search, with older adults showing less sustained recruitment
             of the hippocampus and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex
             (VLPFC) for less episodically rich AMs. Second, there was an
             age-related reduction in the modulation of top-down coupling
             of the VLPFC on the hippocampus for episodically rich AMs.
             In sum, the present study shows that changes in the
             sustained response and coupling of the hippocampus and
             prefrontal cortex (PFC) underlie age-related reductions in
             episodic richness of the personal past.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.11.007},
   Key = {fds253710}
}

@article{fds253707,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Hoyle, RH and Leary, MR},
   Title = {Differential predictability of four dimensions of affect
             intensity.},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {25-41},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21707262},
   Abstract = {Individual differences in affect intensity are typically
             assessed with the Affect Intensity Measure (AIM). Previous
             factor analyses suggest that the AIM is comprised of four
             weakly correlated factors: Positive Affectivity, Negative
             Reactivity, Negative Intensity and Positive Intensity or
             Serenity. However, little data exist to show whether its
             four factors relate to other measures differently enough to
             preclude use of the total scale score. The present study
             replicated the four-factor solution and found that subscales
             derived from the four factors correlated differently with
             criterion variables that assess personality domains,
             affective dispositions, and cognitive patterns that are
             associated with emotional reactions. The results show that
             use of the total AIM score can obscure relationships between
             specific features of affect intensity and other variables
             and suggest that researchers should examine the individual
             AIM subscales.},
   Doi = {10.1080/02699931.2011.561564},
   Key = {fds253707}
}

@article{fds253711,
   Author = {Janssen, SMJ and Rubin, DC and Conway, MA},
   Title = {The reminiscence bump in the temporal distribution of the
             best football players of all time: Pelé, Cruijff or
             Maradona?},
   Volume = {65},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {165-178},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21939366},
   Abstract = {The reminiscence bump is the tendency to recall more
             autobiographical memories from adolescence and early
             adulthood than from adjacent lifetime periods. In this
             online study, the robustness of the reminiscence bump was
             examined by looking at participants' judgements about the
             quality of football players. Dutch participants (N = 619)
             were asked who they thought the five best players of all
             time were. The participants could select the names from a
             list or enter the names when their favourite players were
             not on the list. Johan Cruijff, Pelé, and Diego Maradona
             were the three most often mentioned players. Participants
             frequently named football players who reached the midpoint
             of their career when the participants were adolescents (mode
             = 17). The results indicate that the reminiscence bump can
             also be identified outside the autobiographical memory
             domain.},
   Doi = {10.1080/17470218.2011.606372},
   Key = {fds253711}
}

@misc{fds302143,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The basic system model of autobiographical
             memory},
   Pages = {11-32},
   Booktitle = {Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and
             approaches},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Year = {2012},
   Key = {fds302143}
}

@book{fds309884,
   Author = {D. Berntsen and D.C. Rubin},
   Title = {Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and
             approaches},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Bernsten, D and Rubin, DC},
   Year = {2012},
   Key = {fds309884}
}

@article{fds253713,
   Author = {Boals, A and Hathaway, LM and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The Therapeutic Effects of Completing Autobiographical
             Memory Questionnaires for Positive and Negative Events: An
             Experimental Approach},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {544-549},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {0147-5916},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9782 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Rubin et al. (Cognit Ther Res 34:35-48, 2010) demonstrated
             that completing autobiographical memory questionnaires about
             a very negative event leads to decreases in levels of
             distress related to that event. However, a limitation of
             their methodology was that perhaps completing the memory
             questionnaire about any autobiographical memory could
             produce the reported effects. In the current study, 238
             participants nominated a very negative and a very positive
             event from their lives. Participants were then randomly
             assigned to complete autobiographical memory questionnaires
             about either their nominated negative or nominated positive
             event. The results generally replicated the pattern reported
             in Rubin et al. In comparison to participants who completed
             questionnaires about a positive event, participants who
             completed the memory questionnaires about their nominated
             negative event evidenced decreases in emotional reactions
             and distress levels. Implications for completing
             autobiographical memory questionnaires about negative events
             as a possible therapeutic tool are discussed. © 2011
             Springer Science+Business Media, LLC.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10608-011-9412-9},
   Key = {fds253713}
}

@article{fds253725,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Two versions of life: emotionally negative and positive life
             events have different roles in the organization of life
             story and identity.},
   Journal = {Emotion},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1190-1201},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21875191},
   Abstract = {Over 2,000 adults in their sixties completed the Centrality
             of Event Scale (CES) for the traumatic or negative event
             that now troubled them the most and for their most positive
             life event, as well as measures of current PTSD symptoms,
             depression, well-being, and personality. Consistent with the
             notion of a positivity bias in old age, the positive events
             were judged to be markedly more central to life story and
             identity than were the negative events. The centrality of
             positive events was unrelated to measures of PTSD symptoms
             and emotional distress, whereas the centrality of the
             negative event showed clear positive correlations with these
             measures. The centrality of the positive events increased
             with increasing time since the events, whereas the
             centrality of the negative events decreased. The life
             distribution of the positive events showed a marked peak in
             young adulthood whereas the life distribution for the
             negative events peaked at the participants' present age. The
             positive events were mostly events from the cultural life
             script-that is, culturally shared representations of the
             timing of major transitional events. Overall, our findings
             show that positive and negative autobiographical events
             relate markedly differently to life story and identity.
             Positive events become central to life story and identity
             primarily through their correspondence with cultural norms.
             Negative events become central through mechanisms associated
             with emotional distress.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0024940},
   Key = {fds253725}
}

@article{fds253715,
   Author = {Rice, HJ and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Remembering from any angle: the flexibility of visual
             perspective during retrieval.},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {568-577},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {1053-8100},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9778 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {When recalling autobiographical memories, individuals often
             experience visual images associated with the event. These
             images can be constructed from two different perspectives:
             first person, in which the event is visualized from the
             viewpoint experienced at encoding, or third person, in which
             the event is visualized from an external vantage point.
             Using a novel technique to measure visual perspective, we
             examined where the external vantage point is situated in
             third-person images. Individuals in two studies were asked
             to recall either 10 or 15 events from their lives and
             describe the perspectives they experienced. Wide variation
             in spatial locations was observed within third-person
             perspectives, with the location of these perspectives
             relating to the event being recalled. Results suggest
             remembering from an external viewpoint may be more common
             than previous studies have demonstrated.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.013},
   Key = {fds253715}
}

@article{fds253721,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Dennis, MF and Beckham, JC},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory for stressful events: the role of
             autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress
             disorder.},
   Journal = {Conscious Cogn},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {840-856},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21489820},
   Abstract = {To provide the three-way comparisons needed to test existing
             theories, we compared (1) most-stressful memories to other
             memories and (2) involuntary to voluntary memories (3) in 75
             community dwelling adults with and 42 without a current
             diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Each
             rated their three most-stressful, three most-positive, seven
             most-important and 15 word-cued autobiographical memories,
             and completed tests of personality and mood. Involuntary
             memories were then recorded and rated as they occurred for 2
             weeks. Standard mechanisms of cognition and affect applied
             to extreme events accounted for the properties of stressful
             memories. Involuntary memories had greater emotional
             intensity than voluntary memories, but were not more
             frequently related to traumatic events. The emotional
             intensity, rehearsal, and centrality to the life story of
             both voluntary and involuntary memories, rather than
             incoherence of voluntary traumatic memories and enhanced
             availability of involuntary traumatic memories, were the
             properties of autobiographical memories associated with
             PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.015},
   Key = {fds253721}
}

@article{fds253722,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The coherence of memories for trauma: evidence from
             posttraumatic stress disorder.},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {857-865},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20413327},
   Abstract = {Participants with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and
             participants with a trauma but without PTSD wrote narratives
             of their trauma and, for comparison, of the most-important
             and the happiest events that occurred within a year of their
             trauma. They then rated these three events on coherence.
             Based on participants' self-ratings and on naïve-observer
             scorings of the participants' narratives, memories of
             traumas were not more incoherent than the comparison
             memories in participants in general or in participants with
             PTSD. This study comprehensively assesses narrative
             coherence using a full two (PTSD or not) by two (traumatic
             event or not) design. The results are counter to most
             prevalent theoretical views of memory for
             trauma.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.018},
   Key = {fds253722}
}

@article{fds253724,
   Author = {Boals, A and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The integration of emotions in memories: Cognitive-emotional
             distinctiveness and posttraumatic stress
             disorder},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {811-816},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9780 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The current study examined cognitive-emotional
             distinctiveness (CED), the extent to which emotions are
             linked with event information, in memories associated with
             PTSD. Participants either with PTSD (n=68) or without PTSD
             (n=40) completed a modified multidimensional scaling
             technique to measure CED for their most negative and most
             positive events. The results revealed that participants in
             the PTSD group evidenced significantly lower levels of CED.
             This group difference remained significant when we limited
             the analysis to traumatic events that led to a PTSD
             diagnosis (n=33) in comparison to control participants who
             nominated a traumatic event that did not result in PTSD
             (n=32). Replicating previous findings, CED levels were
             higher in memories of negative events, in comparison to
             positive events. These results provide empirical evidence
             that memories associated with PTSD do contain special
             organizational features with respect to the links between
             emotions and memory. Implications for understanding and
             treating PTSD are discussed. © 2010 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1752},
   Key = {fds253724}
}

@article{fds253712,
   Author = {St, JPL and Kragel, PA and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Dynamic neural networks supporting memory
             retrieval.},
   Volume = {57},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {608-616},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21550407},
   Abstract = {How do separate neural networks interact to support complex
             cognitive processes such as remembrance of the personal
             past? Autobiographical memory (AM) retrieval recruits a
             consistent pattern of activation that potentially comprises
             multiple neural networks. However, it is unclear how such
             large-scale neural networks interact and are modulated by
             properties of the memory retrieval process. In the present
             functional MRI (fMRI) study, we combined independent
             component analysis (ICA) and dynamic causal modeling (DCM)
             to understand the neural networks supporting AM retrieval.
             ICA revealed four task-related components consistent with
             the previous literature: 1) medial prefrontal cortex (PFC)
             network, associated with self-referential processes, 2)
             medial temporal lobe (MTL) network, associated with memory,
             3) frontoparietal network, associated with strategic search,
             and 4) cingulooperculum network, associated with goal
             maintenance. DCM analysis revealed that the medial PFC
             network drove activation within the system, consistent with
             the importance of this network to AM retrieval.
             Additionally, memory accessibility and recollection uniquely
             altered connectivity between these neural networks.
             Recollection modulated the influence of the medial PFC on
             the MTL network during elaboration, suggesting that greater
             connectivity among subsystems of the default network
             supports greater re-experience. In contrast, memory
             accessibility modulated the influence of frontoparietal and
             MTL networks on the medial PFC network, suggesting that ease
             of retrieval involves greater fluency among the multiple
             networks contributing to AM. These results show the
             integration between neural networks supporting AM retrieval
             and the modulation of network connectivity by
             behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.039},
   Key = {fds253712}
}

@article{fds253716,
   Author = {Huijbers, W and Pennartz, CMA and Rubin, DC and Daselaar,
             SM},
   Title = {Imagery and retrieval of auditory and visual information:
             neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful
             performance.},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1730-1740},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0028-3932},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9777 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Remembering past events - or episodic retrieval - consists
             of several components. There is evidence that mental imagery
             plays an important role in retrieval and that the brain
             regions supporting imagery overlap with those supporting
             retrieval. An open issue is to what extent these regions
             support successful vs. unsuccessful imagery and retrieval
             processes. Previous studies that examined regional overlap
             between imagery and retrieval used uncontrolled memory
             conditions, such as autobiographical memory tasks, that
             cannot distinguish between successful and unsuccessful
             retrieval. A second issue is that fMRI studies that compared
             imagery and retrieval have used modality-aspecific cues that
             are likely to activate auditory and visual processing
             regions simultaneously. Thus, it is not clear to what extent
             identified brain regions support modality-specific or
             modality-independent imagery and retrieval processes. In the
             current fMRI study, we addressed this issue by comparing
             imagery to retrieval under controlled memory conditions in
             both auditory and visual modalities. We also obtained
             subjective measures of imagery quality allowing us to
             dissociate regions contributing to successful vs.
             unsuccessful imagery. Results indicated that auditory and
             visual regions contribute both to imagery and retrieval in a
             modality-specific fashion. In addition, we identified four
             sets of brain regions with distinct patterns of activity
             that contributed to imagery and retrieval in a
             modality-independent fashion. The first set of regions,
             including hippocampus, posterior cingulate cortex, medial
             prefrontal cortex and angular gyrus, showed a pattern common
             to imagery/retrieval and consistent with successful
             performance regardless of task. The second set of regions,
             including dorsal precuneus, anterior cingulate and
             dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, also showed a pattern common
             to imagery and retrieval, but consistent with unsuccessful
             performance during both tasks. Third, left ventrolateral
             prefrontal cortex showed an interaction between task and
             performance and was associated with successful imagery but
             unsuccessful retrieval. Finally, the fourth set of regions,
             including ventral precuneus, midcingulate cortex and
             supramarginal gyrus, showed the opposite interaction,
             supporting unsuccessful imagery, but successful retrieval
             performance. Results are discussed in relation to
             reconstructive, attentional, semantic memory, and working
             memory processes. This is the first study to separate the
             neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful performance
             for both imagery and retrieval and for both auditory and
             visual modalities.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.051},
   Key = {fds253716}
}

@article{fds253720,
   Author = {St, JPL and Botzung, A and Miles, A and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Functional neuroimaging of emotionally intense
             autobiographical memories in post-traumatic stress
             disorder.},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {630-637},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21109253},
   Abstract = {Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) affects regions that
             support autobiographical memory (AM) retrieval, such as the
             hippocampus, amygdala and ventral medial prefrontal cortex
             (PFC). However, it is not well understood how PTSD may
             impact the neural mechanisms of memory retrieval for the
             personal past. We used a generic cue method combined with
             parametric modulation analysis and functional MRI (fMRI) to
             investigate the neural mechanisms affected by PTSD symptoms
             during the retrieval of a large sample of emotionally
             intense AMs. There were three main results. First, the PTSD
             group showed greater recruitment of the amygdala/hippocampus
             during the construction of negative versus positive
             emotionally intense AMs, when compared to controls. Second,
             across both the construction and elaboration phases of
             retrieval the PTSD group showed greater recruitment of the
             ventral medial PFC for negatively intense memories, but less
             recruitment for positively intense memories. Third, the PTSD
             group showed greater functional coupling between the ventral
             medial PFC and the amygdala for negatively intense memories,
             but less coupling for positively intense memories. In sum,
             the fMRI data suggest that there was greater recruitment and
             coupling of emotional brain regions during the retrieval of
             negatively intense AMs in the PTSD group when compared to
             controls.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.011},
   Key = {fds253720}
}

@article{fds253714,
   Author = {Janssen, SMJ and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Age Effects in Cultural Life Scripts.},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {291-298},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9767 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Life scripts are culturally shared expectations about the
             timing of life events in an idealized life course. Because
             they are cultural semantic knowledge, they should be known
             by all adult age groups including those who have not lived
             through all events in the life script, but this has not been
             tested previously. Young, middle-aged and older adults from
             the Netherlands were therefore asked in this online study to
             imagine an ordinary Dutch infant and to name the seven most
             important events that were likely to take place in the life
             of this prototypical child. Participants subsequently
             answered questions about at what ages these events were
             expected to occur and about their prevalence, importance and
             valence. We found that the cultural life script was similar
             for young, middle-aged and older adults and for adults with
             different educational attainment.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1690},
   Key = {fds253714}
}

@article{fds253723,
   Author = {Janssen, SMJ and Rubin, DC and St, JPL},
   Title = {The temporal distribution of autobiographical memory:
             changes in reliving and vividness over the life span do not
             explain the reminiscence bump.},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-11},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21264610},
   Abstract = {When autobiographical memories are elicited with word cues,
             personal events from middle childhood to early adulthood are
             overrepresented compared to events from other periods. It
             is, however, unclear whether these memories are also
             associated with greater recollection. In this online study,
             we examined whether autobiographical memories from
             adolescence and early adulthood are recollected more than
             memories from other lifetime periods. Participants rated
             personal events that were elicited with cue words on
             reliving or vividness. Consistent with previous studies,
             most memories came from the period in which the participants
             were between 6 and 20 years old. The memories from this
             period were not relived more or recalled more vividly than
             memories from other lifetime periods, suggesting that they
             do not involve more recollection. Recent events had higher
             levels of reliving and vividness than remote events, and
             older adults reported a stronger recollective experience
             than younger adults.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-010-0003-x},
   Key = {fds253723}
}

@article{fds253689,
   Author = {Siegler, IC and Williams, RB and Rimer, BK and Rubin, DC and Brummett,
             BH and Barefoot, JC and Costa, PT},
   Title = {WHEN I'M 64: FINDINGS FROM THE UNC ALUMNI HEART
             STUDY},
   Journal = {INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL MEDICINE},
   Volume = {17},
   Pages = {9-10},
   Publisher = {SPRINGER},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {1070-5503},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000280088500019&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds253689}
}

@article{fds253726,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Boals, A},
   Title = {People who expect to enter psychotherapy are prone to
             believing that they have forgotten memories of childhood
             trauma and abuse.},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {556-562},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {PMC2904647},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20623421},
   Abstract = {We asked 1004 undergraduates to estimate both the
             probability that they would enter therapy and the
             probability that they experienced but could not remember
             incidents of potentially life-threatening childhood traumas
             or physical and sexual abuse. We found a linear relation
             between the expectation of entering therapy and the belief
             that one had, but cannot now remember, childhood trauma and
             abuse. Thus individuals who are prone to seek psychotherapy
             are also prone to accept a suggested memory of childhood
             trauma or abuse as fitting their expectations. In multiple
             regressions predicting the probability of forgotten memories
             of childhood traumas and abuse, the expectation of entering
             therapy remained as a substantial predictor when self-report
             measures of mood, anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder
             symptom severity, and trauma exposure were
             included.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2010.490787},
   Key = {fds253726}
}

@article{fds253727,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Emotion and autobiographical memory: considerations from
             posttraumatic stress disorder.},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {132-133},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20374934},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.plrev.2010.01.001},
   Key = {fds253727}
}

@article{fds253728,
   Author = {Botzung, A and Rubin, DC and Miles, A and Cabeza, R and Labar,
             KS},
   Title = {Mental hoop diaries: emotional memories of a college
             basketball game in rival fans.},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {2130-2137},
   Publisher = {Society for Neuroscience},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20147540},
   Abstract = {The rivalry between the men's basketball teams of Duke
             University and the University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill
             (UNC) is one of the most storied traditions in college
             sports. A subculture of students at each university form
             social bonds with fellow fans, develop expertise in college
             basketball rules, team statistics, and individual players,
             and self-identify as a member of a fan group. The present
             study capitalized on the high personal investment of these
             fans and the strong affective tenor of a Duke-UNC basketball
             game to examine the neural correlates of emotional memory
             retrieval for a complex sporting event. Male fans watched a
             competitive, archived game in a social setting. During a
             subsequent functional magnetic resonance imaging session,
             participants viewed video clips depicting individual plays
             of the game that ended with the ball being released toward
             the basket. For each play, participants recalled whether or
             not the shot went into the basket. Hemodynamic signal
             changes time locked to correct memory decisions were
             analyzed as a function of emotional intensity and valence,
             according to the fan's perspective. Results showed
             intensity-modulated retrieval activity in midline cortical
             structures, sensorimotor cortex, the striatum, and the
             medial temporal lobe, including the amygdala. Positively
             valent memories specifically recruited processing in dorsal
             frontoparietal regions, and additional activity in the
             insula and medial temporal lobe for positively valent shots
             recalled with high confidence. This novel paradigm reveals
             how brain regions implicated in emotion, memory retrieval,
             visuomotor imagery, and social cognition contribute to the
             recollection of specific plays in the mind of a sports
             fan.},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2481-09.2010},
   Key = {fds253728}
}

@article{fds253731,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Boals, A and Klein, K},
   Title = {Autobiographical Memories for Very Negative Events: The
             Effects of Thinking about and Rating Memories.},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {35-48},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0147-5916},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21423832},
   Abstract = {In three related experiments, 250 participants rated
             properties of their autobiographical memory of a very
             negative event before and after writing about either their
             deepest thoughts and emotions of the event or a control
             topic. Levels of emotional intensity of the event, distress
             associated with the event, intrusive symptoms, and other
             phenomenological memory properties decreased over the course
             of the experiment, but did not differ by writing condition.
             We argue that the act of answering our extensive questions
             about a very negative event led to the decrease, thereby
             masking the effects of expressive writing. To show that the
             changes could not be explained by the mere passage of time,
             we replicated our findings in a fourth experiment in which
             all 208 participants nominated a very negative event, but
             only half the participants rated properties of their memory
             in the first session. Implications for reducing the effects
             of negative autobiographical memories are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10608-008-9226-6},
   Key = {fds253731}
}

@article{fds253729,
   Author = {Botzung, A and Labar, KS and Kragel, P and Miles, A and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Component Neural Systems for the Creation of Emotional
             Memories during Free Viewing of a Complex, Real-World
             Event.},
   Volume = {4},
   Pages = {34},
   Publisher = {Frontiers Media SA},
   Year = {2010},
   ISSN = {PMC2876881},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20508750},
   Abstract = {To investigate the neural systems that contribute to the
             formation of complex, self-relevant emotional memories,
             dedicated fans of rival college basketball teams watched a
             competitive game while undergoing functional magnetic
             resonance imaging (fMRI). During a subsequent recognition
             memory task, participants were shown video clips depicting
             plays of the game, stemming either from previously-viewed
             game segments (targets) or from non-viewed portions of the
             same game (foils). After an old-new judgment, participants
             provided emotional valence and intensity ratings of the
             clips. A data driven approach was first used to decompose
             the fMRI signal acquired during free viewing of the game
             into spatially independent components. Correlations were
             then calculated between the identified components and
             post-scanning emotion ratings for successfully encoded
             targets. Two components were correlated with intensity
             ratings, including temporal lobe regions implicated in
             memory and emotional functions, such as the hippocampus and
             amygdala, as well as a midline fronto-cingulo-parietal
             network implicated in social cognition and self-relevant
             processing. These data were supported by a general linear
             model analysis, which revealed additional valence effects in
             fronto-striatal-insular regions when plays were divided into
             positive and negative events according to the fan's
             perspective. Overall, these findings contribute to our
             understanding of how emotional factors impact distributed
             neural systems to successfully encode dynamic,
             personally-relevant event sequences.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fnhum.2010.00034},
   Key = {fds253729}
}

@article{fds253717,
   Author = {Rice, HJ and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {I can see it both ways: first- and third-person visual
             perspectives at retrieval.},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {877-890},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {1053-8100},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10077 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The number of studies examining visual perspective during
             retrieval has recently grown. However, the way in which
             perspective has been conceptualized differs across studies.
             Some studies have suggested perspective is experienced as
             either a first-person or a third-person perspective, whereas
             others have suggested both perspectives can be experienced
             during a single retrieval attempt. This aspect of
             perspective was examined across three studies, which used
             different measurement techniques commonly used in studies of
             perspective. Results suggest that individuals can experience
             more than one perspective when recalling events.
             Furthermore, the experience of the two perspectives
             correlated differentially with ratings of vividness,
             suggesting that the two perspectives should not be
             considered in opposition of one another. We also found
             evidence of a gender effect in the experience of
             perspective, with females experiencing third-person
             perspectives more often than males. Future studies should
             allow for the experience of more than one perspective during
             retrieval.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2009.07.004},
   Key = {fds253717}
}

@article{fds253730,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Talarico, JM},
   Title = {A comparison of dimensional models of emotion: evidence from
             emotions, prototypical events, autobiographical memories,
             and words.},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {802-808},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {PMC2784275},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19691001},
   Abstract = {The intensity and valence of 30 emotion terms, 30 events
             typical of those emotions, and 30 autobiographical memories
             cued by those emotions were each rated by different groups
             of 40 undergraduates. A vector model gave a consistently
             better account of the data than a circumplex model, both
             overall and in the absence of high-intensity, neutral
             valence stimuli. The Positive Activation - Negative
             Activation (PANA) model could be tested at high levels of
             activation, where it is identical to the vector model. The
             results replicated when ratings of arousal were used instead
             of ratings of intensity for the events and autobiographical
             memories. A reanalysis of word norms gave further support
             for the vector and PANA models by demonstrating that neutral
             valence, high-arousal ratings resulted from the averaging of
             individual positive and negative valence ratings. Thus,
             compared to a circumplex model, vector and PANA models
             provided overall better fits.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210903130764},
   Key = {fds253730}
}

@article{fds253719,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {The frequency of voluntary and involuntary autobiographical
             memories across the life span.},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {679-688},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19487759},
   Abstract = {In the present study, ratings of the memory of an important
             event from the previous week on the frequency of voluntary
             and involuntary retrieval, belief in its accuracy, visual
             imagery, auditory imagery, setting, emotional intensity,
             valence, narrative coherence, and centrality to the life
             story were obtained from 988 adults whose ages ranged from
             15 to over 90. Another 992 adults provided the same ratings
             for a memory from their confirmation day, when they were at
             about age 14. The frequencies of involuntary and voluntary
             retrieval were similar. Both frequencies were predicted by
             emotional intensity and centrality to the life story. The
             results from the present study-which is the first to measure
             the frequency of voluntary and involuntary retrieval for the
             same events-are counter to both cognitive and clinical
             theories, which consistently claim that involuntary memories
             are infrequent as compared with voluntary memories. Age and
             gender differences are noted.},
   Doi = {10.3758/37.5.679},
   Key = {fds253719}
}

@article{fds253701,
   Author = {Talarico, JM and Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral
             details},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {380-398},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0269-9931},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10078 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Emotional arousal and negative affect enhance recall of
             central aspects of an event. However, the role of discrete
             emotions in selective memory processing is understudied.
             Undergraduates were asked to recall and rate
             autobiographical memories of eight emotional events. Details
             of each memory were rated as central or peripheral to the
             event. Significance of the event, vividness, reliving and
             other aspects of remembering were also rated for each
             memory. Positive affect enhanced recall of peripheral
             details. Furthermore, the impairment of peripheral recall
             was greatest in memories of anger, not of fear. Reliving the
             experience at retrieval was negatively correlated with
             recall of peripheral details for some emotions (e.g., anger)
             but not others (e.g., fear), irrespective of similarities in
             affect and intensity. Within individuals, recall of
             peripheral details was correlated with less belief in the
             memory's accuracy and more likelihood to recall the memory
             from one's own eyes (i.e., a field perspective).},
   Doi = {10.1080/02699930801993999},
   Key = {fds253701}
}

@misc{fds253655,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Oral traditions as collective memories: Implications for a
             general theory of individual and collective
             memory},
   Pages = {273-287},
   Booktitle = {Memory in Mind and Culture},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {P. Boyer and J. Wertsch},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780521760782},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511626999.017},
   Abstract = {© Cambridge University Press 2009 and 2010. Historians are
             interested in sites of memory, understood as places where
             groups of people engage in public activity through which
             they express “a collective shared knowledge … of the
             past, on which a group's sense of unity and individuality is
             based” (Assmann, 1995). The group that goes to such sites
             inherits earlier meanings attached to the event, as well as
             adding new meanings. Their activity is crucial to the
             presentation and preservation of commemorative sites. When
             such groups disperse or disappear, sites of memory lose
             their initial force, and may fade away entirely. Thus,
             historians are more interested in remembrance as a cultural
             practice than in memory as an individual's capacity to
             recall or reconfigure the past. The term, sites of memory,
             abumbrated in a seven-volume study edited by Pierre Nora
             (n.d.) has been extended to many different texts, from
             legends, to stories, to concepts. In this brief essay, I
             define the term more narrowly to mean physical sites where
             commemorative acts take place. In the twentieth century,
             most such sites marked the loss of life in war. It is these
             sites that have attracted the attention of entire battalions
             of historians in the past twenty-five years. What makes such
             sites of memory attractive for historical research is their
             character as topoi with a life history. They have an
             initial, creative phase, when they are constructed or
             adapted to particular commemorative purposes. Then follows a
             period of institutionalization and routinization of their
             use.},
   Doi = {10.1017/CBO9780511626999.017},
   Key = {fds253655}
}

@article{fds198585,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Berntsen, D. and Hutson, M.},
   Title = {The normative and the personal life: Individual differences
             in life scripts and life stories among U.S.A. and Danish
             undergraduates.},
   Journal = {Memory},
   Volume = {17},
   Pages = {54-68},
   Year = {2009},
   ISSN = {PMC3042895},
   Key = {fds198585}
}

@article{fds198587,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Berntsen, D.},
   Title = {Most people who think that they are likely to enter
             psychotherapy also think it is plausible that they could
             have forgotten their own memories of childhood sexual
             abuse.},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {23},
   Pages = {170-173},
   Year = {2009},
   ISSN = {PMC2752902},
   Key = {fds198587}
}

@article{fds157259,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Berntsen, D.},
   Title = {The Frequency of Voluntary and Involuntary Autobiographical
             Memories across the Lifespan},
   Year = {2009},
   Key = {fds157259}
}

@article{fds157261,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Talarico, J.M.},
   Title = {A comparison of dimensional models of emotion: Evidence from
             emotions, prototypical events, autobiographical memories,
             and words},
   Year = {2009},
   Key = {fds157261}
}

@misc{fds253661,
   Author = {Talarico, JM and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Flashbulb memories result from ordinary memory processes and
             extraordinary event characteristics},
   Pages = {79-97},
   Booktitle = {Flashbulb Memories: New Issues and New Perspectives},
   Publisher = {Routledge},
   Editor = {Luminet, O. and Curci, A. and Conway, M.A.},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {November},
   ISBN = {9780203889930},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203889930},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203889930},
   Key = {fds253661}
}

@article{fds253740,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Boals, A and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {Memory in posttraumatic stress disorder: properties of
             voluntary and involuntary, traumatic and nontraumatic
             autobiographical memories in people with and without
             posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms.},
   Volume = {137},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {591-614},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0096-3445},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18999355},
   Abstract = {One hundred fifteen undergraduates rated 15 word-cued
             memories and their 3 most negatively stressful, 3 most
             positive, and 7 most important events and completed tests of
             personality and depression. Eighty-nine also recorded
             involuntary memories online for 1 week. In the first 3-way
             comparisons needed to test existing theories, comparisons
             were made of memories of stressful events versus control
             events and involuntary versus voluntary memories in people
             high versus low in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
             symptom severity. For all participants, stressful memories
             had more emotional intensity, more frequent voluntary and
             involuntary retrieval, but not more fragmentation. For all
             memories, participants with greater PTSD symptom severity
             showed the same differences. Involuntary memories had more
             emotional intensity and less centrality to the life story
             than voluntary memories. Meeting the diagnostic criteria for
             traumatic events had no effect, but the emotional responses
             to events did. In 533 undergraduates, correlations among
             measures were replicated and the Negative Intensity factor
             of the Affect Intensity Measure correlated with PTSD symptom
             severity. No special trauma mechanisms were needed to
             account for the results, which are summarized by the
             autobiographical memory theory of PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0013165},
   Key = {fds253740}
}

@article{fds253735,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D and Bohni, MK},
   Title = {A memory-based model of posttraumatic stress disorder:
             evaluating basic assumptions underlying the PTSD
             diagnosis.},
   Volume = {115},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {985-1011},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0033-295X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18954211},
   Abstract = {In the mnemonic model of posttraumatic stress disorder
             (PTSD), the current memory of a negative event, not the
             event itself, determines symptoms. The model is an
             alternative to the current event-based etiology of PTSD
             represented in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
             Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; American Psychiatric
             Association, 2000). The model accounts for important and
             reliable findings that are often inconsistent with the
             current diagnostic view and that have been neglected by
             theoretical accounts of the disorder, including the
             following observations. The diagnosis needs objective
             information about the trauma and peritraumatic emotions but
             uses retrospective memory reports that can have substantial
             biases. Negative events and emotions that do not satisfy the
             current diagnostic criteria for a trauma can be followed by
             symptoms that would otherwise qualify for PTSD. Predisposing
             factors that affect the current memory have large effects on
             symptoms. The inability-to-recall-an-important-aspect-of-the-trauma
             symptom does not correlate with other symptoms. Loss or
             enhancement of the trauma memory affects PTSD symptoms in
             predictable ways. Special mechanisms that apply only to
             traumatic memories are not needed, increasing parsimony and
             the knowledge that can be applied to understanding
             PTSD.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0013397},
   Key = {fds253735}
}

@article{fds253739,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC and Johansen, MK},
   Title = {Contrasting Models of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Reply
             to.},
   Volume = {115},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1099-1106},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0033-295X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10082 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {We address the four main points in Monroe and Mineka
             (2008)'s Comment. First, we first show that the DSM PTSD
             diagnosis includes an etiology and that it is based on a
             theoretical model with a distinguished history in psychology
             and psychiatry. Two tenets of this theoretical model are
             that voluntary (strategic) recollections of the trauma are
             fragmented and incomplete while involuntary (spontaneous)
             recollections are vivid and persistent and yield privileged
             access to traumatic material. Second, we describe
             differences between our model and other cognitive models of
             PTSD. We argue that these other models share the same two
             tenets as the diagnosis and we show that these two tenets
             are largely unsupported by empirical evidence. Third, we
             counter arguments about the strength of the evidence
             favoring the mnemonic model, and fourth, we show that
             concerns about the causal role of memory in PTSD are based
             on views of causality that are generally inappropriate for
             the explanation of PTSD in the social and biological
             sciences.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0013730},
   Key = {fds253739}
}

@article{fds253718,
   Author = {St, JP and Rubin, DC and LaBar, KS and Cabeza, R},
   Title = {The short and long of it: neural correlates of
             temporal-order memory for autobiographical
             events.},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1327-1341},
   Publisher = {MIT Press},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18284345},
   Abstract = {Previous functional neuroimaging studies of temporal-order
             memory have investigated memory for laboratory stimuli that
             are causally unrelated and poor in sensory detail. In
             contrast, the present functional magnetic resonance imaging
             (fMRI) study investigated temporal-order memory for
             autobiographical events that were causally interconnected
             and rich in sensory detail. Participants took photographs at
             many campus locations over a period of several hours, and
             the following day they were scanned while making
             temporal-order judgments to pairs of photographs from
             different locations. By manipulating the temporal lag
             between the two locations in each trial, we compared the
             neural correlates associated with reconstruction processes,
             which we hypothesized depended on recollection and
             contribute mainly to short lags, and distance processes,
             which we hypothesized to depend on familiarity and
             contribute mainly to longer lags. Consistent with our
             hypotheses, parametric fMRI analyses linked shorter lags to
             activations in regions previously associated with
             recollection (left prefrontal, parahippocampal, precuneus,
             and visual cortices), and longer lags with regions
             previously associated with familiarity (right prefrontal
             cortex). The hemispheric asymmetry in prefrontal cortex
             activity fits very well with evidence and theories regarding
             the contributions of the left versus right prefrontal cortex
             to memory (recollection vs. familiarity processes) and
             cognition (systematic vs. heuristic processes). In sum,
             using a novel photo-paradigm, this study provided the first
             evidence regarding the neural correlates of temporal-order
             for autobiographical events.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn.2008.20091},
   Key = {fds253718}
}

@article{fds253732,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D and Hutson, M},
   Title = {The normative and personal life: Individual and cultural
             differences in personal life stories and cultural life
             scripts},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {54-68},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19105087},
   Abstract = {Life scripts are culturally shared expectations about the
             order and timing of life events in a prototypical life
             course. American and Danish undergraduates produced life
             story events and life scripts by listing the seven most
             important events in their own lives and in the lives of
             hypothetical people living ordinary lives. They also rated
             their events on several scales and completed measures of
             depression, PTSD symptoms, and centrality of a negative
             event to their lives. The Danish life script replicated
             earlier work; the American life script showed minor
             differences from the Danish life script, apparently
             reflecting genuine differences in shared events as well as
             less homogeneity in the American sample. Both consisted of
             mostly positive events that came disproportionately from
             ages 15 to 30. Valence of life story events correlated with
             life script valence, depression, PTSD symptoms, and
             identity. In the Danish undergraduates, measures of life
             story deviation from the life script correlated with
             measures of depression and PTSD symptoms.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210802541442},
   Key = {fds253732}
}

@article{fds253752,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The reappearance hypothesis revisited: recurrent involuntary
             memories after traumatic events and in everyday
             life.},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {449-460},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10088 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Recurrent involuntary memories are autobiographical memories
             that come to mind with no preceding retrieval attempt and
             that are subjectively experienced as being repetitive.
             Clinically, they are classified as a symptom of
             posttraumatic stress disorder. The present work is the first
             to systematically examine recurrent involuntary memories
             outside clinical settings. Study 1 examines recurrent
             involuntary memories among survivors of the tsunami
             catastrophe in Southeast Asia in 2004. Study 2 examines
             recurrent involuntary memories in a large general
             population. Study 3 examines whether the contents of
             recurrent involuntary memories recorded in a diary study are
             duplicates of, or differ from, one another. We show that
             recurrent involuntary memories are not limited to clinical
             populations or to emotionally negative experiences; that
             they typically do not come to mind in a fixed and
             unchangeable form; and that they show the same pattern
             regarding accessibility as do autobiographical memories in
             general. We argue that recurrent involuntary memories after
             traumas and in everyday life can be explained in terms of
             general and well-established mechanisms of autobiographical
             memory.},
   Doi = {10.3758/mc.36.2.449},
   Key = {fds253752}
}

@article{fds253741,
   Author = {Skotko, BG and Rubin, DC and Tupler, LA},
   Title = {H.M.'s personal crossword puzzles: understanding memory and
             language.},
   Journal = {Memory},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {89-96},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18286414},
   Abstract = {The amnesic patient H.M. has been solving crossword puzzles
             nearly all his life. Here, we analysed the linguistic
             content of 277 of H.M.'s crossword-puzzle solutions. H.M.
             did not have any unusual difficulties with the orthographic
             and grammatical components inherent to the puzzles. He
             exhibited few spelling errors, responded with appropriate
             parts of speech, and provided answers that were, at times,
             more convincing to observers than those supplied by the
             answer keys. These results suggest that H.M.'s lexical
             word-retrieval skills remain fluid despite his profound
             anterograde amnesia. Once acquired, the maintenance of
             written language comprehension and production does not seem
             to require intact medial temporal lobe structures.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210701864580},
   Key = {fds253741}
}

@article{fds253736,
   Author = {Daselaar, SM and Rice, HJ and Greenberg, DL and Cabeza, R and LaBar, KS and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory:
             neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and
             reliving.},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {217-229},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press (OUP)},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17548799},
   Abstract = {We sought to map the time course of autobiographical memory
             retrieval, including brain regions that mediate
             phenomenological experiences of reliving and emotional
             intensity. Participants recalled personal memories to
             auditory word cues during event-related functional magnetic
             resonance imaging (fMRI). Participants pressed a button when
             a memory was accessed, maintained and elaborated the memory,
             and then gave subjective ratings of emotion and reliving. A
             novel fMRI approach based on timing differences capitalized
             on the protracted reconstructive process of autobiographical
             memory to segregate brain areas contributing to initial
             access and later elaboration and maintenance of episodic
             memories. The initial period engaged hippocampal,
             retrosplenial, and medial and right prefrontal activity,
             whereas the later period recruited visual, precuneus, and
             left prefrontal activity. Emotional intensity ratings were
             correlated with activity in several regions, including the
             amygdala and the hippocampus during the initial period.
             Reliving ratings were correlated with activity in visual
             cortex and ventromedial and inferior prefrontal regions
             during the later period. Frontopolar cortex was the only
             brain region sensitive to emotional intensity across both
             periods. Results were confirmed by time-locked averages of
             the fMRI signal. The findings indicate dynamic recruitment
             of emotion-, memory-, and sensory-related brain regions
             during remembering and their dissociable contributions to
             phenomenological features of the memories.},
   Doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhm048},
   Key = {fds253736}
}

@article{fds157255,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Boals, A. and Berntsen, D.},
   Title = {Memory in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Properties of
             voluntary and involuntary, traumatic and non-traumatic
             autobiographical memories in people with and without PTSD
             symptoms},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: General},
   Number = {137},
   Pages = {591-614},
   Year = {2008},
   Key = {fds157255}
}

@article{fds253734,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Mix levels of analysis with care; genres not at
             all},
   Volume = {7},
   Pages = {66-71},
   Year = {2008},
   Key = {fds253734}
}

@article{fds253737,
   Author = {Boals, A and Rubin, DC and Klein, K},
   Title = {Memory and coping with stress: the relationship between
             cognitive-emotional distinctiveness, memory valence, and
             distress.},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {637-657},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2008},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10083 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Cognitive-emotional distinctiveness (CED), the extent to
             which an individual separates emotions from an event in the
             cognitive representation of the event, was explored in four
             studies. CED was measured using a modified multidimensional
             scaling procedure. The first study found that lower levels
             of CED in memories of the September 11 terrorist attacks
             predicted greater frequency of intrusive thoughts about the
             attacks. The second study revealed that CED levels are
             higher in negative events, in comparison to positive events
             and that low CED levels in emotionally intense negative
             events are associated with a pattern of greater
             event-related distress. The third study replicated the
             findings from the previous study when examining CED levels
             in participants' memories of the 2004 Presidential election.
             The fourth study revealed that low CED in emotionally
             intense negative events is associated with worse mental
             health. We argue that CED is an adaptive and healthy coping
             feature of stressful memories.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210802083098},
   Key = {fds253737}
}

@article{fds253738,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC and Bohni, MK},
   Title = {Postscript: Evidence and counterevidence},
   Volume = {115},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1106-1107},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2008},
   ISSN = {0033-295X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10086 Duke open
             access},
   Doi = {10.1037/0033-295X.115.4.1106},
   Key = {fds253738}
}

@misc{fds302144,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {How Memory for Stressful Events affects Identity},
   Pages = {118-129},
   Booktitle = {Self Psychology: An approach to cognitive
             psychology},
   Publisher = {Kaneko Shobo},
   Editor = {Naka, M and Yamashita, K},
   Year = {2008},
   Key = {fds302144}
}

@article{fds253743,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {People believe it is plausible to have forgotten memories of
             childhood sexual abuse.},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {776-778},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17972748},
   Abstract = {Pezdek, Blandon-Gitlin, and Gabbay (2006) found that
             perceptions of the plausibility of events increase the
             likelihood that imagination may induce false memories of
             those events. Using a survey conducted by Gallup, we asked a
             large sample of the general population how plausible it
             would be for a person with longstanding emotional problems
             and a need for psychotherapy to be a victim of childhood
             sexual abuse, even though the person could not remember the
             abuse. Only 18% indicated that it was implausible or very
             implausible, whereas 67% indicated that such an occurrence
             was either plausible or very plausible. Combined with Pezdek
             et al.s' findings, and counter to their conclusions, our
             findings imply that there is a substantial danger of
             inducing false memories of childhood sexual abuse through
             imagination in psychotherapy.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196836},
   Key = {fds253743}
}

@article{fds253745,
   Author = {Talarico, JM and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Flashbulb memories are special after all; in phenomenology,
             not accuracy},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {557-578},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10092 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Consistency of flashbulb memories (FBMs) of the 11th
             September terrorist attacks and of everyday memories (EDMs)
             of the preceding weekend do not differ, in both cases
             declining over the following year for a group of Duke
             University undergraduates. However, ratings of recollection,
             vividness and other phenomenological properties were
             consistently higher for FBMs than for EDMs across time.
             Belief in the accuracy of memory was initially high for both
             memories, but declined over time only for EDMs. These
             findings confirm that FBMs are not extraordinarily accurate,
             but they may systematically differ from EDMs in other
             meaningful ways. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1293},
   Key = {fds253745}
}

@article{fds253746,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schrauf, RW and Gulgoz, S and Naka,
             M},
   Title = {Cross-cultural variability of component processes in
             autobiographical remembering: Japan, Turkey, and the
             USA.},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {536-547},
   Publisher = {PSYCHOLOGY PRESS},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10093 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Although the underlying mechanics of autobiographical memory
             may be identical across cultures, the processing of
             information differs. Undergraduates from Japan, Turkey, and
             the USA rated 30 autobiographical memories on 15
             phenomenological and cognitive properties. Mean values were
             similar across cultures, with means from the Japanese sample
             being lower on most measures but higher on belief in the
             accuracy of their memories. Correlations within individuals
             were also similar across cultures, with correlations from
             the Turkish sample being higher between measures of language
             and measures of recollection and belief. For all three
             cultures, in multiple regression analyses, measures of
             recollection were predicted by visual imagery, auditory
             imagery, and emotions, whereas measures of belief were
             predicted by knowledge of the setting. These results show
             subtle cultural differences in the experience of
             remembering.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210701332679},
   Key = {fds253746}
}

@misc{fds253662,
   Author = {Davis, M and Loftus, EF and Rubin, DC and Wixted,
             JT},
   Title = {Forgetting},
   Pages = {315-337},
   Booktitle = {Science of Memory: Concepts},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {May},
   ISBN = {9780195310443},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195310443.003.0015},
   Abstract = {© 2007 by Henry L. Roediger III, Yadin Dudai, and Susan M.
             Fitzpatrick. All rights reserved. This part presents four
             chapters on the concept of forgetting. The first chapter
             analyzes the term "forgetting". The second discusses the
             impact of misinformation on the ability to remember previous
             event details. The third considers whether forgetting is a
             useful concept in the science of memory. It argues that it
             is not an especially useful in terms of what it denotes, but
             that what it connotes needs to be kept. The fourth presents
             a synthesis of the chapters in this part.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195310443.003.0015},
   Key = {fds253662}
}

@article{fds253742,
   Author = {Berntse, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {When a trauma becomes a key to identity: Enhanced
             integration of trauma memories predicts posttraumatic stress
             disorder symptoms},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {417-431},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10095 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The Centrality of Event Scale (CES) measures the extent to
             which a traumatic memory forms a central component of
             personnal identity, a turning point in the life story and a
             reference point for everyday inferences. In two studies, we
             show that the CES is positively correlated with severity of
             PTSD symptoms, even when controlling for measures of
             anxiety, depression, dissociation and self-consciousness.
             The findings contradict the widespread view that poor
             integration of the traumatic memory into one's life story is
             a main cause of PTSD symptoms. Instead, enhanced integration
             appears to be a key issue. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley &
             Sons, Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1290},
   Key = {fds253742}
}

@misc{fds157206,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Forgetting: Its role in the science of memory},
   Pages = {325-328},
   Booktitle = {Science of memory: Concepts},
   Publisher = {New York: Oxford University Press},
   Editor = {H.L. Roediger, III and Y. Dudai and S.M. Fitzpatrick},
   Year = {2007},
   Key = {fds157206}
}

@misc{fds157207,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory and aging},
   Booktitle = {Cognitive aging: A primer. Second edition},
   Publisher = {New York: Psychology Press},
   Editor = {D.C. Park and N. Schwartz},
   Year = {2007},
   Key = {fds157207}
}

@article{fds253748,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The Basic-Systems Model of Episodic Memory.},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {277-311},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {1745-6916},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000207450200001&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Behavior, neuropsychology, and neuroimaging suggest that
             episodic memories are constructed from interactions among
             the following basic systems: vision, audition, olfaction,
             other senses, spatial imagery, language, emotion, narrative,
             motor output, explicit memory, and search and retrieval.
             Each system has its own well-documented functions, neural
             substrates, processes, structures, and kinds of schemata.
             However, the systems have not been considered as interacting
             components of episodic memory, as is proposed here.
             Autobiographical memory and oral traditions are used to
             demonstrate the usefulness of the basic-systems model in
             accounting for existing data and predicting novel findings,
             and to argue that the model, or one similar to it, is the
             only way to understand episodic memory for complex stimuli
             routinely encountered outside the laboratory.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00017.x},
   Key = {fds253748}
}

@article{fds253749,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Emotion and vantage point in autobiographical},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1193-1215},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {0269-9931},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10098 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memories may be recalled from two different
             perspectives: Field memories in which the person seems to
             remember the scene from his/her original point of view and
             observer memories in which the rememberer sees him/herself
             in the memory image. Here, 122 undergraduates participated
             in an experiment examining the relation between field vs.
             observer perspective in memory for 10 different emotional
             states, including both positive and negative emotions and
             emotions associated with high vs. low intensity. Observer
             perspective was associated with reduced sensory and
             emotional reliving across all emotions. This effect was
             observed for naturally occurring memory perspective and when
             participants were instructed to change their perspective
             from field to observer, but not when participants were
             instructed to change perspective from observer to
             field.},
   Doi = {10.1080/02699930500371190},
   Key = {fds253749}
}

@article{fds253747,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {People over forty feel 20% younger than their age:
             subjective age across the lifespan.},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {776-780},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17328372},
   Abstract = {Subjective age--the age people think of themselves
             asbeing--is measured in a representative Danish sample of
             1,470 adults between 20 and 97 years of age through
             personal, in-home interviews. On the average, adults younger
             than 25 have older subjective ages, and those older than 25
             have younger subjective ages, favoring a
             lifespan-developmental view over an age-denial view of
             subjective age. When the discrepancy between subjective and
             chronological age is calculated as a proportion of
             chronological age, no increase is seen after age 40; older
             respondents feel 20% younger than their actual age.
             Demographic variables (gender, income, and education)
             account for very little variance in subjective
             age.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03193996},
   Key = {fds253747}
}

@article{fds253753,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Flashbulb memories and posttraumatic stress reactions across
             the life span: age-related effects of the German occupation
             of Denmark during World War II.},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {127-139},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0882-7974},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10099 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {A representative sample of older Danes were interviewed
             about experiences from the German occupation of Denmark in
             World War II. The number of participants with flashbulb
             memories for the German invasion (1940) and capitulation
             (1945) increased with participants' age at the time of the
             events up to age 8. Among participants under 8 years at the
             time of their most traumatic event, age at the time
             correlated positively with the current level of
             posttraumatic stress reactions and the vividness of
             stressful memories and their centrality to life story and
             identity. These findings were replicated in Study 2 for
             self-nominated stressful events sampled from the entire life
             span using a representative sample of Danes born after 1945.
             The results are discussed in relation to posttraumatic
             stress disorder and childhood amnesia.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.127},
   Key = {fds253753}
}

@article{fds253750,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The centrality of event scale: a measure of integrating a
             trauma into one's identity and its relation to
             post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms.},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {219-231},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0005-7967},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10104 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {We introduce a new scale that measures how central an event
             is to a person's identity and life story. For the most
             stressful or traumatic event in a person's life, the full
             20-item Centrality of Event Scale (CES) and the short 7-item
             scale are reliable (alpha's of .94 and .88, respectively) in
             a sample of 707 undergraduates. The scale correlates .38
             with PTSD symptom severity and .23 with depression. The
             present findings are discussed in relation to previous work
             on individual differences related to PTSD symptoms. Possible
             connections between the CES and measures of maladaptive
             attributions and rumination are considered along with
             suggestions for future research.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.brat.2005.01.009},
   Key = {fds253750}
}

@article{fds253698,
   Author = {Sheen, M and Kemp, S and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Disputes over memory ownership: What memories are
             disputed?},
   Volume = {5 Suppl 1},
   Pages = {9-13},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2006},
   ISSN = {1601-1848},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10097 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The ownership of memories is sometimes disputed,
             particularly by twins. Examination of 77 disputed memories,
             71 provided by twins, showed that most of the remembered
             events are negative and that the disputants appear to be
             self-serving. They claim for themselves memories for
             achievements and suffered misfortunes but are more likely to
             give away memories of personal wrongdoing. The research
             suggests that some of the memories in which we play a
             leading role might in fact have been the experiences of
             others.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00189.x},
   Key = {fds253698}
}

@article{fds253757,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A basic-systems approach to autobiographical
             memory},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {79-83},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0963-7214},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10105 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Memory for complex everyday events involving vision,
             hearing, smell, emotion, narrative, and language cannot be
             understood without considering the properties of the
             separate systems that process and store each of these forms
             of information. Using this premise as a starting point, my
             colleagues and I found that visual memory plays a central
             role in autobiographical memory: The strength of
             recollection of an event is predicted best by the vividness
             of its visual imagery, and a loss of visual memory causes a
             general amnesia. Examination of autobiographical memories in
             individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
             suggests that the lack of coherence often noted in memories
             of traumatic events is not due to a lack of coherence either
             of the memory itself or of the narrative that integrates the
             memory into the life story. Rather, making the traumatic
             memory central to the life story correlates positively with
             increased PTSD symptoms. The basic-systems approach has
             yielded insights into autobiographical memory's
             phenomenology, neuropsychology, clinical disorders, and
             neural basis. Copyright © 2005 American Psychological
             Society.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00339.x},
   Key = {fds253757}
}

@article{fds253760,
   Author = {Bluck, S and Alea, N and Habermas, T and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A tale of three functions: The self-reported uses of
             autobiographical memory},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {91-117},
   Publisher = {Guilford Publications},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0278-016X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10106 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Theories hold that autobiographical memory serves several
             broad functions (directive, self, and social). In the
             current study, items were derived from the theoretical
             literature to create the Thinking About Life Experiences
             (TALE) questionnaire to empirically assess these three
             functions. Participants (N = 167) completed the TALE. To
             examine convergent validity, they also rated their overall
             tendency to think about and to talk about the past and
             completed the Reminiscence Functions Scale (Webster, 1997).
             The results lend support to the existence of these
             theoretical functions, but also offer room for refinements
             in future thinking about both the breadth and specificity of
             the functions that autobiographical memory
             serves.},
   Doi = {10.1521/soco.23.1.91.59198},
   Key = {fds253760}
}

@article{fds253758,
   Author = {Greenberg, DL and Rice, HJ and Cooper, JJ and Cabeza, R and Rubin, DC and Labar, KS},
   Title = {Co-activation of the amygdala, hippocampus and inferior
             frontal gyrus during autobiographical memory
             retrieval.},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {659-674},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2005},
   ISSN = {0028-3932},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15721179},
   Abstract = {Functional MRI was used to investigate the role of medial
             temporal lobe and inferior frontal lobe regions in
             autobiographical recall. Prior to scanning, participants
             generated cue words for 50 autobiographical memories and
             rated their phenomenological properties using our
             autobiographical memory questionnaire (AMQ). During
             scanning, the cue words were presented and participants
             pressed a button when they retrieved the associated memory.
             The autobiographical retrieval task was interleaved in an
             event-related design with a semantic retrieval task
             (category generation). Region-of-interest analyses showed
             greater activation of the amygdala, hippocampus, and right
             inferior frontal gyrus during autobiographical retrieval
             relative to semantic retrieval. In addition, the left
             inferior frontal gyrus showed a more prolonged duration of
             activation in the semantic retrieval condition. A targeted
             correlational analysis revealed pronounced functional
             connectivity among the amygdala, hippocampus, and right
             inferior frontal gyrus during autobiographical retrieval but
             not during semantic retrieval. These results support
             theories of autobiographical memory that hypothesize
             co-activation of frontotemporal areas during recollection of
             episodes from the personal past.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.002},
   Key = {fds253758}
}

@article{fds253759,
   Author = {Greenberg, DL and Eacott, MJ and Brechin, D and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Visual memory loss and autobiographical amnesia: a case
             study.},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {1493-1502},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2005},
   ISSN = {0028-3932},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15989939},
   Abstract = {Amnesia typically results from trauma to the medial temporal
             regions that coordinate activation among the disparate areas
             of cortex that represent the information that make up
             autobiographical memories. We proposed that amnesia should
             also result from damage to these regions, particularly
             regions that subserve long-term visual memory [Rubin, D. C.,
             & Greenberg, D. L. (1998). Visual memory-deficit amnesia: A
             distinct amnesic presentation and etiology. Proceedings of
             the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95, 5413-5416].
             We previously found 11 such cases in the literature, and all
             11 had amnesia. We now present a detailed investigation of
             one of these patients. M.S. suffers from long-term visual
             memory loss along with some semantic deficits; he also
             manifests a severe retrograde amnesia and moderate
             anterograde amnesia. The presentation of his amnesia differs
             from that of the typical medial-temporal or lateral-temporal
             amnesic; we suggest that his visual deficits may be
             contributing to his autobiographical amnesia.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.009},
   Key = {fds253759}
}

@misc{fds302145,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Wenzel, A},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory tasks: Six common
             methods},
   Pages = {215-217},
   Booktitle = {Cognitive methods and their application to clinical
             research},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association Press},
   Editor = {Wenzel, A and Rubin, DC},
   Year = {2005},
   Key = {fds302145}
}

@misc{fds302146,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory tasks in cognitive
             research},
   Pages = {219-241},
   Booktitle = {Cognitive methods and their application to clinical
             research},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association Press},
   Editor = {Wenzel, A and Rubin, DC},
   Year = {2005},
   Key = {fds302146}
}

@book{fds309885,
   Title = {Cognitive Methods and Their Application to Clinical
             Research},
   Pages = {289 pages},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association Press},
   Editor = {Wenzel, A and Rubin, DC},
   Year = {2005},
   Key = {fds309885}
}

@article{fds253704,
   Author = {Cabeza, R and Prince, SE and Daselaar, SM and Greenberg, DL and Budde,
             M and Dolcos, F and LaBar, KS and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Brain activity during episodic retrieval of autobiographical
             and laboratory events: an fMRI study using a novel photo
             paradigm.},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1583-1594},
   Publisher = {MIT Press - Journals},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15622612},
   Abstract = {Functional neuroimaging studies of episodic memory retrieval
             generally measure brain activity while participants remember
             items encountered in the laboratory ("controlled laboratory
             condition") or events from their own life ("open
             autobiographical condition"). Differences in activation
             between these conditions may reflect differences in
             retrieval processes, memory remoteness, emotional content,
             retrieval success, self-referential processing,
             visual/spatial memory, and recollection. To clarify the
             nature of these differences, a functional MRI study was
             conducted using a novel "photo paradigm," which allows
             greater control over the autobiographical condition,
             including a measure of retrieval accuracy. Undergraduate
             students took photos in specified campus locations
             ("controlled autobiographical condition"), viewed in the
             laboratory similar photos taken by other participants
             (controlled laboratory condition), and were then scanned
             while recognizing the two kinds of photos. Both conditions
             activated a common episodic memory network that included
             medial temporal and prefrontal regions. Compared with the
             controlled laboratory condition, the controlled
             autobiographical condition elicited greater activity in
             regions associated with self-referential processing (medial
             prefrontal cortex), visual/spatial memory (visual and
             parahippocampal regions), and recollection (hippocampus).
             The photo paradigm provides a way of investigating the
             functional neuroanatomy of real-life episodic memory under
             rigorous experimental control.},
   Doi = {10.1162/0898929042568578},
   Key = {fds253704}
}

@article{fds253763,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Siegler, IC},
   Title = {Facets of personality and the phenomenology of
             autobiographical memory},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {913-930},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10114 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The relationship between individual differences in
             autobiographical memory and personality was examined by
             having 118 undergraduates complete the NEO Personality
             Inventory after rating 15 word-cued autobiographical
             memories on 20 scales. The Openness to Feelings facet (O3)
             correlated with measures of belief in the accuracy of
             memories, recollection, sensory imagery and emotion. Four
             other facets had correlations with belief (A3 - Altruism, E1
             - Warmth, E4 - Activity, E6 - Positive Emotions). These
             facets also deal with emotional components of personality.
             In multiple regressions, measures of belief and measures of
             recollection were predicted by different variables, and for
             measures of belief, the O3 facet increased the variance
             accounted for beyond that of just the cognitive variables.
             Our results are consistent with and extend studies of the
             effects of depression and emotional suppression on
             autobiographical memory. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley &
             Sons, Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1038},
   Key = {fds253763}
}

@article{fds253756,
   Author = {Talarico, JM and LaBar, KS and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Emotional intensity predicts autobiographical memory
             experience.},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1118-1132},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15813494},
   Abstract = {College students generated autobiographical memories from
             distinct emotional categories that varied in valence
             (positive vs. negative) and intensity (high vs. low). They
             then rated various perceptual, cognitive, and emotional
             properties for each memory. The distribution of these
             emotional memories favored a vector model over a circumplex
             model. For memories of all specific emotions, intensity
             accounted for significantly more variance in
             autobiographical memory characteristics than did valence or
             age of the memory. In two additional experiments, we
             examined multiple memories of emotions of high intensity and
             positive or negative valence and of positive valence and
             high or low intensity. Intensity was a more consistent
             predictor of autobiographical memory properties than was
             valence or the age of the memory in these experiments as
             well. The general effects of emotion on autobiographical
             memory properties are due primarily to intensity differences
             in emotional experience, not to benefits or detriments
             associated with a specific valence.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196886},
   Key = {fds253756}
}

@article{fds253761,
   Author = {Skotko, BG and Kensinger, EA and Locascio, JJ and Einstein, G and Rubin,
             DC and Tupler, LA and Krendl, A and Corkin, S},
   Title = {Puzzling thoughts for H. M.: can new semantic information be
             anchored to old semantic memories?},
   Journal = {Neuropsychology},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {756-769},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0894-4105},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15506844},
   Abstract = {Researchers currently debate whether new semantic knowledge
             can be learned and retrieved despite extensive damage to
             medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures. The authors explored
             whether H. M., a patient with amnesia, could acquire new
             semantic information in the context of his lifelong hobby of
             solving crossword puzzles. First, H. M. was tested on a
             series of word-skills tests believed important in solving
             crosswords. He also completed 3 new crosswords: 1 puzzle
             testing pre-1953 knowledge, another testing post-1953
             knowledge, and another combining the 2 by giving
             postoperative semantic clues for preoperative answers. From
             the results, the authors concluded that H. M. can acquire
             new semantic knowledge, at least temporarily, when he can
             anchor it to mental representations established
             preoperatively.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0894-4105.18.4.756},
   Key = {fds253761}
}

@article{fds253765,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical
             memory.},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {427-442},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10111 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Three classes of evidence demonstrate the existence of life
             scripts, or culturally shared representations of the timing
             of major transitional life events. First, a reanalysis of
             earlier studies on age norms shows an increase in the number
             of transitional events between the ages of 15 and 30 years,
             and these events are associated with narrower age ranges and
             more positive emotion than events outside this period.
             Second, 1,485 Danes estimated how old hypothetical
             centenarians were when they had been happiest, saddest, most
             afraid, most in love, and had their most important and most
             traumatic experiences. Only the number of positive events
             showed an increase between the ages of 15 and 30 years.
             Third, undergraduates generated seven important events that
             were likely to occur in the life of a newborn. Pleasantness
             and whether events were expected to occur between the ages
             of 15 and 30 years predicted how frequently events were
             recorded. Life scripts provide an alternative explanation of
             the reminiscence bump. Emphasis is on culture, not
             individuals.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03195836},
   Key = {fds253765}
}

@article{fds253786,
   Author = {Wenzel, A and Pinna, K and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Autobiographical memories of anxiety-related
             experiences.},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {329-341},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10109 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Ninety-nine undergraduate students retrieved three memories
             associated with each of the five emotional experiences:
             panic, trauma, worry, social anxiety, and feeling content.
             Subsequently, they answered 24 questions assessing
             properties of each memory, including the vividness and
             perceived accuracy of the memories and sensory, emotional,
             and anxiety-related experiences during retrieval. Memories
             were coded for affective tone and specificity. Results
             indicated that panic-related and trauma-related memories
             were rated similarly as content memories, but that they
             generally were associated with more imagery and emotional
             experiencing than worry-related or social anxiety-related
             memories. Participants experienced panic and worry symptoms
             to the greatest degree when they retrieved panic-related and
             trauma-related memories. All anxiety-related memories were
             characterized by more negative tone than content memories.
             Panic-related and trauma-related memories were more specific
             than worry-related, social anxiety-related, and content
             memories. These findings can explain partially why
             individuals with some, but not all, anxiety disorders
             experience enhanced memory for threatening
             material.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00142-6},
   Key = {fds253786}
}

@article{fds253844,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Feldman, ME and Beckham, JC},
   Title = {Reliving, emotions, and fragmentation in the
             autobiographical memories of veterans diagnosed with
             PTSD},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {17-35},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10123 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Fifty veterans diagnosed with posttraumatic stress disorder
             (PTSD) each recalled four autobiographical memories: one
             from the 2 years before service, one non-combat memory from
             the time in service, one from combat, and one from service
             that had often come as an intrusive memory. For each memory,
             they provided 21 ratings about reliving, belief, sensory
             properties, reexperiencing emotions, visceral emotional
             responses, fragmentation, and narrative coherence. We used
             these ratings to examine three claims about traumatic
             memories: a separation of cognitive and visceral aspects of
             emotion, an increased sense of reliving, and increased
             fragmentation. There was evidence for a partial separation
             of cognitive judgments of reexperiencing an emotion and
             reports of visceral symptoms of the emotion, with visceral
             symptoms correlating more consistently with scores on PTSD
             tests. Reliving, but not fragmentation of the memories,
             increased with increases in the trauma relatedness of the
             event and with increases in scores on standardized tests of
             PTSD severity. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.950},
   Key = {fds253844}
}

@article{fds253762,
   Author = {Schrauf, RW and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The ‘language’ and ‘feel’ of bilingual memory:
             Mnemonic traces},
   Volume = {5},
   Pages = {21-39},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds253762}
}

@article{fds253785,
   Author = {Greenberg, DL and Rubin, DC and Schrauf, RW},
   Title = {Stability in autobiographical memories},
   Volume = {12},
   Pages = {712-721},
   Year = {2004},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10115 Duke open
             access},
   Key = {fds253785}
}

@article{fds253755,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Burt, CDB and Fifield, SJ},
   Title = {Experimental manipulations of the phenomenology of
             memory.},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {877-886},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14651296},
   Abstract = {We investigated the effects of visual input at encoding and
             retrieval on the phenomenology of memory. In Experiment 1,
             participants took part in events with and without wearing
             blindfolds, and later were shown a video of the events.
             Blindfolding, as well as later viewing of the video, both
             tended to decrease recollection. In Experiment 2,
             participants were played videos, with and without the visual
             component, of events involving other people. Events listened
             to without visual input were recalled with less
             recollection; later adding of the visual component increased
             recollection. In Experiment 3, participants were provided
             with progressively more information about events that they
             had experienced, either in the form of photographs that they
             had taken of the events or narrative descriptions of those
             photographs. In comparison with manipulations at encoding,
             the addition of more visual or narrative cues at recall had
             similar but smaller effects on recollection.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196442},
   Key = {fds253755}
}

@article{fds253787,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schrauf, RW and Greenberg, DL},
   Title = {Belief and recollection of autobiographical
             memories.},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {887-901},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14651297},
   Abstract = {In three experiments, undergraduates rated autobiographical
             memories on scales derived from existing theories of memory.
             In multiple regression analyses, ratings of the degree to
             which subjects recollected (i.e., relived) their memories
             were predicted by visual imagery, auditory imagery, and
             emotions, whereas ratings of belief in the accuracy of their
             memories were predicted by knowledge of the setting.
             Recollection was predicted equally well in between- and
             within-subjects analyses, but belief consistently had
             smaller correlations and multiple regression predictions
             between subjects; individual differences in the cognitive
             scales that we measured could not account well for
             individual differences in belief. In contrast, measures of
             mood (Beck Depression Index) and dissociation (Dissociative
             Experience Scale) added predictive value for belief, but not
             for recollection. We also found that highly relived memories
             almost always had strong visual images and that
             remember/know judgments made on autobiographical memories
             were more closely related to belief than to
             recollection.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196443},
   Key = {fds253787}
}

@article{fds253832,
   Author = {Greenberg, DL and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The neuropsychology of autobiographical memory.},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {4-5},
   Pages = {687-728},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0010-9452},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14584549},
   Abstract = {This special issue of Cortex focuses on the relative
             contribution of different neural networks to memory and the
             interaction of 'core' memory processes with other cognitive
             processes. In this article, we examine both. Specifically,
             we identify cognitive processes other than encoding and
             retrieval that are thought to be involved in memory; we then
             examine the consequences of damage to brain regions that
             support these processes. This approach forces a
             consideration of the roles of brain regions outside of the
             frontal, medial-temporal, and diencephalic regions that form
             a central part of neurobiological theories of memory.
             Certain kinds of damage to visual cortex or lateral temporal
             cortex produced impairments of visual imagery or semantic
             memory; these patterns of impairment are associated with a
             unique pattern of amnesia that was distinctly different from
             the pattern associated with medial-temporal trauma. On the
             other hand, damage to language regions, auditory cortex, or
             parietal cortex produced impairments of language, auditory
             imagery, or spatial imagery; however, these impairments were
             not associated with amnesia. Therefore, a full model of
             autobiographical memory must consider cognitive processes
             that are not generally considered 'core processes,' as well
             as the brain regions upon which these processes
             depend.},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70860-8},
   Key = {fds253832}
}

@article{fds253841,
   Author = {Talarico, JM and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb
             memories.},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {455-461},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0956-7976},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12930476},
   Abstract = {On September 12, 2001, 54 Duke students recorded their
             memory of first hearing about the terrorist attacks of
             September 11 and of a recent everyday event. They were
             tested again either 1, 6, or 32 weeks later. Consistency for
             the flashbulb and everyday memories did not differ, in both
             cases declining over time. However, ratings of vividness,
             recollection, and belief in the accuracy of memory declined
             only for everyday memories. Initial visceral emotion ratings
             correlated with later belief in accuracy, but not
             consistency, for flashbulb memories. Initial visceral
             emotion ratings predicted later posttraumatic stress
             disorder symptoms. Flashbulb memories are not special in
             their accuracy, as previously claimed, but only in their
             perceived accuracy.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-9280.02453},
   Key = {fds253841}
}

@article{fds304737,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Willert, M and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Splintered memories or vivid landmarks? Qualities and
             organization of traumatic memories with and without
             PTSD},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {675-693},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10124 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {One hundred and eighty-one students answered a standardized
             questionnaire on Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD): 25
             reported trauma(s) and indicated a pattern of after-effects
             that matched a PTSD symptom profile, whereas 88 indicated
             trauma(s) but no PTSD symptom profile. Both groups answered
             a questionnaire addressing the recollective quality,
             integration and coherence of the traumatic memory that
             currently affected them most. Participants with a PTSD
             symptom profile reported more vivid recollection of emotion
             and sensory impressions. They reported more observer
             perspective in the memory (seeing themselves 'from the
             outside'), but no more fragmentation. They also agreed more
             with the statement that the trauma had become part of their
             identity, and perceived more thematic connections between
             the trauma and current events in their lives. The two groups
             showed different patterns of correlations which indicated
             different coping styles. Overall, the findings suggest that
             traumas form dysfunctional reference points for the
             organization of other personal memories in people with PTSD
             symptoms, leading to fluctuations between vivid intrusions
             and avoidance. Copyright © 2003 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.894},
   Key = {fds304737}
}

@misc{fds347788,
   Author = {Rubin, D and Greenberg, DL},
   Title = {The role of narrative in recollection: A view from cognitive
             and neuropsychology.},
   Booktitle = {Narrative and Consciousness: Literature, Psychology, and the
             Brain},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press.},
   Editor = {G. Fireman and T. McVay and O. Flanagan},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {March},
   Key = {fds347788}
}

@misc{fds253660,
   Author = {Schrauf, RW and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {On the bilingual's two sets of memories},
   Pages = {121-145},
   Booktitle = {Autobiographical Memory and the Construction of a Narrative
             Self: Developmental and Cultural Perspectives},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press},
   Editor = {R. Fivush and C. Haden},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {141060747X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781410607478},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781410607478},
   Key = {fds253660}
}

@article{fds253842,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Berntsen, D},
   Title = {Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of
             highly positive, but not highly negative,
             events.},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-14},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12699138},
   Abstract = {A representative sample of 1,307 respondents between the
             ages of 20 and 94 was asked how old they were when they felt
             most afraid, most proud, most jealous, most in love, and
             most angry. They were also asked when they had experienced
             their most important event and whether this event was
             positive or negative. In general, there was a reminiscence
             "bump" for positive but not negative events. To provide data
             on life scripts, 87 psychology students answered the same
             questions for a hypothetical 70-year-old. The undergraduates
             were more confident in dating positive than in dating
             negative events, and when they were confident, the
             distribution of responses predicted the survey data. The
             results support the idea of culturally shared life scripts
             for positive but not negative events, which structure
             retrieval processes and spaced practice.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196077},
   Key = {fds253842}
}

@article{fds253843,
   Author = {Fromholt, P and Mortensen, DB and Torpdahl, P and Bender, L and Larsen,
             P and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Life-narrative and word-cued autobiographical memories in
             centenarians: comparisons with 80-year-old control,
             depressed, and dementia groups.},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {81-88},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10121 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Centenarians provided autobiographical memories to either a
             request for a life narrative or a request to produce
             autobiographical memories to cue words. Both methods
             produced distributions with childhood-amnesia,
             reminiscence-bump, and recency components. The
             life-narrative method produced relatively more bump memories
             at the expense of recent memories. The life-narrative
             distributions were similar to those obtained from
             80-year-old adults without clinical symptoms and from
             80-year-old Alzheimer's dementia and depression patients,
             except that the centenarians had an additional 20-year
             period of relatively low recall between the bump and recency
             components. The centenarians produced more emotionally
             neutral memories than the other three groups and produced
             fewer and less detailed memories than the non-clinical
             80-year-old sample.},
   Doi = {10.1080/741938171},
   Key = {fds253843}
}

@misc{fds26253,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Autobiographical Memory},
   Series = {Volume 1},
   Pages = {286-289},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science},
   Publisher = {London: Nature Publishing Group},
   Editor = {L. Nadel},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds26253}
}

@article{fds26388,
   Author = {Greenberg, D.L. and Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {The neuropsychology of autobiographical memory},
   Journal = {Cortex},
   Volume = {39},
   Pages = {687-728},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds26388}
}

@article{fds253793,
   Author = {SCHULKIND, MD and POSNER, RJ and RUBIN, DC},
   Title = {Musical features that facilitate melody identification: How
             do you know it's "your" song when they finally play
             it?},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {217-249},
   Publisher = {University of California Press},
   Year = {2003},
   ISSN = {0730-7829},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000186992000002&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Doi = {10.1525/mp.2003.21.2.217},
   Key = {fds253793}
}

@article{fds253744,
   Author = {Berntsen, D and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the
             life span: the recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and
             involuntary memories.},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {636-652},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.636},
   Abstract = {A sample of 1,241 respondents between 20 and 93 years old
             were asked their age in their happiest, saddest, most
             traumatic, most important memory, and most recent
             involuntary memory. For older respondents, there was a clear
             bump in the 20s for the most important and happiest
             memories. In contrast, saddest and most traumatic memories
             showed a monotonically decreasing retention function. Happy
             involuntary memories were over twice as common as unhappy
             ones, and only happy involuntary memories showed a bump in
             the 20s. Life scripts favoring positive events in young
             adulthood can account for the findings. Standard accounts of
             the bump need to be modified, for example, by repression or
             reduced rehearsal of negative events due to life change or
             social censure.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.636},
   Key = {fds253744}
}

@article{fds253823,
   Author = {Due, DL and Huettel, SA and Hall, WG and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Activation in mesolimbic and visuospatial neural circuits
             elicited by smoking cues: evidence from functional magnetic
             resonance imaging.},
   Journal = {Am J Psychiatry},
   Volume = {159},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {954-960},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0002-953X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12042183},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: The authors sought to increase understanding of
             the brain mechanisms involved in cigarette addiction by
             identifying neural substrates modulated by visual smoking
             cues in nicotine-deprived smokers. METHOD: Event-related
             functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to
             detect brain activation after exposure to smoking-related
             images in a group of nicotine-deprived smokers and a
             nonsmoking comparison group. Subjects viewed a pseudo-random
             sequence of smoking images, neutral nonsmoking images, and
             rare targets (photographs of animals). Subjects pressed a
             button whenever a rare target appeared. RESULTS: In smokers,
             the fMRI signal was greater after exposure to
             smoking-related images than after exposure to neutral images
             in mesolimbic dopamine reward circuits known to be activated
             by addictive drugs (right posterior amygdala, posterior
             hippocampus, ventral tegmental area, and medial thalamus) as
             well as in areas related to visuospatial attention
             (bilateral prefrontal and parietal cortex and right fusiform
             gyrus). In nonsmokers, no significant differences in fMRI
             signal following exposure to smoking-related and neutral
             images were detected. In most regions studied, both subject
             groups showed greater activation following presentation of
             rare target images than after exposure to neutral images.
             CONCLUSIONS: In nicotine-deprived smokers, both reward and
             attention circuits were activated by exposure to
             smoking-related images. Smoking cues are processed like rare
             targets in that they activate attentional regions. These
             cues are also processed like addictive drugs in that they
             activate mesolimbic reward regions.},
   Doi = {10.1176/appi.ajp.159.6.954},
   Key = {fds253823}
}

@article{fds253838,
   Author = {Zervakis, J and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Production and recognition bias of stylistic sentences using
             a story reading task.},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {107-130},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0090-6905},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12022791},
   Abstract = {Four experiments examined participants' ability to produce
             surface characteristics of sentences using an on-line story
             reading task. Participants read a series of stories in which
             either all, or the majority of sentences were written in the
             same "style," or surface form. Twice per story, participants
             were asked to fill in a blank consistent with the story. For
             sentences that contained three stylistic regularities,
             participants imitated either all three characteristics
             (Experiment 2) or two of the three characteristics
             (Experiment 1), depending on the proportion of in-style
             sentences. Participants demonstrated a recognition bias for
             the read style in an unannounced recognition task. When
             participants read stories in which the two styles were the
             dative/double object alternation, participants demonstrated
             a syntactic priming effect in the cloze task, but no
             consistent recognition bias in a later recognition test
             (Experiments 3 and 4).},
   Doi = {10.1023/a:1014922700023},
   Key = {fds253838}
}

@article{fds253840,
   Author = {Larsen, SF and Schrauf, RW and Fromholt, P and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {Inner speech and bilingual autobiographical memory: a
             Polish-Danish cross-cultural study.},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {45-54},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10132 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Thirty years after fleeing from Poland to Denmark, 20
             immigrants were enlisted in a study of bilingual
             autobiographical memory. Ten "early immigrators" averaged 24
             years old at the time of immigration, and ten "late
             immigrators" averaged 34 years old at immigration. Although
             all 20 had spent 30 years in Denmark, early immigrators
             reported more current inner speech behaviours in Danish,
             whereas late immigrators showed more use of Polish. Both
             groups displayed proportionally more numerous
             autobiographical retrievals that were reported as coming to
             them internally in Polish (vs Danish) for the decades prior
             to immigration and more in Danish (vs Polish) after
             immigration. We propose a culture- and language-specific
             shaping of semantic and conceptual stores that underpins
             autobiographical and world knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210143000218},
   Key = {fds253840}
}

@misc{fds26724,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Autobiographical Memory Across the Lifespan},
   Pages = {159-184},
   Booktitle = {Lifespan Development of Human Memory},
   Publisher = {Cambridge, MA: MIT Press},
   Editor = {P. Graf and N. Ohta},
   Year = {2002},
   Key = {fds26724}
}

@misc{fds302147,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory across the lifespan},
   Pages = {159-184},
   Booktitle = {Lifespan development of human memory},
   Publisher = {MIT Press},
   Editor = {Graf, P and Ohta, N},
   Year = {2002},
   Key = {fds302147}
}

@article{fds253837,
   Author = {Schrauf, RW and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Effects of Voluntary Immigration on the Distribution of
             Autobiographical Memory over the Lifespan},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {S75-S88},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10135 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Immigration may be considered a 'traumatic' event with acute
             phases followed by long latency effects. Ten older, Hispanic
             adults who immigrated to the USA at ages 20-22, 24-28, and
             34-35 narrated their 'life-stories' on two occasions, once
             in English and once in Spanish. Instead of the usual
             reminiscence bump they showed an increase in
             autobiographical recalls corresponding specifically to their
             ages at immigration. Each of the narrated life stories was
             independently coded for amount of detail, emotional valence,
             status as transitional event, and backward/forward search
             strategy. Memories for the time of immigration did not
             differ from other memories on any of these ratings.
             Increased recall for the period of immigration may be due to
             the encoding of novel events and the 'effort after meaning'
             required to integrate these events followed by a relatively
             stable period (settlement) marked by release from proactive
             interference and spaced rehearsal. Copyright © 2001 John
             Wiley & Sons, Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.835},
   Key = {fds253837}
}

@article{fds253839,
   Author = {Gulgoz, S and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Kisisel Anilarin Hartirlanmasi: Bir Betimleme Calismasi
             [Retrieval of personal memory: A descriptive
             study]},
   Volume = {16},
   Pages = {37-55},
   Publisher = {TURKISH PSYCHOLOGISTS ASSOC},
   Year = {2001},
   Key = {fds253839}
}

@article{fds337163,
   Author = {Sheen, M and Kemp, S and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Twins Dispute Memory Ownership: A New False Memory
             Phenomenon},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {779-788},
   Publisher = {Allyn & Bacon},
   Year = {2001},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03196407},
   Abstract = {In three experiments, we examined a new memory phenomenon:
             disputed memories, in which people dispute ownership of a
             memory. For example, in one disputed memory each of two
             twins recollected being sent home from school for wearing
             too short a skirt, although only one of them was actually
             sent home. In Experiment 1, 20 sets of same-sex adult twins
             were asked to produce a memory for each of 45 words, and
             most twins spontaneously produced at least one disputed
             memory. In Experiment 2,20 different sets of same-sex adult
             twins rated disputed memories as higher in recollective
             experience, imagery, and emotional reliving than nondisputed
             memories. In Experiment 3, siblings who were close in age as
             well as same-sex friends were also found to have disputed
             memories, but less often than twins.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196407},
   Key = {fds337163}
}

@article{fds253834,
   Author = {Coyle, S and Arnold, HM and Goldberg-Arnold, JS and Rubin, DC and Hall,
             WG},
   Title = {Olfactory conditioning facilitates diet transition in human
             infants.},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {144-152},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0012-1630},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11044862},
   Abstract = {We evaluated whether Pavlovian conditioning methods could be
             used to increase the ingestion of non-preferred solutions by
             formula-fed human infants. In baseline measures, 5-7 month
             old infants sucked less frequently and consumed less water
             than regular formula. During a 3-day olfactory conditioning
             period, parents placed a small scented disk, the conditioned
             stimulus, on the rim of their infants' formula bottle at
             every feeding. Following this training, infants' responses
             to water were tested when their water bottles had a disk
             scented with the training odor, a novel odor, or no odor.
             Infants tested with the training odor sucked more frequently
             and consumed significantly more water than they had at
             baseline. Infants tested with no odor or a novel odor
             consumed water at or below baseline levels. These data
             demonstrate that olfactory conditioning can be used to
             enhance ingestion in infants and suggest that such methods
             may be useful for infants experiencing difficulty when
             making transitions from one diet to another.},
   Doi = {10.1002/1098-2302(200011)37:3<144::aid-dev3>3.0.co;2-z},
   Key = {fds253834}
}

@article{fds253791,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The distribution of early childhood memories.},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {265-269},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10932795},
   Abstract = {The quantitative distribution of autobiographical memories
             for the first decade of life is described. The distribution,
             based on over 11,000 autobiographical memories from age 10
             and younger from published studies, is nearly identical for
             males and females, for participants of different ages, and
             for different methods of collecting data, including using
             words to cue memories from anywhere in the lifespan or from
             just the childhood years, exhaustive listing of all early
             memories, and interviews.},
   Doi = {10.1080/096582100406810},
   Key = {fds253791}
}

@article{fds253835,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schrauf, RW},
   Title = {Internal languages of retrieval: the bilingual encoding of
             memories for the personal past.},
   Volume = {28},
   Pages = {616-623},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10134 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {In contrast to most research on bilingual memory that
             focuses on how words in either lexicon are mapped onto
             memory for objects and concepts, we focus on memory for
             events in the personal past. Using a word-cue technique in
             sessions devoted exclusively to one language, we found that
             older Hispanic immigrants who had come to the United States
             as adults internally retrieved autobiographical memories in
             Spanish for events in the country of origin and in English
             for events in the U.S. These participants were consistently
             capable of discerning whether a memory had come to them "in
             words" or not, reflecting the distinction between purely
             imagistic or conceptual memories and specifically linguistic
             memories. Via examination of other phenomenological features
             of these memories (sense of re-living, sensory detail,
             emotionality, and rehearsal), we conclude that the
             linguistic/nonlinguistic distinction is fundamental and
             independent of these other characteristics. Bilinguals
             encode and retrieve certain autobiographical memories in one
             or the other language according to the context of encoding,
             and these linguistic characteristics are stable properties
             of those memories over time.},
   Key = {fds253835}
}

@misc{fds302148,
   Author = {Rubin, D},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory and aging},
   Pages = {131-149},
   Booktitle = {Cognitive aging: A primer},
   Publisher = {Philadelphia: Psychology Press},
   Editor = {Park, D and Schwartz, N},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {January},
   Key = {fds302148}
}

@article{fds26486,
   Author = {Coyle, S. and Arnold, H.M. and Goldberg-Arnold, J.S. and Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Olfactory conditioning facilities diet transition in human
             infants},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychobiology},
   Volume = {37},
   Pages = {144-152},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds26486}
}

@misc{fds26487,
   Author = {Hall, W.G. and Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Flavor dot and odorizer method},
   Journal = {United States Patent, Number 6,112,749},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds26487}
}

@article{fds253788,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schulkind, MD and Rahhal, TA},
   Title = {A Study of Gender Differences in Autobiographical Memory:
             Broken Down by Age and Sex},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {61-71},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {1068-0667},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10141 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Data from 40 older adults who produced autobiographical
             memories to word cues and to the request to list five
             important memories, and data from 60 older adults who
             answered factual multiple-choice questions for events spread
             across their lives, were analyzed for gender differences. In
             spite of considerable statistical power, there were no
             gender differences in the distribution of autobiographical
             memories over the lifespan, in the distribution of important
             memories, in various ratings provided to these memories, or
             in the distribution of knowledge for events. The only gender
             difference found was that men performed better on factual
             questions about current events and baseball. Thus, counter
             to what might be expected from Darwinian theory and some
             behavioral data, gender differences were
             minimal.},
   Doi = {10.1023/A:1021676309064},
   Key = {fds253788}
}

@article{fds253790,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Frontal-striatal circuits in cognitive aging: Evidence for
             caudate involvement},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {241-259},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10142 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Changes in cognition with aging have been claimed to be due
             in large part to a decline in frontal lobe function.
             However, at our present state of knowledge, the emphasis on
             the frontal lobes to the exclusion of the rest of the
             frontal-striatal circuits of which they are a part is
             unwarranted. To argue this point, I consider another
             anatomical candidate within these circuits, the caudate.
             Evidence is presented that the caudate decreases in size
             with age as much as the frontal lobes and that damage to
             either the frontal lobes or the caudate is accompanied by
             declines in inhibitory processes, executive control, and
             cognitive speed similar to those seen in normal aging.
             Separating the unique contributions of the frontal lobes and
             the caudate to these circuits is difficult but should be the
             focus of future studies of the biological basis of cognitive
             aging.},
   Doi = {10.1076/1382-5585(199912)06:04;1-B;FT241},
   Key = {fds253790}
}

@article{fds253822,
   Author = {Schulkind, MD and Hennis, LK and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Music, emotion, and autobiographical memory: they're playing
             your song.},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {948-955},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10143 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Very long-term memory for popular music was investigated.
             Older and younger adults listened to 20-sec excerpts of
             popular songs drawn from across the 20th century. The
             subjects gave emotionality and preference ratings and tried
             to name the title, artist, and year of popularity for each
             excerpt. They also performed a cued memory test for the
             lyrics. The older adults' emotionality ratings were highest
             for songs from their youth; they remembered more about these
             songs, as well. However, the stimuli failed to cue many
             autobiographical memories of specific events. Further
             analyses revealed that the older adults were less likely
             than the younger adults to retrieve multiple attributes of a
             song together (i.e., title and artist) and that there was a
             significant positive correlation between emotion and memory,
             especially for the older adults. These results have
             implications for research on long-term memory, as well as on
             the relationship between emotion and memory.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03201225},
   Key = {fds253822}
}

@article{fds253789,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Hinton, S and Wenzel, A},
   Title = {The Precise Time Course of Retention},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1161-1176},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0278-7393},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10146 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Fits of retention data were examined from 5 conditions: 3
             types of cued recall, an old-new recognition task, and a
             remember-know recognition task. In each condition, 100
             participants had either 18 recall or 27 recognition trials
             at each of 10 delays between 0 and 99 intervening items,
             providing the first data obtained in experimental psychology
             that were precise enough to distinguish clearly among simple
             functions. None of the 105 2-parameter functions tested
             produced adequate fits to the data. The function y = a 1e
             -t/1.15 + a 2e -t/T2 + a 3 fit each of the 5 retention
             conditions. The T 2 parameter in this equation equaled 28
             for the 3 recall conditions and the remember-know
             recognition condition and 13 for the old-new recognition
             condition. Individuals' recall data fit the same function
             with parameters varying with gender and scholastic aptitude
             scores. Reaction times support the claim that the a 1e
             -t/1.15 term describes working memory, and the remaining 2
             terms describe long-term memory.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0278-7393.25.5.1161},
   Key = {fds253789}
}

@article{fds253833,
   Author = {Watson, ME and Welsh-Bohmer, KA and Hoffman, JM and Lowe, V and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {The neural basis of naming impairments in Alzheimer's
             disease revealed through positron emission
             tomography.},
   Journal = {Arch Clin Neuropsychol},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {347-357},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0887-6177},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14590589},
   Abstract = {The naming impairments in Alzheimer's disease (AD) have been
             attributed to a variety of cognitive processing deficits,
             including impairments in semantic memory, visual perception,
             and lexical access. To further understand the underlying
             biological basis of the naming failures in AD, the present
             investigation examined the relationship of various classes
             of naming errors to regional brain measures of cerebral
             glucose metabolism as measured with 18 F-Fluoro-2-deoxyglucose
             (FDG) and positron emission tomography (PET). Errors
             committed on a visual naming test were categorized according
             to a cognitive processing schema and then examined in
             relationship to metabolism within specific brain regions.
             The results revealed an association of semantic errors with
             glucose metabolism in the frontal and temporal regions.
             Language access errors, such as circumlocutions, and word
             blocking nonresponses were associated with decreased
             metabolism in areas within the left hemisphere.
             Visuoperceptive errors were related to right inferior
             parietal metabolic function. The findings suggest that
             specific brain areas mediate the perceptual, semantic, and
             lexical processing demands of visual naming and that visual
             naming problems in dementia are related to dysfunction in
             specific neural circuits.},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0887-6177(98)00027-4},
   Key = {fds253833}
}

@misc{fds302149,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory and aging: Distributions of memories
             across the life-span and their implications for survey
             research},
   Pages = {163-183},
   Booktitle = {Cognition, aging, and self-reports},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press},
   Editor = {Schwartz, N and Park, DC and Knauper, B and Sudman,
             S},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds302149}
}

@article{fds253797,
   Author = {Schrauf, RW and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Bilingual Autobiographical Memory in Older Adult Immigrants:
             A Test of Cognitive Explanations of the Reminiscence Bump
             and the Linguistic Encoding of Memories},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {437-457},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10147 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Twelve people who emigrated as adults from Spanish-speaking
             cultures and then spent at least 30 years in an Anglo
             culture were asked to provide autobiographical memories to
             word cues. All communication was in Spanish on one day and
             English on a second. In previous studies, there has been a
             bump or increase in autobiographical memories for the 10 to
             30 decades. Here the increase in memories followed the age
             of immigration and settlement, supporting a cognitive theory
             of the reminiscence bump. The distributions of memories
             across the lifespan were similar for the Spanish sessions
             and the English sessions. Participants identified 20% of
             their memories as recalled internally in the language not
             being used that day. For this subset of memories, events
             prior to migration were more frequently recalled in Spanish,
             whereas events after migration were more frequently recalled
             in English. © 1998 Academic Press.},
   Doi = {10.1006/jmla.1998.2585},
   Key = {fds253797}
}

@article{fds253768,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Knowledge and judgments about events that occurred prior to
             birth: The measurement of the persistence of
             information},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {397-400},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10145 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Data from five laboratories using five different techniques
             were reanalyzed to measure subjects' knowledge of events
             that occurred over the past 70 years. Subjects were about 20
             years of age, so the measures included events that extended
             up to 50 years before birth. The functions relating
             knowledge about the events to age do not decrease
             precipitously at birth but gradually drop to above-chance
             levels. Techniques usually used to study retention within
             the individual can be used to study the persistence of ideas
             and fashions within an age cohort in a culture.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03208816},
   Key = {fds253768}
}

@article{fds253796,
   Author = {Zervakis, J and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Memory and learning for a novel written style.},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {754-767},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9701967},
   Abstract = {Subjects read and recalled a series of five short stories in
             one of four plot and style combinations. The stories were
             written in one of two styles that consisted of opposing
             clause orders (i.e., independent-dependent vs.
             dependent-independent), tense forms (i.e., past vs.
             present), and descriptor forms (modifier modifier vs.
             modifier as a noun). The subjects incorporated both plot and
             style characteristics into their recalls. Other subjects,
             who, after five recalls, either generated a new story or
             listed the rules that had been followed by the stories read,
             included the marked forms of the characteristics they
             learned more often, except for tense. The subjects read and
             recalled four stories of the same plot and style and then
             read and recalled a fifth story of the same plot and style
             or of one of the other three plot/style combinations.
             Ability to switch style depended on both the characteristic
             and the markedness.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03211395},
   Key = {fds253796}
}

@article{fds253799,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Greenberg, DL},
   Title = {Visual memory-deficit amnesia: a distinct amnesic
             presentation and etiology.},
   Volume = {95},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {5413-5416},
   Publisher = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9560290},
   Abstract = {We describe a form of amnesia, which we have called visual
             memory-deficit amnesia, that is caused by damage to areas of
             the visual system that store visual information. Because it
             is caused by a deficit in access to stored visual material
             and not by an impaired ability to encode or retrieve new
             material, it has the otherwise infrequent properties of a
             more severe retrograde than anterograde amnesia with no
             temporal gradient in the retrograde amnesia. Of the 11 cases
             of long-term visual memory loss found in the literature, all
             had amnesia extending beyond a loss of visual memory, often
             including a near total loss of pretraumatic episodic memory.
             Of the 6 cases in which both the severity of retrograde and
             anterograde amnesia and the temporal gradient of the
             retrograde amnesia were noted, 4 had a more severe
             retrograde amnesia with no temporal gradient and 2 had a
             less severe retrograde amnesia with a temporal
             gradient.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.95.9.5413},
   Key = {fds253799}
}

@article{fds253798,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Rahhal, TA and Poon, LW},
   Title = {Things learned in early adulthood are remembered
             best.},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {3-19},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9519693},
   Abstract = {Evidence is reviewed that for older adults the period from
             10 to 30 years of age produces recall of the most
             autobiographical memories, the most vivid memories, and the
             most important memories. It is the period from which
             peoples' favorite films, music, and books come and the
             period from which they judge the most important world events
             to have originated. Factual, semantic, general-knowledge,
             multiple-choice questions about the Academy Awards, the
             World Series, and current events from this period were
             answered more accurately by two different groups of 30 older
             adults tested 10 years apart. A cognitive theory based on
             the importance of transitions and several noncognitive
             theories are considered as explanations of this pervasive
             phenomenon.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03211366},
   Key = {fds253798}
}

@article{fds253800,
   Author = {Braun, K and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The spacing effect depends on an encoding deficit,
             retrieval, and time in working memory: evidence from
             once-presented words.},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {37-65},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9640432},
   Abstract = {The spacing effect in list learning occurs because identical
             massed items suffer encoding deficits and because spaced
             items benefit from retrieval and increased time in working
             memory. Requiring the retrieval of identical items produced
             a spacing effect for recall and recognition, both for
             intentional and incidental learning. Not requiring retrieval
             produced spacing only for intentional learning because
             intentional learning encourages retrieval. Once-presented
             words provided baselines for these effects. Next, massed and
             spaced word pairs were judged for matches on their first
             three letters, forcing retrieval. The words were not
             identical, so there was no encoding deficit. Retrieval could
             and did cause spacing only for the first word of each pair;
             time in working memory, only for the second.},
   Doi = {10.1080/741941599},
   Key = {fds253800}
}

@misc{fds300085,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Beginnings of a theory of autobiographical
             remembering},
   Pages = {47-67},
   Booktitle = {Autobiographical memory: Theoretical and applied
             perspectives},
   Publisher = {Erlbaum},
   Editor = {Thompson, CP and Herrmann, DJ and Bruce, D and Read, JD and Payne, DG and Toglia, MP},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {0-8058-2075-2},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000071966300004&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds300085}
}

@article{fds253801,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schulkind, MD},
   Title = {The distribution of autobiographical memories across the
             lifespan.},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {859-866},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9421572},
   Abstract = {Words were used to cue autobiographical memories from 20-
             and 70-year-old subjects. Both groups showed a decrease in
             memories from the childhood years and a power-function
             retention function for their most recent 10 years. Older
             subjects also had an increase in the number of memories from
             the ages 10 to 30. These results held for individual
             subjects as well as grouped data and held when either 124 or
             921 memories were cued. Reaction times to produce memories
             were constant across decades except for childhood where they
             were longer.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03211330},
   Key = {fds253801}
}

@article{fds253803,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schulkind, MD},
   Title = {Distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical
             memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults.},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {524-535},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0882-7974},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9308099},
   Abstract = {For word-cued autobiographical memories, older adults had an
             increase, or bump, from the ages 10 to 30. All age groups
             had fewer memories from childhood than from other years and
             a power-function retention for memories from the most recent
             10 years. There were no consistent differences in reaction
             times and rating scale responses across decades. Concrete
             words cued older memories, but no property of the cues
             predicted which memories would come from the bump. The 5
             most important memories given by 20- and 35-year-old
             participants were distributed similarly to their word-cued
             memories, but those given by 70-year-old participants came
             mostly from the single 20-to-30 decade. No theory fully
             accounts for the bump.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0882-7974.12.3.524},
   Key = {fds253803}
}

@article{fds253804,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Ciobanu, V and Langston, W},
   Title = {Children's memory for counting-out rhymes: A cross-language
             comparison},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {421-424},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10151 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {In English, counting-out rhymes, such as "Eenie Meenie,"
             vary little over retellings. Recall is not rote but is
             sensitive to the structure of the genre. To test the
             generality of this finding, a sample of Romanian rhymes was
             collected. Although there was no overlap with the English
             rhymes, the corpus of rhymes collected had similar structure
             in terms of number of lines, repeating words, rhyme,
             alliteration, and the inclusion of nonsense words. Variation
             within rhymes preserved the poetic structure of the genre.
             The results suggest that verbatim recall can be schema
             driven if there is sufficient structure.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03210804},
   Key = {fds253804}
}

@article{fds253802,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Schulkind, MD},
   Title = {Properties of word cues for autobiographical
             memory.},
   Volume = {81},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {47-50},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0033-2941},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9293192},
   Abstract = {A sample of 124 words were used to cue autobiographical
             memories in 120 adults varying in age from 20 to 73 years.
             Individual words reliably cued autobiographical memories of
             different ages with different speeds. For all age groups,
             words rated high in imagery produced older memories and
             faster reaction times.},
   Doi = {10.2466/pr0.1997.81.1.47},
   Key = {fds253802}
}

@article{fds253806,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Wenzel, AE},
   Title = {One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description
             of retention},
   Volume = {103},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {734-760},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10157 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {A sample of 210 published data sets were assembled that (a)
             plotted amount remembered versus time, (b) had 5 or more
             points, and (c) were smooth enough to fit at least 1 of the
             functions tested with a correlation coefficient of .90 or
             greater. Each was fit to 105 different 2-parameter
             functions. The best fits were to the logarithmic function,
             the power function, the exponential in the square root of
             time, and the hyperbola in the square root of time. It is
             difficult to distinguish among these 4 functions with the
             available data, but the same set of 4 functions fit most
             data sets, with autobiographical memory being the exception.
             Theoretical motivations for the best fitting functions are
             offered. The methodological problems of evaluating functions
             and the advantages of searching existing data for
             regularities before formulating theories are
             considered.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.734},
   Key = {fds253806}
}

@article{fds253805,
   Author = {Watson, ME and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Spatial imagery preserves temporal order.},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {515-534},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8884744},
   Abstract = {Line drawings were presented in either a spatial or a
             nonspatial format. Subjects recalled each of four sets of 24
             items in serial order. Amount recalled in the correct serial
             order and sequencing errors were scored. In Experiment 1
             items appeared either in consecutive locations of a matrix
             or in one central location. Subjects who saw the items in
             different locations made fewer sequencing errors than those
             who saw each item in a central location, but serial recall
             levels for these two conditions did not differ. When items
             appeared in nonconsecutive locations in Experiment 2, the
             advantage of the spatial presentation on sequencing errors
             disappeared. Experiment 3 included conditions in which both
             the consecutive and nonconsecutive spatial formats were
             paired with retrieval cues that either did or did not
             indicate the sequence of locations in which the items had
             appeared. Spatial imagery aided sequencing when, and only
             when, the order of locations in which the stimuli appeared
             could be reconstructed at retrieval.},
   Doi = {10.1080/741940777},
   Key = {fds253805}
}

@book{fds309886,
   Title = {Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical
             Memory},
   Pages = {448 pages},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Rubin, DC},
   Year = {1996},
   Abstract = {This book reviews the latest research in the field of
             autobiographical memory.},
   Key = {fds309886}
}

@misc{fds302150,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Stories about Stories},
   Pages = {153-164},
   Booktitle = {Knowledge and memory: The real story},
   Publisher = {Lawrence Erlbaum Associates},
   Editor = {Wyer Jr. and RS},
   Year = {1995},
   Key = {fds302150}
}

@book{fds302163,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Memory in Oral Traditions: The Cognitive Psychology of Epic,
             Ballads, and Counting-out Rhymes},
   Pages = {385 pages},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
   Year = {1995},
   Abstract = {Dr. Rubin has brought cognitive psychology into a wholly
             unprecedented dialogue with studies in oral
             tradition.},
   Key = {fds302163}
}

@article{fds253807,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Wallace, WT and Houston, BC},
   Title = {The beginnings of expertise for ballads},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {435-462},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0364-0213},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10161 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {To study the beginning stages of expertise, 14 students, who
             were inexperienced with ballads, heard and recalled a series
             of 5 ballads over the course of 5 weeks. Compared with their
             first recall of the first ballad, their first recall of the
             fifth ballad had one and a half times as many words, two
             times as many rhyming words, and three times as much line
             structure evident in the written recall protocols. Compared
             with novices, the 14 beginning experts more often filled in
             blank spaces in novel ballads with words of the correct
             number of syllables and more often chose the original stanza
             of a novel ballad that was paired with a changed version of
             the stanza. The beginning experts were also able to compose,
             in 20 min, ballads about two thirds as long as the 10-stanza
             ballads they learned. Thirty characteristics were identified
             in the set of the five learned ballads. The ballads composed
             by the beginning experts used over half of these. The
             beginning experts also explicitly stated about one quarter
             of these 30 characteristics, but there was no statistical
             relationship between the characteristics used and the
             characteristics stated. Memory expertise is viewed as a
             pervasive aspect of cognition in which people make use of a
             variety of regularities in the material to be learned. ©
             1993 Cognitive Science Society, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15516709cog1703_4},
   Key = {fds253807}
}

@misc{fds302151,
   Author = {Conway, MA and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The structure of autobiographical memory},
   Pages = {103-137},
   Booktitle = {Theories of memory},
   Publisher = {Erlbaum},
   Editor = {Collins, AE and Gathercole, SE and Conway, MA and Morris,
             PE},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds302151}
}

@misc{fds26485,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Oral tradition},
   Pages = {502-503},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of learning and memory},
   Publisher = {New York: MacMillan},
   Editor = {L. Squire},
   Year = {1992},
   Key = {fds26485}
}

@misc{fds302152,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Constraints on memory},
   Pages = {265-273},
   Booktitle = {Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of "flashbulb"
             memorie},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Winoglad, E and Neisser, U},
   Year = {1992},
   Key = {fds302152}
}

@misc{fds302153,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Definitions of autobiographical memory},
   Pages = {495-499},
   Booktitle = {Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical
             memory},
   Publisher = {Kluwer Academic Publishers},
   Editor = {Conway, MA and Rubin, DC and Spinnler, H and Wagenaar,
             WA},
   Year = {1992},
   Key = {fds302153}
}

@book{fds309887,
   Title = {Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical
             memory},
   Publisher = {Kluwer Academic Publishers},
   Editor = {Conway, MA and Rubin, DC and Spinnler, H and Wagenaar,
             WA},
   Year = {1992},
   Key = {fds309887}
}

@article{fds253808,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Wanda, TW},
   Title = {Characteristics and Constraints in Ballads and Their Effects
             on Memory},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {181-202},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1991},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0163-853X},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:A1991HC27300004&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Four sets of ballads, chosen as a sample of an oral
             tradition as it existed in North Carolina in the early
             1900s, were examined in order to determine whether ballad
             characteristics used in combination are sufficient to
             account for the stability observed from performance to
             performance, as well as across generations of oral
             transmission. The characteristics included verse length,
             presence of refrains, presence and location of poetics, the
             pattern and number of end rhymes, the metrical patterns,
             average number of syllables per word, the pattern of meaning
             and imagery in lines, the frequency of repeated lines both
             within and across ballads in the set, the musical scales
             used, and the agreement of metrical stresses and musical
             beats. The combination of these characteristics provides
             many constraints which limit the possible word choices and
             can act to stabilize transmissions. © 1991, Taylor &
             Francis Group, LLC. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1080/01638539109544781},
   Key = {fds253808}
}

@article{fds253809,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Stoltzfus, ER and Wall, KL},
   Title = {The abstraction of form in semantic categories.},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-7},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1991},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2017026},
   Abstract = {Undergraduates were asked to generate a name for a
             hypothetical new exemplar of a category. They produced names
             that had the same numbers of syllables, the same endings,
             and the same types of word stems as existing exemplars of
             that category. In addition, novel exemplars, each consisting
             of a nonsense syllable root and a prototypical ending, were
             accurately assigned to categories. The data demonstrate the
             abstraction and use of surface properties of
             words.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03198491},
   Key = {fds253809}
}

@article{fds253810,
   Author = {Hyman, IE and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Memorabeatlia: a naturalistic study of long-term
             memory.},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {205-214},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1990},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10162 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Seventy-six undergraduates were given the titles and first
             lines of Beatles' songs and asked to recall the songs. Seven
             hundred and four different undergraduates were cued with one
             line from each of 25 Beatles' songs and asked to recall the
             title. The probability of recalling a line was best
             predicted by the number of times a line was repeated in the
             song and how early the line first appeared in the song. The
             probability of cuing to the title was best predicted by
             whether the line shared words with the title. Although the
             subjects recalled only 21% of the lines, there were very few
             errors in recall, and the errors rarely violated the
             rhythmic, poetic, or thematic constraints of the songs.
             Acting together, these constraints can account for the near
             verbatim recall observed. Fourteen subjects, who transcribed
             one song, made fewer and different errors than the subjects
             who had recalled the song, indicating that the errors in
             recall were not primarily the result of errors in
             encoding.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03197096},
   Key = {fds253810}
}

@misc{fds26462,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Directed graphs as memory respresentations: The case of
             rhyme},
   Pages = {121-133},
   Booktitle = {Pathfinder associative networks: Studies in knowledge
             organization},
   Publisher = {Norwood, NJ: Ablex},
   Editor = {R.W. Schvaneveldt},
   Year = {1990},
   Key = {fds26462}
}

@misc{fds302154,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Directed graphs as memory representations: The case of
             rhyme},
   Pages = {121-133},
   Booktitle = {Pathfinder associative networks: Studies in knowledge
             organization},
   Publisher = {Ablex},
   Editor = {Schvaneveldt, RW},
   Year = {1990},
   Key = {fds302154}
}

@article{fds253812,
   Author = {Baddeley, AD and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Telescoping is not time compression: a model of the dating
             of autobiographical events.},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {653-661},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10165 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {A model of telescoping is proposed that assumes no
             systematic errors in dating. Rather, the overestimation of
             recent occurrences of events is based on the combination of
             three factors: (1) Retention is greater for recent events;
             (2) errors in dating, though unbiased, increase linearly
             with the time since the dated event; and (3) intrusions
             often occur from events outside the period being asked
             about, but such intrusions do not come from events that have
             not yet occurred. In Experiment 1, we found that recall for
             colloquia fell markedly over a 2-year interval, the
             magnitude of errors in psychologists' dating of the
             colloquia increased at a rate of .4 days per day of delay,
             and the direction of the dating error was toward the middle
             of the interval. In Experiment 2, the model used the
             retention function and dating errors from the first study to
             predict the distribution of the actual dates of colloquia
             recalled as being within a 5-month period. In Experiment 3,
             the findings of the first study were replicated with
             colloquia given by, instead of for, the subjects.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03202626},
   Key = {fds253812}
}

@article{fds253811,
   Author = {Rubin, D and WALLACE, WT},
   Title = {Rhyme and Reason: Analyses of Dual Retrieval
             Cues},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {698-709},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0278-7393},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10163 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {If and only if each single cue uniquely defines its target,
             a independence model based on fragment theory can predict
             the strength of a combined dual cue from the strengths of
             its single cue components. If the single cues do not each
             uniquely define their target, no single monotonic function
             can predict the strength of the dual cue from its
             components; rather, what matters is the number of possible
             targets. The probability of generating a target word was .19
             for rhyme cues, .14 for category cues, and .97 for
             rhyme-plus-category dual cues. Moreover, some pairs of cues
             had probabilities of producing their targets of .03 when
             used individually and 1.00 when used together, whereas other
             pairs had moderate probabilities individually and together.
             The results, which are interpreted in terms of multiple
             constraints limiting the number of responses, show why
             rhymes, which play a minimal role in laboratory studies of
             memory, are common in real-world mnemonics.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0278-7393.15.4.698},
   Key = {fds253811}
}

@book{fds26391,
   Title = {Everyday cognition in adulthood and later
             life},
   Publisher = {Cambridge: Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Poon, L.W. and Rubin, D.C. and Wilson, B.A.},
   Year = {1989},
   Key = {fds26391}
}

@misc{fds26484,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Memory, autobiographical},
   Pages = {101-102},
   Booktitle = {Neuroscience year: Supplement 1 to the encyclopedia of
             neuroscience},
   Publisher = {Cambridge: Birkhauser Boston Inc.},
   Editor = {G. Adelman},
   Year = {1989},
   Key = {fds26484}
}

@misc{fds302155,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Issues of regularity and control: Confessions of a
             regularity freak},
   Pages = {84-103},
   Booktitle = {Everyday cognition in adult and later life},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Poon, LW and Rubin, DC and Wilson, BA},
   Year = {1989},
   Key = {fds302155}
}

@book{fds309888,
   Title = {Everyday Cognition in Adulthood and Late
             Life},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Poon, L and Rubin, DC and Wilson, BA},
   Year = {1989},
   Abstract = {The authors present relevant data that open up new
             directions for those studying cognitive aging.},
   Key = {fds309888}
}

@article{fds253813,
   Author = {Kelly, MH and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Natural rhythmic patterns in English verse: Evidence from
             child counting-out rhymes},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {718-740},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0749-596X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10167 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Counting-out rhymes are part of an oral tradition whose
             primary participants are children. These facts are used to
             justify the claim that the rhythmic structure of
             counting-out rhymes can be attributed to natural preferences
             more readily than could the structure of formal verse
             considered alone. The first study uncovered a number of
             similarities between formal and counting-out verse. The
             rhythm of counting-out rhymes is constrained by the
             principle of rhythmic alternation, the nuclear stress and
             compound rules, and foot boundaries. In addition, both
             trochaic and iambic meters seem to exist as natural
             categories in counting-out rhymes. Finally, children appear
             to associate different grammatical categories with different
             degrees of stress, in a manner very similar to patterns
             found in adult verse. The second study explored the variants
             of one common counting-out rhyme and reports changes that
             preserve or improve the poetics of the rhyme. The discussion
             offers one definition of what a "natural" rhythmic pattern
             in verse might mean: A pattern that has parallels in the
             rhythmic structure of ordinary speech. This definition makes
             the study of verse attractive to psycholinguists for three
             reasons. First, factors that influence speech rhythm can be
             examined for their relevance to poetic rhythm, a possibility
             that renders tractable the psycholinguistic study of one
             form of language creativity. Second, hypotheses about the
             rhythmic structure of speech can be formulated from
             knowledge of poetic rhythm. Finally, studying the structure
             of child verse could foster understanding of the acquisition
             of prosodic rules, an area that has been relatively
             neglected in developmental psycholinguistics. ©
             1988.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0749-596X(88)90017-4},
   Key = {fds253813}
}

@misc{fds302157,
   Author = {Wallace, WT and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Memory of a ballad singer},
   Volume = {1},
   Pages = {257-262},
   Booktitle = {Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues,
             Vol. 1, Memory in everyday life},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Editor = {Gruenberg, MM and Morris, PE and Sykes, RN},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds302157}
}

@misc{fds302158,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Practical aspects of autobiographical memory},
   Pages = {253-256},
   Booktitle = {Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues:
             Vol. 1. Memory in everyday life},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Editor = {Gruenberg, MM and Morris, PE and Sykes, RN},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds302158}
}

@misc{fds302160,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Go for the skill},
   Pages = {374-382},
   Booktitle = {Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional
             approaches to the study of memory},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Neisser, U and Winograd, E},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds302160}
}

@misc{fds302159,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Learning poetic language},
   Pages = {339-351},
   Booktitle = {The development of language and language researchers: Essays
             in honor of Roger Brown},
   Publisher = {Erlbaum},
   Editor = {Kessel, F},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds302159}
}

@misc{fds302156,
   Author = {Wallace, WT and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The Wreck of the Old 97”: A real event remembered in
             song},
   Pages = {283-310},
   Booktitle = {Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional
             approaches to the study of memory},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Neisser, U and Winograd, E},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds302156}
}

@misc{fds26483,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory},
   Pages = {49-50},
   Booktitle = {The Encyclopedia of aging},
   Publisher = {New York: Springer},
   Editor = {G.L. Maddox},
   Year = {1987},
   Key = {fds26483}
}

@article{fds253814,
   Author = {Schultz, KA},
   Title = {Unit Analysis of Prose Memory in Clinical and Elderly
             Populations},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {77-87},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565648609540331},
   Abstract = {Interpretation of clinical memory tests generally emphasizes
             the quantitative aspects of recall. This study presents an
             additional unit analysis of the Logical Memory subtest of
             Russell's revision of the Wechsler Memory Scale for a
             variety of older adult groups. Patients' neuropsychological
             test data were reviewed, and the paragraphs from the Logical
             Memory subtest were analyzed using unit analysis (Rubin,
             1978). The older adults consisted of a healthy group as well
             as groups whose diagnoses included Alzheimer's and
             multi-infarct dementias, head trauma, and metabolic and
             affective disorders. Quantitative analyses of recall
             revealed group differences. Qualitative analysis of which
             memory units were recalled, however, showed similarities in
             memory processing among these groups. © 1986, Taylor &
             Francis Group, LLC. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1080/87565648609540331},
   Key = {fds253814}
}

@article{fds253815,
   Author = {Friendly, M and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Predicting which words get recalled: measures of free
             recall, availability, goodness, emotionality, and
             pronunciability for 925 nouns.},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {79-94},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10168 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {To investigate the properties that make a word easy to
             recall, we added to existing norms for 925 nouns measures of
             availability, goodness, emotionality, pronunciability, and
             probability of recall in multiple-trial free recall.
             Availability, imagery, and emotionality were found to be the
             best predictors of which words were recalled. This result,
             which is stable across recall data collected in three
             separate laboratories, argues for the importance of
             availability as a predictor of recall and questions the role
             of the correlated variables of word frequency and
             meaningfulness. Consistent with earlier work on a smaller
             sample of words, six factors describe the numerous
             properties of words studied by psychologists. The six
             factors are composed of variables based on orthography,
             imagery and meaning, word frequency, recall, emotionality,
             and goodness. © 1986 Psychonomic Society,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03209231},
   Key = {fds253815}
}

@misc{fds26455,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Wetzler, S.E. and Nebes, R.D.},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory across the adult
             lifespan},
   Pages = {202-221},
   Booktitle = {Autobiographical memory},
   Publisher = {Cambridge: Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {D.C. Rubin},
   Year = {1986},
   Key = {fds26455}
}

@misc{fds302161,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Wetzler, SE and Nebes, RD},
   Title = {Autobiographical memory across the lifespan},
   Pages = {202-221},
   Booktitle = {Autobiographical memory},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Rubin, DC},
   Year = {1986},
   Key = {fds302161}
}

@book{fds309889,
   Title = {Autobiographical Memory},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Rubin, DC},
   Year = {1986},
   Abstract = {Rubin brings together and integrates the best contemporary
             work on the cognitive psychology of memories of the
             self.},
   Key = {fds309889}
}

@article{fds253769,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Memorability as a measure of processing: a unit analysis of
             prose and list learning.},
   Volume = {114},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {213-238},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0096-3445},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10171 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The percentage of subjects recalling each unit in a list or
             prose passage is considered as a dependent measure. When the
             same units are recalled in different tasks, processing is
             assumed to be the same; when different units are recalled,
             processing is assumed to be different. Two collections of
             memory tasks are presented, one for lists and one for prose.
             The relations found in these two collections are supported
             by an extensive reanalysis of the existing prose memory
             literature. The same set of words were learned by 13
             different groups of subjects under 13 different conditions.
             Included were intentional free-recall tasks, incidental free
             recall following lexical decision, and incidental free
             recall following ratings of orthographic distinctiveness and
             emotionality. Although the nine free-recall tasks varied
             widely with regard to the amount of recall, the relative
             probability of recall for the words was very similar among
             the tasks. Imagery encoding and recognition produced
             relative probabilities of recall that were different from
             each other and from the free-recall tasks. Similar results
             were obtained with a prose passage. A story was learned by
             13 different groups of subjects under 13 different
             conditions. Eight free-recall tasks, which varied with
             respect to incidental or intentional learning, retention
             interval, and the age of the subjects, produced similar
             relative probabilities of recall, whereas recognition and
             prompted recall produced relative probabilities of recall
             that were different from each other and from the free-recall
             tasks. A review of the prose literature was undertaken to
             test the generality of these results. Analysis of variance
             is the most common statistical procedure in this literature.
             If the relative probability of recall of units varied across
             conditions, a units by condition interaction would be
             expected. For the 12 studies that manipulated retention
             interval, an average of 21% of the variance was accounted
             for by the main effect of retention interval, 17% by the
             main effect of units, and only 2% by the retention interval
             by units interaction. Similarly, for the 12 studies that
             varied the age of the subjects, 6% of the variance was
             accounted for by the main effect of age, 32% by the main
             effect of units, and only 1% by the interaction of age by
             units.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)},
   Doi = {10.1037//0096-3445.114.2.213},
   Key = {fds253769}
}

@article{fds253766,
   Author = {RUBIN, DC},
   Title = {THE SUBTLE DECEIVER - RECALLING OUR PAST},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {38-&},
   Publisher = {PSYCHOLOGY TODAY},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0033-3107},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:A1985APR8500033&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds253766}
}

@article{fds26389,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {The subtle deceiver: Recalling our past},
   Journal = {Psychology Today},
   Pages = {38-46},
   Year = {1985},
   Key = {fds26389}
}

@article{fds253816,
   Author = {Biermann, AW and Rodman, RD and Rubin, DC and Heidlage,
             JF},
   Title = {NATURAL-LANGUAGE WITH DISCRETE SPEECH AS A MODE FOR
             HUMAN-TO-MACHINE COMMUNICATION},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {628-636},
   Publisher = {Association for Computing Machinery (ACM)},
   Year = {1985},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10170 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {A voice interactive natural language system, which allows
             users to solve problems with spoken English commands, has
             been constructed. The system utilizes a commercially
             available discrete speech recognizer which requires that
             each word be followed by approximately a 300 millisecond
             pause. In a test of the system, subjects were able to learn
             its use after about two hours of training. The system
             correctly processed about 77 percent of the over 6000 input
             sentences spoken in problem-solving sessions. Subjects spoke
             at the rate of about three sentences per minute and were
             able to effectively use the system to complete the given
             tasks. Subjects found the system relatively easy to learn
             and use, and gave a generally positive report of their
             experience. © 1985, ACM. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1145/3812.3817},
   Key = {fds253816}
}

@article{fds253817,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Kozin, M},
   Title = {Vivid memories.},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {81-95},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1984},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0010-0277},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10173 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Fifty-eight undergraduates each recorded their three
             clearest autobiographical memories and answered questions
             about them. The resulting 174 memories were almost all rated
             to be of high personal importance, but low national
             importance. In contrast to published results of flashbulb
             memories cued by events which were specific, nationally
             important, surprising, and consequential, the ratings
             collected here covered the scales of surprise and
             consequentiality in a fairly uniform manner. The subjects
             also answered questions about memories cued by 20 events.
             For each subject, some of these memories were of 'flashbulb'
             clarity and some were not. The clearer memories were more
             surprising, consequential, and emotional, indicating that
             these factors are associated with, though not necessary for,
             vivid memories. © 1984.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0010-0277(84)90037-4},
   Key = {fds253817}
}

@misc{fds26481,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C. and Kimble, G.A.},
   Title = {Instructor's resource manual},
   Series = {6th ed.},
   Booktitle = {Principles of psychology,},
   Publisher = {New York: Wiley},
   Editor = {G.A. Kimble and N. Garmezy and E. Zigler},
   Year = {1984},
   Key = {fds26481}
}

@article{fds253818,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Groth, E and Goldsmith, DJ},
   Title = {Olfactory cuing of autobiographical memory.},
   Volume = {97},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {493-507},
   Publisher = {JSTOR},
   Year = {1984},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10172 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {In Experiment 1, subjects were presented with either the
             odors or the names of 15 common objects. In Experiment 2,
             subjects were presented with either the odors, photographs,
             or names of 16 common objects. All subjects were asked to
             describe an autobiographical memory evoked by each cue, to
             date each memory, and to rate each memory on vividness,
             pleasantness, and the number of times that the memory had
             been thought of and talked about prior to the experiment.
             Compared with memories evoked by photographs or names,
             memories evoked by odors were reported to be thought of and
             talked about less often prior to the experiment and were
             more likely to be reported as never having been thought of
             or talked about prior to the experiment. No other effects
             were consistently found, though there was a suggestion that
             odors might evoke more pleasant and emotional memories than
             other types of cues. The relation of these results to the
             folklore concerning olfactory cuing is discussed.},
   Doi = {10.2307/1422158},
   Key = {fds253818}
}

@article{fds253820,
   Author = {Bacon, EH and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Story recall by mentally retarded children.},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {3 Pt 1},
   Pages = {791-796},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10176 Duke open
             access},
   Doi = {10.2466/pr0.1983.53.3.791},
   Key = {fds253820}
}

@article{fds253819,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Kontis, TC},
   Title = {A schema for common cents.},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {335-341},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10174 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Drawings from memory of a penny, nickel, dime, and quarter
             were obtained from 125 undergraduates. The modal recalls for
             the heads of all four coins were identical, suggesting that
             a schema for coins exists. In an attempt to have the schema
             produced directly, another 75 students designed either a 2-,
             7-, or 20-cent piece that would fit into our present
             currency. The modal drawing for this task was identical to
             the modal recalls. The schema for the heads derived from
             these experiments differs from existing coins and is not an
             average of them. © 1983 Psychonomic Society,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03202446},
   Key = {fds253819}
}

@article{fds253770,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Associative asymmetry, availability, and
             retrieval.},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {83-92},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10175 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Associative frequency, the ease with which a word comes to
             mind in free association, is taken as a measure of general
             response availability. As expected from this view, in both
             controlled experiments and in reanalyses of previously
             published correlational data, high associative frequency
             words were judged to be more familiar and were easier to
             recall but harder to recognize than low associative
             frequency words, even with meaningfulness, imagery, length
             in letters, and frequency excluded as factors. When used as
             foils in a recognition experiment, high associative
             frequency words attracted more responses than low
             associative frequency words. In addition, associative
             frequency and meaningfulness correlated only moderately and
             had different patterns of correlations with other variables,
             suggesting that the number of associations leading to and
             from a word differ. © 1983 Psychonomic Society,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03197665},
   Key = {fds253770}
}

@misc{fds26480,
   Author = {Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Studies of learning and memory},
   Volume = {2},
   Pages = {111-1 - 111-18},
   Booktitle = {Behavioral sciences research in mental health: An assessment
             of the state of the science and recommendations for research
             directions},
   Publisher = {Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental
             Health},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds26480}
}

@article{fds253824,
   Author = {Friendly, M and Franklin, PE and Hoffman, D and Rubin,
             DC},
   Title = {The Toronto Word Pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness,
             orthographic variables, and grammatical usage for 1,080
             words},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {375-399},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1982},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {1554-351X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10179 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Imagery and concreteness norms and percentage noun usage
             were obtained on the 1,080 verbal items from the Toronto
             Word Pool. Imagery was defined as the rated ease with which
             a word aroused a mental image, and concreteness was defined
             in relation to level of abstraction. The degree to which a
             word was functionally a noun was estimated in a sentence
             generation task. The mean and standard deviation of the
             imagery and concreteness ratings for each item are reported
             together with letter and printed frequency counts for the
             words and indications of sex differences in the ratings.
             Additional data in the norms include a grammatical function
             code derived from dictionary definitions, a percent noun
             judgment, indexes of statistical approximation to English,
             and an orthographic neighbor ratio. Validity estimates for
             the imagery and concreteness ratings are derived from
             comparisons with scale values drawn from the Paivio, Yuille,
             and Madigan (1968) noun pool and the Toglia and Battig
             (1978) norms. © 1982 Psychonomic Society,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03203275},
   Key = {fds253824}
}

@article{fds253682,
   Author = {Rubin, D},
   Title = {Memorability as an indicator of processing},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {127-127},
   Publisher = {PSYCHONOMIC SOC INC},
   Year = {1982},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-5054},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:A1982PK24800031&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds253682}
}

@article{fds253771,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Corbett, S},
   Title = {Adaptation-level theory and the free recall of
             mixed-frequency lists},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {27-29},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1982},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0090-5054},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:A1982PB57800009&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Subjects learned a list containing both high-frequency
             (common) and low-frequency (rare) words after learning five
             lists of either high-or low-frequency words. As predicted by
             adaptation-level theory, preexposure to lists at one
             frequency made words at that same frequency more difficult
             to learn relative to words at other frequencies. © 1982,
             Psychonomic Society, Inc.. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03334792},
   Key = {fds253771}
}

@article{fds253772,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {On the retention function for autobiographical
             memory},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {21-38},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1982},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0022-5371},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10177 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {College undergraduates were asked to record events from
             their lives, and then to date those events. Data were
             collected from groups of subjects using a set of cue words
             to prompt the events, from individual subjects, for
             individual cue words, from groups of subjects using no cue
             words, and from subjects who kept diaries. If it is assumed
             that the subjects encoded an equal number of events from
             each day of their lives, the distribution of events recorded
             as a function of time can be viewed as a retention function.
             The data from all experiments provided an excellent fit to
             the single-trace fragility function proposed by Wickelgren
             to account for more traditional laboratory learning
             experiments. Taken together these experiments indicate that
             the retention function is not an artifact of summing
             different functions produced by individual subjects or cue
             words and that the episodes recorded are, for the most part,
             accurately dated memories of actual events. Thus, episodic
             memory of a naturalistic, autobiographical nature and
             episodic memory for lists appear to have the same retention
             properties. © 1982 Academic Press, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90423-6},
   Key = {fds253772}
}

@article{fds253784,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A depth aftereffect caused by viewing a rotating Ames
             window.},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {703-705},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1982},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p110703},
   Abstract = {After a rotating Ames window has been viewed, a normal test
             window held diagonal to the subject's line of sight appears
             to be distorted, having a larger back than front. The effect
             does not occur if a normal window is rotated or if the test
             window is held perpendicular to the subject's line of
             sight.},
   Doi = {10.1068/p110703},
   Key = {fds253784}
}

@article{fds253821,
   Author = {Solso, RL and Juel, C and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {THE FREQUENCY AND VERSATILITY OF INITIAL AND TERMINAL
             LETTERS IN ENGLISH WORDS},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {220-235},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1982},
   ISSN = {0022-5371},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10178 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {A review of previous word and letter counts in addition to
             the applications of these counts were reported. A
             comprehensive count of initial and terminal letters and
             bigrams was compiled based on the Kučera and Francis
             (Computational analysis of present-day American English.
             Providence: Brown Univ. Press, 1967) corpus of English
             words. The count included frequency of occurrence and
             versatility, or number of different words in which letters
             or bigrams occurred. It was shown how such counts can be
             used to describe "Englishness" and make predictions as to
             the information load of letters in words and pseudo words.
             © 1982 Academic Press, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90581-3},
   Key = {fds253821}
}

@misc{fds302180,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Very long-term memory for prose and verse},
   Pages = {229-310},
   Booktitle = {Memory observed: Remembering in natural contexts},
   Publisher = {W. H. Freeman},
   Editor = {Neisser, U},
   Year = {1982},
   Key = {fds302180}
}

@article{fds253774,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {First-order approximation to English, second-order
             approximation to English, and orthographic neighbor ratio
             norms for 925 nouns},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {713-721},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {1554-351X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03207956},
   Abstract = {First- and second-order approximations to English and
             orthographic neighbor ratio values are provided for Paivio,
             Yuille, and Madigan's (1968) 925 nouns. First- and
             second-order approximations to English are information
             theory measures of the probability of generating a word on a
             letter-by-letter basis. The orthographic neighbor ratio is
             the frequency of a word divided by the sum of the
             frequencies of all words that can be generated by changing
             one of its letters. Thus, the orthographic neighbor ratio
             provides a measure of a sophisticated guessing model in
             which partial information about a word is obtained and a
             decision is made on the basis of the relative frequencies of
             the possible responses. Correlations with existing norms are
             reported. © 1981 Psychonomic Society, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03207956},
   Key = {fds253774}
}

@article{fds253825,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Olson, EH and Richter, M and Butters,
             N},
   Title = {Memory for prose in Korsakoff and schizophrenic
             populations.},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {2-3},
   Pages = {81-85},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207458109043304},
   Abstract = {Twelve alcoholic Korsakoff patients, their 12 alcoholic
             controls, and 27 institutionalized schizophrenics and their
             19 controls, recalled two stories. The clinical populations
             recalled approximately half as much as their controls, yet
             recalled the same parts of the stories as their controls.
             For both groups the ability to process simple prose does not
             seem to be severely impaired when what is recalled rather
             than how much is recalled is measured.},
   Doi = {10.3109/00207458109043304},
   Key = {fds253825}
}

@article{fds253826,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Butters, N},
   Title = {Clustering by alcoholic Korsakoff patients.},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {137-140},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0028-3932},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(81)90053-1},
   Abstract = {Twelve alcoholic Korsakoff patients and 12 alcoholic
             controls recalled two clusterable lists, and two
             nonclusterable lists. Korsakoff patients recalled more from
             the clusterable than the nonclusterable lists. Detailed
             analysis of the Korsakoff results indicate that while some
             forms of semantic organization are impaired, the ability to
             use associative structure remains intact. ©
             1981.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0028-3932(81)90053-1},
   Key = {fds253826}
}

@article{fds253773,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Norms for 34 properties of 125 words},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {19},
   Pages = {Ms. 2213},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds253773}
}

@misc{fds302162,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Cognitive processes and oral traditions},
   Pages = {173-180},
   Booktitle = {International Musicological Society: Report of the Twelfth
             Congress Berkeley 1977},
   Publisher = {Barenreiter-Verlag},
   Editor = {Heartz, D and Wade, B},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds302162}
}

@article{fds253827,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Olson, MJ},
   Title = {Recall of semantic domains.},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {354-356},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03198275},
   Abstract = {The order of recall of lists of words learned incidentally
             was analyzed by multidimensional scaling similarity matrices
             based on the number of times words were retrieved next to
             each other. For the semantic domains of mammals, birds, and
             kinship terms, retrieval from very long-term memory, both
             for groups and individuals, and recall of recently learned
             lists produced multidimensional solutions similar to
             published solutions based on judged relatedness and
             associative overlap. For the squares of the Monopoly board
             and the names of the members of the Lawrence University
             faculty, for which clear a priori category structures exist,
             the form of clustering in the order and timing of recall
             that is commonly found in recall of lists learned recently
             in the laboratory was also found in the retrieval of lists
             learned incidentally through multiple exposures over long
             periods of time in the real world. © 1980 Psychonomic
             Society, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03198275},
   Key = {fds253827}
}

@article{fds253775,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {51 properties of 125 words: A unit analysis of verbal
             behavior},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {736-755},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0022-5371},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90415-6},
   Abstract = {Values for 125 words were obtained for 51 scales including
             measures of orthography, pronunciation, imagery,
             categorizability, association, number of attributes,
             age-of-acquisition, word frequency, goodness, emotionality,
             autobiographical memory, tachistoscopic recognition, reading
             latency, lexical decision, incidental and intentional
             recall, recall using a mnemonic pathway, paired-associate
             learning, and recognition. Six factors emerged: Spelling and
             Sound, Imagery and Meaning, Word Frequency, Recall,
             Emotionality, and Goodness. Implications for current
             methodology and theory are discussed, including the claims:
             that multivariate research is a necessary addition to the
             study of verbal behavior; that a unidimensional concept such
             as depth does not do justice to the complexity of recall;
             and that associative frequency, emotionality, and
             pronunciability are among the best predictors of our
             commonly used tasks. © 1980 Academic Press,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90415-6},
   Key = {fds253775}
}

@article{fds253687,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {On measuring fuzziness: a comment on "A fuzzy set approach
             to modifiers and vagueness in natural language".},
   Volume = {108},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {486-489},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1979},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {0096-3445},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:A1979HW41500004&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Hersh and Caramazza's application of fuzzy set theory to
             vagueness in natural language is criticized for including in
             their measures of fuzziness response variability due to
             experimental and statistical procedures.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0096-3445.108.4.486},
   Key = {fds253687}
}

@article{fds253828,
   Author = {Brouwer, JR and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A simple design for an impossible triangle.},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {349-350},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1979},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p080349},
   Doi = {10.1068/p080349},
   Key = {fds253828}
}

@article{fds253777,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Word⇔initial and word⇔final ngram frequencies},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {171-183},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1978},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862967809547266},
   Abstract = {Every word-initial and word-final letter cluster, or ngram,
             that occurred in 30 or more different words in one million
             words of running text is listed along with the number of
             different words and the total number of words it appeared
             in. The relation of this list to other counts is discussed.
             © 1978, SAGE Publications. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1080/10862967809547266},
   Key = {fds253777}
}

@article{fds253778,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A unit analysis of prose memory},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {599-620},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1978},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0022-5371},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90370-5},
   Abstract = {Four stories were divided into function word units. These
             units were assigned dependent variable values determined by
             the scoring of subjects' recalls and independent variable
             values determined by measures of gist, imagery, repetition,
             frequency of occurrence, serial position, grammatical
             connectedness, centrality in a propositional net, and
             subjects' intuitions of which units would be remembered. The
             independent variables were all statistically significant
             predictors of recall. Subjects' intuitions and gist were the
             best predictors of the more structured stories, while
             repetition and serial position were the best predictors of
             the less structured stories. For each story, the underlying
             rank ordering of function word units from most to least
             likely to be remembered was the same for all subjects (i.e.,
             scalable). While changes in the retention interval, subject
             population, and motivation level affected the amount
             recalled, these changes had little affect on the rank
             ordering of the units from most to least likely to be
             remembered. Changes in the retrieval task from free recall
             to prompted recall and recognition affected both the amount
             and rank ordering of units. © 1978 Academic Press,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90370-5},
   Key = {fds253778}
}

@article{fds253706,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Very long-term memory for prose and verse},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {611-621},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1977},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0022-5371},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80023-6},
   Abstract = {Recalls from five passages learned by undergraduates in the
             course of growing up in America were obtained. Unlike
             passages learned in the laboratory, the recalls, while
             partial, were exact with no evidence of constructive memory.
             Although there was no control over learning, practice or
             retention interval, the data are among the most regular in
             cognitive psychology. Function word, first letter, and music
             prompts increased recalls while they decreased a mared
             primacy effect evident in the free recall data. Free recalls
             obtained from fifth and sixth graders resembled the adult
             data. Recalls tended to begin and end at breath pause
             locations. The results fit a simple model of associative
             chaining retrieval of passively stored surface structure
             units. © 1977 Academic Press, Inc. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80023-6},
   Key = {fds253706}
}

@article{fds253829,
   Author = {Rubin, DC and Rebson, DJ},
   Title = {A halo visual illusion.},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {227-230},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1977},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p060227},
   Abstract = {A visual illusion consisting of transparent halos extending
             beyond the boundaries of rotating discs is reported. The
             effect can be obtained by rotating a variety of
             black-and-white discs at moderate speeds. It is not due
             solely to rods, as opposed to cones, and does not appear to
             be explainable in terms of intermittent stimulation of
             portions of visual fields of fixed visual
             angle.},
   Doi = {10.1068/p060227},
   Key = {fds253829}
}

@article{fds253830,
   Author = {Kenny, DA and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Estimating chance reproducibility in Guttman
             Scaling},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {188-196},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1977},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0049-089X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0049-089X(77)90007-2},
   Abstract = {A definition of reproducibility in Guttman Scaling and two
             chance measures of reproducibility are suggested. The first
             measure assumes that the items are independent. The second
             method assumes nonindependent items and fits respondent and
             item margins by an iterative method used in fitting
             log-linear models. Chance reproducibility is conceptualized
             in terms of assumptions about respondent variability. ©
             1977.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0049-089X(77)90007-2},
   Key = {fds253830}
}

@article{fds253831,
   Author = {Brown, L and Heymann, S and Preskill, B and Rubin, DC and Wuletich,
             T},
   Title = {Leading questions and the eyewitness report of a live and a
             described incident},
   Volume = {40},
   Pages = {1041-1042},
   Year = {1977},
   Key = {fds253831}
}

@article{fds253780,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Frequency of occurrence as a psychophysical continuum:
             Weber's fraction, Ekman's fraction, range effects, and the
             phi-gamma hypothesis},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {327-330},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0031-5117},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03199413},
   Abstract = {Using the continuum of frequency of occurrence of words in
             English, it was found that: (1) errors in judgment are
             distributed lognormally rather than normally, and therefore
             the standard method of calculating Weber's fraction
             underestimates its definition, (2) Weber's fraction has an
             extremely large value of 3.3, (3)Ekman's fraction equals
             1.81, not .03 as with sensory continua, and (4)the logarithm
             of the dynamic range times Stevens' law exponent equals
             3.83, not 1.53 as with sensory continua. The last two
             results favor Teghtsoonian's underlying sensory scale
             interpretation over Poulton's range effects interpretation
             of the range-exponent relation. © 1976 Psychonomic Society,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03199413},
   Key = {fds253780}
}

@article{fds253779,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The effectiveness of context before, after, and around a
             missing word},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {214-216},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0031-5117},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03204230},
   Abstract = {The probability of correctly guessing a missing word was
             measured using lour different kinds of context: all words
             before the missing word (forward context), all words after
             the missing word (backward context), all words before and
             the one word after the missing word (surround context), and
             just the one word after the missing word. The probability of
             correctly guessing a missing word was greater with the
             forward than with the backward context. The probability of
             guessing correctly with the surround context was much
             greater than would be predicted from the independent
             combination of its forward and one word after components.
             The results provide evidence that expectations are formed
             continuously during comprehension, but not in a strict
             word-by-word order. Implications for information theory are
             noted. © 1976 Psychonomic Society, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03204230},
   Key = {fds253779}
}

@article{fds253781,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Applying psychometric methods in linguistic research: Some
             recent advances},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {168},
   Pages = {63-66},
   Publisher = {Walter de Gruyter GmbH},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ling.1976.14.168.63},
   Doi = {10.1515/ling.1976.14.168.63},
   Key = {fds253781}
}

@article{fds253767,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {A simple method for producing figures for
             publication},
   Journal = {Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation},
   Volume = {8},
   Pages = {40-41},
   Year = {1976},
   Key = {fds253767}
}

@article{fds253782,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Within word structure in the tip-of-the-tongue
             phenomenon},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {392-397},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1975},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0022-5371},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80018-1},
   Abstract = {Definitions of four rare words were read to 259 undergradua
             tes. Those subjects who were in the tip-of-the-tongue (TOT)
             state recorded all the letters they knew. The within-word
             structure of the resulting 101 partial recalls was
             indistinguishable from that of similar sounding words from
             earlier studies. In both sets of data, morpheme-like
             clusters of letters were evident. The recall of high
             frequency clusters at the end of words could not be
             explained in terms of sophisticated guessing. The results
             support a distinct memory system for word names which is
             organized for use in the production and perception of speech
             and writing. © 1975 Academic Press, Inc. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80018-1},
   Key = {fds253782}
}

@article{fds253783,
   Author = {Rubin, DC},
   Title = {The subjective estimation of relative syllable
             frequency},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {193-196},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {1974},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0031-5117},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203273},
   Abstract = {Ss are able to judge the relative frequency of occurrence in
             English of nonmorphemic syllables independent of phoneme
             frequency. The results support a theory of speech perception
             based on the syllable as a unit as opposed to the phoneme.
             © 1974 Psychonomic Society, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.3758/BF03203273},
   Key = {fds253783}
}

@misc{fds26478,
   Author = {Garfein, A. and Rindner, W. and Rubin, D.C.},
   Title = {Electricity measurement devices employing liquid crystalline
             materials},
   Journal = {United States Patent Number 3,667,039},
   Year = {1972},
   Key = {fds26478}
}

@article{fds253794,
   Author = {Melamed, L and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Selected optical properties of mixtures of cholesteric
             liquid crystals},
   Journal = {Applied Optics},
   Volume = {10},
   Pages = {1103-1107},
   Year = {1971},
   Key = {fds253794}
}

@article{fds253795,
   Author = {Melamed, L and Rubin, DC},
   Title = {Electric field hysteresis effects in cholesteric liquid
             crystals},
   Journal = {Applied Physics Letters},
   Volume = {16},
   Pages = {149-150},
   Year = {1970},
   Key = {fds253795}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Linguistics * Faculty * Librarian * Staff * Reload * Login