Psychology and Neuroscience Faculty Database
Psychology and Neuroscience
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > pn > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Lawrence G. Appelbaum    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Papers Published   
@article{fds367627,
   Author = {Subramaniam, A and Liu, S and Lochhead, L and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {A systematic review of transcranial direct current
             stimulation on eye movements and associated psychological
             function.},
   Journal = {Rev Neurosci},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {349-364},
   Publisher = {WALTER DE GRUYTER GMBH},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2022-0082},
   Abstract = {The last decades have seen a rise in the use of transcranial
             direct current stimulation (tDCS) approaches to modulate
             brain activity and associated behavior. Concurrently, eye
             tracking (ET) technology has improved to allow more precise
             quantitative measurement of gaze behavior, offering a window
             into the mechanisms of vision and cognition. When combined,
             tDCS and ET provide a powerful system to probe brain
             function and measure the impact on visual function, leading
             to an increasing number of studies that utilize these
             techniques together. The current pre-registered, systematic
             review seeks to describe the literature that integrates
             these approaches with the goal of changing brain activity
             with tDCS and measuring associated changes in eye movements
             with ET. The literature search identified 26 articles that
             combined ET and tDCS in a probe-and-measure model and are
             systematically reviewed here. All studies implemented
             controlled interventional designs to address topics related
             to oculomotor control, cognitive processing, emotion
             regulation, or cravings in healthy volunteers and patient
             populations. Across these studies, active stimulation
             typically led to changes in the number, duration, and timing
             of fixations compared to control stimulation. Notably, half
             the studies addressed emotion regulation, each showing
             hypothesized effects of tDCS on ET metrics, while tDCS
             targeting the frontal cortex was widely used and also
             generally produced expected modulation of ET. This review
             reveals promising evidence of the impact of tDCS on eye
             movements and associated psychological function, offering a
             framework for effective designs with recommendations for
             future studies.},
   Doi = {10.1515/revneuro-2022-0082},
   Key = {fds367627}
}

@article{fds367447,
   Author = {Bukhari-Parlakturk, N and Lutz, MW and Al-Khalidi, HR and Unnithan,
             S and Wang, JE-H and Scott, B and Termsarasab, P and Appelbaum, LG and Calakos, N},
   Title = {Suitability of Automated Writing Measures for Clinical Trial
             Outcome in Writer's Cramp.},
   Journal = {Mov Disord},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {123-132},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mds.29237},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND: Writer's cramp (WC) dystonia is a rare disease
             that causes abnormal postures during the writing task.
             Successful research studies for WC and other forms of
             dystonia are contingent on identifying sensitive and
             specific measures that relate to the clinical syndrome and
             achieve a realistic sample size to power research studies
             for a rare disease. Although prior studies have used writing
             kinematics, their diagnostic performance remains unclear.
             OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to evaluate the diagnostic
             performance of automated measures that distinguish subjects
             with WC from healthy volunteers. METHODS: A total of 21
             subjects with WC and 22 healthy volunteers performed a
             sentence-copying assessment on a digital tablet using
             kinematic and hand recognition softwares. The sensitivity
             and specificity of automated measures were calculated using
             a logistic regression model. Power analysis was performed
             for two clinical research designs using these measures. The
             test and retest reliability of select automated measures was
             compared across repeat sentence-copying assessments. Lastly,
             a correlational analysis with subject- and clinician-rated
             outcomes was performed to understand the clinical meaning of
             automated measures. RESULTS: Of the 23 measures analyzed,
             the measures of word legibility and peak accelerations
             distinguished subjects with WC from healthy volunteers with
             high sensitivity and specificity and demonstrated smaller
             sample sizes suitable for rare disease studies, and the
             kinematic measures showed high reliability across repeat
             visits, while both word legibility and peak accelerations
             measures showed significant correlations with the subject-
             and clinician-rated outcomes. CONCLUSIONS: Novel automated
             measures that capture key aspects of the disease and are
             suitable for use in clinical research studies of WC dystonia
             were identified. © 2022 International Parkinson and
             Movement Disorder Society.},
   Doi = {10.1002/mds.29237},
   Key = {fds367447}
}

@article{fds369099,
   Author = {Ho, J and Liu, S and Feng, Z and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Psychomotor and visual skills underlying position
             specialization in 1352 elite youth baseball
             players.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e0278689},
   Publisher = {Public Library of Science (PLoS)},
   Editor = {Gu, Y},
   Year = {2023},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0278689},
   Abstract = {Baseball is an international sport with participation from
             tens of thousands of people worldwide. In the United States,
             the Prospect Development Pipeline (PDP) is a collaborative
             effort between Major League Baseball and USA Baseball to
             establish a developmental pipeline leading to the
             professional draft. Players participating in the PDP undergo
             comprehensive evaluations that measure athletic performance,
             speed-of-processing, visual function, and on-field talent.
             The present study evaluated data from 1352 elite junior male
             PDP participants (aged 14 to 21) who signed informed
             consent, collected between 2017 and 2020, to identify latent
             abilities and their association with player specialization.
             Data were first subjected to Exploratory Factor Analysis
             (EFA) to reduce the 22 measured variables to a smaller set
             of latent abilities. The resulting factors were evaluated
             using multiple linear regression to predict each factor
             using age, height, weight, and position. EFA revealed a
             combination of physical and psychomotor skills accounting
             for 52% of the overall variance that grouped into four
             abilities: grip strength, functional vision, explosiveness,
             and rapid decision-making. Regression analyses demonstrated
             that these skills are associated with position assignments,
             controlling for age, weight, and height, and revealed that
             outfielders are the most explosive, infielders perform best
             on psychomotor measures, and catchers perform best on
             functional vision tests (ps < 0.001). These findings
             indicate skills that contribute to player specialization,
             providing new information about the developmental trajectory
             of junior elite baseball athletes that can be used for
             scouting and player development.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0278689},
   Key = {fds369099}
}

@article{fds361836,
   Author = {Neacsiu, AD and Beynel, L and Graner, JL and Szabo, ST and Appelbaum,
             LG and Smoski, MJ and LaBar, KS},
   Title = {Enhancing cognitive restructuring with concurrent
             fMRI-guided neurostimulation for emotional dysregulation-A
             randomized controlled trial.},
   Journal = {J Affect Disord},
   Volume = {301},
   Pages = {378-389},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.01.053},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND: Transdiagnostic clinical emotional dysregulation
             is a key component of many mental health disorders and
             offers an avenue to address multiple disorders with one
             transdiagnostic treatment. In the current study, we pilot an
             intervention that combines a one-time teaching and practice
             of cognitive restructuring (CR) with repetitive transcranial
             magnetic stimulation (rTMS), targeted based on functional
             magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). METHODS: Thirty-seven
             clinical adults who self-reported high emotional
             dysregulation were enrolled in this randomized,
             double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. fMRI was collected
             as participants were reminded of lifetime stressors and
             asked to downregulate their distress using CR tactics. fMRI
             BOLD data were analyzed to identify the cluster of voxels
             within the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) with
             the highest activation when participants attempted to
             downregulate, versus passively remember, distressing
             memories. Participants underwent active or sham rTMS (10 Hz)
             over the left dlPFC target while practicing CR following
             emotional induction using recent autobiographical stressors.
             RESULTS: Receiving active versus sham rTMS led to
             significantly higher high frequency heart rate variability
             during regulation, lower regulation duration during the
             intervention, and higher likelihood to use CR during the
             week following the intervention. There were no differences
             between conditions when administering neurostimulation alone
             without the CR skill and compared to sham. Participants in
             the sham versus active condition experienced less distress
             the week after the intervention. There were no differences
             between conditions at the one-month follow up. CONCLUSION:
             This study demonstrated that combining active rTMS with
             emotion regulation training for one session significantly
             enhances emotion regulation and augments the impact of
             training for as long as a week. These findings are a
             promising step towards a combined intervention for
             transdiagnostic emotion dysregulation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jad.2022.01.053},
   Key = {fds361836}
}

@article{fds362795,
   Author = {Dubljević, V and Young, JR and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Diversifying the Bioethics Funding Landscape: The Case of
             TMS.},
   Journal = {Am J Bioeth},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {28-30},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15265161.2021.2001106},
   Doi = {10.1080/15265161.2021.2001106},
   Key = {fds362795}
}

@article{fds359595,
   Author = {Neacsiu, AD and Beynel, L and Powers, JP and Szabo, ST and Appelbaum,
             LG and Lisanby, SH and LaBar, KS},
   Title = {Enhancing Cognitive Restructuring with Concurrent Repetitive
             Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: A Transdiagnostic
             Randomized Controlled Trial.},
   Journal = {Psychother Psychosom},
   Volume = {91},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {94-106},
   Year = {2022},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000518957},
   Abstract = {INTRODUCTION: Emotional dysregulation constitutes a serious
             public health problem in need of novel transdiagnostic
             treatments. OBJECTIVE: To this aim, we developed and tested
             a one-time intervention that integrates behavioral skills
             training with concurrent repetitive transcranial magnetic
             stimulation (rTMS). METHODS: Forty-six adults who met
             criteria for at least one DSM-5 disorder and self-reported
             low use of cognitive restructuring (CR) were enrolled in a
             randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled trial that used a
             between-subjects design. Participants were taught CR and
             underwent active rTMS applied at 10 Hz over the right (n =
             17) or left (n = 14) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC)
             or sham rTMS (n = 15) while practicing reframing and
             emotional distancing in response to autobiographical
             stressors. RESULTS: Those who received active left or active
             right as opposed to sham rTMS exhibited enhanced regulation
             (ds = 0.21-0.62) as measured by psychophysiological indices
             during the intervention (higher high-frequency heart rate
             variability, lower regulation duration). Those who received
             active rTMS over the left dlPFC also self-reported reduced
             distress throughout the intervention (d = 0.30), higher
             likelihood to use CR, and lower daily distress during the
             week following the intervention. The procedures were
             acceptable and feasible with few side effects. CONCLUSIONS:
             These findings show that engaging frontal circuits
             simultaneously with cognitive skills training and rTMS may
             be clinically feasible, well-tolerated and may show promise
             for the treatment of transdiagnostic emotional
             dysregulation. Larger follow-up studies are needed to
             confirm the efficacy of this novel therapeutic
             approach.},
   Doi = {10.1159/000518957},
   Key = {fds359595}
}

@article{fds364090,
   Author = {Laby, DM and Appelbaum, LG and Hülsdünker, T and Putrino,
             D},
   Title = {Editorial: Neural Mechanisms of Perceptual-Cognitive
             Expertise in Elite Performers.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Human Neuroscience},
   Volume = {16},
   Pages = {923816},
   Year = {2022},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.923816},
   Doi = {10.3389/fnhum.2022.923816},
   Key = {fds364090}
}

@article{fds359594,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Dannhauer, M and Palmer, H and Hilbig, SA and Crowell, CA and Wang, JE-H and Michael, AM and Wood, EA and Luber, B and Lisanby, SH and Peterchev, AV and Cabeza, R and Davis, SW and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Network-based rTMS to modulate working memory: The difficult
             choice of effective parameters for online
             interventions.},
   Journal = {Brain and Behavior},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {e2361},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2361},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND: Online repetitive transcranialmagnetic
             stimulation (rTMS) has been shown to modulate working memory
             (WM) performance in a site-specific manner, with behavioral
             improvements due to stimulation of the dorsolateral
             prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and impairment from stimulation
             to the lateral parietal cortex (LPC). Neurobehavioral
             studies have demonstrated that subprocesses of WM allowing
             for the maintenance and manipulation of information in the
             mind involve unique cortical networks. Despite promising
             evidence of modulatory effects of rTMS on WM, no studies
             have yet demonstrated distinct modulatory control of these
             two subprocesses. The current study therefore sought to
             explore this possibility through site-specific stimulation
             during an online task invoking both skills. METHODS:
             Twenty-nine subjects completed a 4-day protocol, in which
             active or sham 5Hz rTMS was applied over the DLPFC and LPC
             in separate blocks of trials while participants performed
             tasks that required either maintenance alone, or both
             maintenance and manipulation (alphabetization) of
             information. Stimulation targets were defined individually
             based on fMRI activation and structural network properties.
             Stimulation amplitude was adjusted using electric field
             modeling to equate induced current in the target region
             across participants. RESULTS: Despite the use of advanced
             techniques, no significant differences or interactions
             between active and sham stimulation were found. Exploratory
             analyses testing stimulation amplitude, fMRI activation, and
             modal controllability showed nonsignificant but interesting
             trends with rTMS effects. CONCLUSION: While this study did
             not reveal any significant behavioral changes in WM, the
             results may point to parameters that contribute to positive
             effects, such as stimulation amplitude and functional
             activation.},
   Doi = {10.1002/brb3.2361},
   Key = {fds359594}
}

@article{fds358711,
   Author = {Young, JR and Galla, JT and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Treatment for Smoking
             Cessation: An Introduction for Primary Care
             Clinicians.},
   Journal = {Am J Med},
   Volume = {134},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {1339-1343},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amjmed.2021.06.037},
   Abstract = {Tobacco use remains the number one preventable cause of
             death in the United States, resulting in significant public
             health and economic costs. Despite progress in reducing
             tobacco use through pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy
             smoking cessation interventions, additional treatment
             options are still needed to improve treatment effectiveness.
             As an adjunctive treatment, the US Food and Drug
             Administration recently cleared transcranial magnetic
             stimulation (TMS), a noninvasive brain stimulation
             technique, as an aid for smoking cessation in adults. Given
             that most smoking cessation interventions occur in the
             primary care setting, this article aims to introduce TMS, to
             provide an overview of the evidence of TMS for smoking
             cessation, and to outline the procedures for implementing
             TMS in the primary care setting when referral to an
             interventional psychiatrist is not possible. With growing
             scientific evidence and increasing regulatory approval of
             TMS for smoking cessation, this novel treatment option is
             now available for patients who want to quit smoking but have
             been unsuccessful with pharmacologic approaches.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.amjmed.2021.06.037},
   Key = {fds358711}
}

@article{fds359930,
   Author = {Liu, S and Folstein, JR and Appelbaum, LG and Tenenbaum,
             G},
   Title = {Effects of control strategies on the activation of unwanted
             intrusive thoughts in elite athletes.},
   Journal = {J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {1395-1408},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000945},
   Abstract = {Unwanted intrusive thoughts (UITs) are negative ruminations
             that occur commonly and show similar characteristics between
             clinical and nonclinical forms. Despite their prevalence and
             impact, the control processes of UITs remain unclear. This
             study aimed to capture the effects of three thought-control
             strategies on UITs through an interventional design with
             individualized choking thoughts among Elite athletes, while
             measuring behavioral and brain responses. Ninety athletes
             recollected recent "choking" experiences prior to being
             randomized into one of three groups that used either
             acceptance, suppression, or passive monitoring (control
             condition). The activation of choking thoughts during and
             after the thought control intervention was gauged through
             three measurement approaches, including conscious presence
             in mind, priming, as well as the amplitudes of the P3b and
             N400 event-related potentials (ERPs). To strengthen the
             causal inferences concerning thought control strategies,
             athletes' working memory capacity was measured and
             controlled at baseline. Results indicated that, relative to
             passive monitoring, suppression led to enhanced priming and
             reduced conscious presence of UITs, whereas acceptance
             resulted in an opposite pattern of reduced priming and
             increased conscious presence of UITs. Moreover, UIT-related
             stimuli elicited less negative-going N400 amplitudes and
             more positive-going P3b amplitudes than UIT-irrelevant
             stimuli, although no effect of thought control strategies
             was identified on the ERPs. These findings contribute to the
             theoretical and mechanistic understanding of UIT control
             processes by revealing subtle effects of two fundamental UIT
             control strategies: suppression and acceptance. Such
             insights also bear meaningful applied implications.
             (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xhp0000945},
   Key = {fds359930}
}

@article{fds358315,
   Author = {Laby, DM and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Review: Vision and On-field Performance: A Critical Review
             of Visual Assessment and Training Studies with
             Athletes.},
   Journal = {Optom Vis Sci},
   Volume = {98},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {723-731},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/OPX.0000000000001729},
   Abstract = {Sports vision is an emerging field that seeks to establish
             the relationships between visual function and sports
             performance. Here we provide the first critical review of
             empirical studies that attempt to link visual assessments
             and vision training to competitive game performance.Vision
             is essential to producing controlled movement, and
             therefore, it is intuitive that better visual abilities
             should relate to better sporting performance. This notion
             has been central to the field of sports vision, an area of
             study that seeks to determine the visual skills that
             underlie optimal sports performance and investigate
             approaches to train these abilities to improve sports
             performance. Although this field now contains hundreds of
             published articles addressing visual assessment and training
             in athletes, relatively few have attempted to directly link
             these capabilities to on-field production statistics from
             competitive matches. The objectives of this article are both
             to describe the theoretical and experimental framework
             necessary for such research and to critically review the
             empirical literature that has attempted to directly link
             visual assessments and/or training to athletic performance.
             We begin by describing why such associations are important
             and then provide an evidence-based framework for evaluating
             the quality of research in this domain. This is followed by
             a summary and review of the qualified literature that has
             addressed either relationships between baseline assessments
             and game performance or the effects of visual training
             interventions on game performance. Based on this review, it
             is concluded that, despite promising evidence supporting the
             role of vision in sports performance and improvements due to
             training, the specialty is still in need of methodological
             improvements. It is recommended that studies aim for larger
             better-powered studies, consistent and precise outcome
             measures, and greater scientific rigor such as obtained
             through randomized placebo-controlled designs with
             pre-registration of hypotheses.},
   Doi = {10.1097/OPX.0000000000001729},
   Key = {fds358315}
}

@article{fds358314,
   Author = {Fogt, N and Appelbaum, LG and Dalton, K and Erickson, G and Gray,
             R},
   Title = {Guest Editorial: Visual Function and Sports
             Performance.},
   Journal = {Optom Vis Sci},
   Volume = {98},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {669-671},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/OPX.0000000000001717},
   Doi = {10.1097/OPX.0000000000001717},
   Key = {fds358314}
}

@article{fds357346,
   Author = {Laby, DM and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Comment on Nascimento et al. Citations Network Analysis of
             Vision and Sport. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,
             17, 7574.},
   Journal = {International Journal of Environmental Research and Public
             Health},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {6488},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126488},
   Abstract = {In October 2020, the paper [...].},
   Doi = {10.3390/ijerph18126488},
   Key = {fds357346}
}

@article{fds358005,
   Author = {Liu, S and Clements, JM and Kirsch, EP and Rao, HM and Zielinski, DJ and Lu, Y and Mainsah, BO and Potter, ND and Sommer, MA and Kopper, R and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Psychophysiological Markers of Performance and Learning
             during Simulated Marksmanship in Immersive Virtual
             Reality.},
   Journal = {J Cogn Neurosci},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1253-1270},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01713},
   Abstract = {The fusion of immersive virtual reality, kinematic movement
             tracking, and EEG offers a powerful test bed for
             naturalistic neuroscience research. Here, we combined these
             elements to investigate the neuro-behavioral mechanisms
             underlying precision visual-motor control as 20 participants
             completed a three-visit, visual-motor, coincidence-anticipation
             task, modeled after Olympic Trap Shooting and performed in
             immersive and interactive virtual reality. Analyses of the
             kinematic metrics demonstrated learning of more efficient
             movements with significantly faster hand RTs, earlier
             trigger response times, and higher spatial precision,
             leading to an average of 13% improvement in shot scores
             across the visits. As revealed through spectral and
             time-locked analyses of the EEG beta band (13-30 Hz), power
             measured prior to target launch and visual-evoked potential
             amplitudes measured immediately after the target launch
             correlated with subsequent reactive kinematic performance in
             the shooting task. Moreover, both launch-locked and
             shot/feedback-locked visual-evoked potentials became earlier
             and more negative with practice, pointing to neural
             mechanisms that may contribute to the development of
             visual-motor proficiency. Collectively, these findings
             illustrate EEG and kinematic biomarkers of precision motor
             control and changes in the neurophysiological substrates
             that may underlie motor learning.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_01713},
   Key = {fds358005}
}

@article{fds356042,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Campbell, E and Naclerio, M and Galla, JT and Ghosal, A and Michael, AM and Kimbrel, NA and Davis, SW and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {The Effects of Functionally Guided, Connectivity-Based rTMS
             on Amygdala Activation.},
   Journal = {Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {494},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11040494},
   Abstract = {While repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is
             widely used to treat psychiatric disorders, innovations are
             needed to improve its efficacy. An important limitation is
             that while psychiatric disorders are associated with
             fronto-limbic dysregulation, rTMS does not have sufficient
             depth penetration to modulate affected subcortical
             structures. Recent advances in task-related functional
             connectivity provide a means to better link superficial and
             deeper cortical sources with the possibility of increasing
             fronto-limbic modulation to induce stronger therapeutic
             effects. The objective of this pilot study was to test
             whether task-related, connectivity-based rTMS could modulate
             amygdala activation through its connectivity with the medial
             prefrontal cortex (mPFC). fMRI was collected to identify a
             node in the mPFC showing the strongest connectivity with the
             amygdala, as defined by psychophysiological interaction
             analysis. To promote Hebbian-like plasticity, and
             potentially stronger modulation, 5 Hz rTMS was applied while
             participants viewed frightening video-clips that engaged the
             fronto-limbic network. Significant increases in both the
             mPFC and amygdala were found for active rTMS compared to
             sham, offering promising preliminary evidence that
             functional connectivity-based targeting may provide a useful
             approach to treat network dysregulation. Further research is
             needed to better understand connectivity influences on rTMS
             effects to leverage this information to improve therapeutic
             applications.},
   Doi = {10.3390/brainsci11040494},
   Key = {fds356042}
}

@article{fds358938,
   Author = {Neacsiu, A and Beynel, L and Powers, J and Szabo, S and Appelbaum, L and Lisanby, S and LaBar, K},
   Title = {Enhancing Cognitive Restructuring with Concurrent Repetitive
             Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: A Transdiagnostic
             Randomized Controlled Trial},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.18.21250060},
   Abstract = {<h4>Introduction</h4> Emotional dysregulation constitutes a
             serious public health problem in need of novel
             transdiagnostic treatments. <h4>Objective</h4> To this aim,
             we developed and tested a one-time intervention that
             integrates behavioral skills training with concurrent
             repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS).
             <h4>Methods</h4> Forty-six adults who met criteria for at
             least one DSM-5 disorder and self-reported low use of
             cognitive restructuring (CR) were enrolled in a randomized,
             double-blind, sham-controlled trial that used a
             between-subjects design. Participants were taught CR and
             underwent active rTMS applied at 10 Hz over the right (n=
             17) or left (n= 14) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC)
             or sham rTMS (n= 15) while practicing reframing and
             emotional distancing in response to autobiographical
             stressors. <h4>Results</h4> Those who received active left
             or active right as opposed to sham rTMS exhibited enhanced
             regulation ( d s = 0.21 - 0.62) as measured by
             psychophysiological indices during the intervention (higher
             high-frequency heart rate variability, lower regulation
             duration). Those who received active rTMS over the left
             DLPFC also self-reported reduced distress througout the
             intervention ( d = 0.30), higher likelihood to use CR, and
             lower daily distress during the week following the
             intervention. The procedures were acceptable and feasible
             with few side effects. <h4>Conclusions</h4> These findings
             show that engaging frontal circuits simultaneously with
             cognitive skills training and rTMS may be clinically
             feasible, well-tolerated and may show promise for the
             treatment of transdiagnostic emotional dysregulation. Larger
             follow up studies are needed to confirm the efficacy of this
             novel therapeutic approach.},
   Doi = {10.1101/2021.01.18.21250060},
   Key = {fds358938}
}

@article{fds351223,
   Author = {Liu, S and Ferris, LM and Hilbig, S and Asamoa, E and LaRue, JL and Lyon,
             D and Connolly, K and Port, N and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Dynamic vision training transfers positively to batting
             practice performance among collegiate baseball
             batters},
   Journal = {Psychology of Sport and Exercise},
   Volume = {51},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101759},
   Abstract = {A growing body of evidence demonstrates visual, perceptual,
             and oculomotor abilities contribute to batting performance
             in baseball and there is interest in whether training such
             abilities can transfer positively to batting performance.
             The current study tested this question through a
             pre-registered, randomized, and placebo-controlled
             intervention, conducted with 24 collegiate baseball players
             at two NCAA Division 1 universities. Athletes were
             randomized to receive either dynamic vision training
             consisting of stroboscopic, anticipatory timing, and eye
             quickness drills, or placebo drills stylized after control
             procedures in previous vision therapy studies. Generalized
             near-transfer was tested via a digital visual-motor task
             battery (n = 20), while sports-specific intermediate and far
             transfer of training were evaluated through instrumented
             batting practice metrics (n = 14) and box score performance
             in NCAA-sanctioned games (n = 12), respectively. The effects
             of training group were tested on these outcome measures
             while controlling for covariates such as pre-training
             expectations and site. Participants averaged 8.50 hours of
             training with no significant group differences in training
             adherence, expectations, or baseline assessments. ANCOVA
             revealed no group differences in measures of visual-motor
             skills or NCAA game statistics. However, batting practice
             demonstrated significant improvements in launch angle (p =
             0.002, Cohen's d = 0.74) and hit distance (p < 0.001,
             Cohen's d = 0.70) for the active training cohort relative to
             the placebo control. This controlled and pre-registered
             pilot study therefore provides preliminary evidence that
             vision training may improve batting practice performance,
             creating new opportunities for the transfer of skill
             training and warranting further study.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101759},
   Key = {fds351223}
}

@article{fds351416,
   Author = {Gamboa Arana and OL and Palmer, H and Dannhauer, M and Hile, C and Liu, S and Hamdan, R and Brito, A and Cabeza, R and Davis, SW and Peterchev, AV and Sommer, MA and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Intensity- and timing-dependent modulation of motion
             perception with transcranial magnetic stimulation of visual
             cortex.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologia},
   Volume = {147},
   Pages = {107581},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107581},
   Abstract = {Despite the widespread use of transcranial magnetic
             stimulation (TMS) in research and clinical care, the
             dose-response relations and neurophysiological correlates of
             modulatory effects remain relatively unexplored. To fill
             this gap, we studied modulation of visual processing as a
             function of TMS parameters. Our approach combined
             electroencephalography (EEG) with application of single
             pulse TMS to visual cortex as participants performed a
             motion perception task. During each participants' first
             visit, motion coherence thresholds, 64-channel visual evoked
             potentials (VEPs), and TMS resting motor thresholds (RMT)
             were measured. In second and third visits, single pulse TMS
             was delivered at one of two latencies, either 30 ms before
             the onset of motion or at the onset latency of the N2 VEP
             component derived from the first session. TMS was delivered
             at 0%, 80%, 100%, or 120% of RMT over the site of N2 peak
             activity, or at 120% over vertex. Behavioral results
             demonstrated a significant main effect of TMS timing on
             accuracy, with better performance when TMS was applied at
             the N2-Onset timing versus Pre-Onset, as well as a
             significant interaction, indicating that 80% intensity
             produced higher accuracy than other conditions at the
             N2-Onset. TMS effects on the P3 VEP showed reduced
             amplitudes in the 80% Pre-Onset condition, an increase for
             the 120% N2-Onset condition, and monotonic amplitude scaling
             with stimulation intensity. The N2 component was not
             affected by TMS. These findings reveal the influence of TMS
             intensity and timing on visual perception and
             electrophysiological responses, with optimal facilitation at
             stimulation intensities below RMT.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107581},
   Key = {fds351416}
}

@article{fds351500,
   Author = {Powers, JP and Davis, SW and Neacsiu, AD and Beynel, L and Appelbaum,
             LG and LaBar, KS},
   Title = {Examining the Role of Lateral Parietal Cortex in Emotional
             Distancing Using TMS.},
   Journal = {Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1090-1102},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00821-5},
   Abstract = {We recently proposed a neurocognitive model of distancing-an
             emotion regulation tactic-with a focus on the lateral
             parietal cortex. Although this brain area has been
             implicated in both cognitive control and self-projection
             processes during distancing, fMRI work suggests that these
             processes may be dissociable here. This preregistered
             (NCT03698591) study tested the contribution of left
             temporoparietal junction (TPJ) to distancing using
             repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. We
             hypothesized that inhibiting left TPJ would decrease the
             efficiency of distancing but not distraction, another
             regulation tactic with similar cognitive control
             requirements, thus implicating this region in the
             self-projection processes unique to distancing. Active and
             sham continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS) were applied
             to 30 healthy adults in a single-session crossover design.
             Tactic efficiency was measured using online reports of
             valence and effort. The stimulation target was established
             from the group TPJ fMRI activation peak in an independent
             sample using the same distancing task, and anatomical MRI
             scans were used for individual targeting. Analyses employed
             both repeated-measures ANOVA and analytic procedures
             tailored to crossover designs. Irrespective of cTBS,
             distancing led to greater decreases in negative valence over
             time relative to distraction, and distancing effort
             decreased over time while distraction effort remained
             stable. Exploratory analyses also revealed that active cTBS
             made distancing more effortful, but not distraction. Thus,
             left TPJ seems to support self-projection processes in
             distancing, and these processes may be facilitated by
             repeated use. These findings help to clarify the role of
             lateral parietal cortex in distancing and inform
             applications of distancing and distraction.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13415-020-00821-5},
   Key = {fds351500}
}

@article{fds349781,
   Author = {Crowell, CA and Davis, SW and Beynel, L and Deng, L and Lakhlani, D and Hilbig, SA and Palmer, H and Brito, A and Peterchev, AV and Luber, B and Lisanby, SH and Appelbaum, LG and Cabeza, R},
   Title = {Older adults benefit from more widespread brain network
             integration during working memory.},
   Journal = {Neuroimage},
   Volume = {218},
   Pages = {116959},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116959},
   Abstract = {Neuroimaging evidence suggests that the aging brain relies
             on a more distributed set of cortical regions than younger
             adults in order to maintain successful levels of performance
             during demanding cognitive tasks. However, it remains
             unclear how task demands give rise to this age-related
             expansion in cortical networks. To investigate this issue,
             functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure
             univariate activity, network connectivity, and cognitive
             performance in younger and older adults during a working
             memory (WM) task. Here, individuals performed a WM task in
             which they held letters online while reordering them
             alphabetically. WM load was titrated to obtain four
             individualized difficulty levels with different set sizes.
             Network integration-defined as the ratio of within-versus
             between-network connectivity-was linked to individual
             differences in WM capacity. The study yielded three main
             findings. First, as task difficulty increased, network
             integration decreased in younger adults, whereas it
             increased in older adults. Second, age-related increases in
             network integration were driven by increases in right
             hemisphere connectivity to both left and right cortical
             regions, a finding that helps to reconcile existing theories
             of compensatory recruitment in aging. Lastly, older adults
             with higher WM capacity demonstrated higher levels of
             network integration in the most difficult task condition.
             These results shed light on the mechanisms of age-related
             network reorganization by demonstrating that changes in
             network connectivity may act as an adaptive form of
             compensation, with older adults recruiting a more
             distributed cortical network as task demands
             increase.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116959},
   Key = {fds349781}
}

@article{fds351078,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Deng, L and Crowell, CA and Dannhauer, M and Palmer, H and Hilbig, S and Peterchev, AV and Luber, B and Lisanby, SH and Cabeza, R and Appelbaum, LG and Davis, SW},
   Title = {Structural Controllability Predicts Functional Patterns and
             Brain Stimulation Benefits Associated with Working
             Memory.},
   Journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {35},
   Pages = {6770-6778},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0531-20.2020},
   Abstract = {The brain is an inherently dynamic system, and much work has
             focused on the ability to modify neural activity through
             both local perturbations and changes in the function of
             global network ensembles. Network controllability is a
             recent concept in network neuroscience that purports to
             predict the influence of individual cortical sites on global
             network states and state changes, thereby creating a
             unifying account of local influences on global brain
             dynamics. While this notion is accepted in engineering
             science, it is subject to ongoing debates in neuroscience as
             empirical evidence linking network controllability to brain
             activity and human behavior remains scarce. Here, we present
             an integrated set of multimodal brain-behavior relationships
             derived from fMRI, diffusion tensor imaging, and online
             repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) applied
             during an individually calibrated working memory task
             performed by individuals of both sexes. The modes describing
             the structural network system dynamics showed direct
             relationships to brain activity associated with task
             difficulty, with difficult-to-reach modes contributing to
             functional brain states in the hard task condition. Modal
             controllability (a measure quantifying the contribution of
             difficult-to-reach modes) at the stimulated site predicted
             both fMRI activations associated with increasing task
             difficulty and rTMS benefits on task performance.
             Furthermore, fMRI explained 64% of the variance between
             modal controllability and the working memory benefit
             associated with 5 Hz online rTMS. These results therefore
             provide evidence toward the functional validity of network
             control theory, and outline a clear technique for
             integrating structural network topology and functional
             activity to predict the influence of stimulation on
             subsequent behavior.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The network
             controllability concept proposes that specific cortical
             nodes are able to steer the brain into certain physiological
             states. By applying external perturbation to these control
             nodes, it is theorized that brain stimulation is able to
             selectively target difficult-to-reach states, potentially
             aiding processing and improving performance on cognitive
             tasks. The current study used rTMS and fMRI during a working
             memory task to test this hypothesis. We demonstrate that
             network controllability correlates with fMRI modulation
             because of working memory load and with the behavioral
             improvements that result from a multivisit intervention
             using 5 Hz rTMS. This study demonstrates the validity of
             network controllability and offers a new targeting approach
             to improve efficacy.},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0531-20.2020},
   Key = {fds351078}
}

@article{fds350574,
   Author = {Liu, S and Edmunds, FR and Burris, K and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Visual and oculomotor abilities predict professional
             baseball batting performance},
   Journal = {International Journal of Performance Analysis in
             Sport},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {683-700},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/24748668.2020.1777819},
   Abstract = {Scientists and practitioners have long debated about the
             specific visual skills needed to excel at hitting a pitched
             baseball. To advance this debate, we evaluated the
             relationship between pre-season visual and oculomotor
             evaluations and pitch-by-pitch season performance data from
             professional baseball batters. Eye tracking, visual-motor,
             and optometric evaluations collected during spring training
             2018 were obtained from 71 professional baseball players.
             Pitch-level data from Trackman 3D Doppler radar were
             obtained from these players during the subsequent season and
             used to generate batting propensity scores for swinging at
             pitches out of the strike zone (O-Swing), swinging at
             pitches in the strike zone (Z-Swing), and swinging at, but
             missing pitches in the strike zone (Z-Miss). Nested
             regression models to tested which evaluation(s) best
             predicted standardised plate discipline scores as well as
             batters’ highest attained league levels during the season.
             Results indicated that visual evaluations relying on eye
             tracking (smooth pursuit accuracy and oculomotor processing
             speed) significantly predicted the highest attained league
             level andpropensity scores associated with O-Swing and
             Z-Swing, but not Z-Miss. These exploratory findings indicate
             that batters with superior visual and oculomotor abilities
             are more discerning at the plate. These results provide new
             information about the role of vision in baseball
             batting.},
   Doi = {10.1080/24748668.2020.1777819},
   Key = {fds350574}
}

@article{fds349666,
   Author = {Gamboa, OL and Brito, A and Abzug, Z and D'Arbeloff, T and Beynel, L and Wing, EA and Dannhauer, M and Palmer, H and Hilbig, SA and Crowell, CA and Liu, S and Donaldson, R and Cabeza, R and Davis, SW and Peterchev, AV and Sommer, MA and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Application of long-interval paired-pulse transcranial
             magnetic stimulation to motion-sensitive visual cortex does
             not lead to changes in motion discrimination.},
   Journal = {Neurosci Lett},
   Volume = {730},
   Pages = {135022},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135022},
   Abstract = {The perception of visual motion is dependent on a set of
             occipitotemporal regions that are readily accessible to
             neuromodulation. The current study tested if paired-pulse
             Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (ppTMS) could modulate
             motion perception by stimulating the occipital cortex as
             participants viewed near-threshold motion dot stimuli. In
             this sham-controlled study, fifteen subjects completed two
             sessions. On the first visit, resting motor threshold (RMT)
             was assessed, and participants performed an adaptive
             direction discrimination task to determine individual motion
             sensitivity. During the second visit, subjects performed the
             task with three difficulty levels as TMS pulses were
             delivered 150 and 50 ms prior to motion stimulus onset at
             120% RMT, under the logic that the cumulative inhibitory
             effect of these pulses would alter motion sensitivity. ppTMS
             was delivered at one of two locations: 3 cm dorsal and
             5 cm lateral to inion (scalp-based coordinate), or at the
             site of peak activation for "motion" according to the
             NeuroSynth fMRI database (meta-analytic coordinate). Sham
             stimulation was delivered on one-third of trials by tilting
             the coil 90°. Analyses showed no significant
             active-versus-sham effects of ppTMS when stimulation was
             delivered to the meta-analytic (p = 0.15) or scalp-based
             coordinates (p = 0.17), which were separated by 29 mm
             on average. Active-versus-sham stimulation differences did
             not interact with either stimulation location (p = 0.12)
             or difficulty (p = 0.33). These findings fail to support
             the hypothesis that long-interval ppTMS recruits inhibitory
             processes in motion-sensitive cortex but must be considered
             within the limited parameters used in this
             design.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135022},
   Key = {fds349666}
}

@article{fds366337,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Powers, JP and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on
             resting-state connectivity: A systematic
             review.},
   Journal = {Neuroimage},
   Volume = {211},
   Pages = {116596},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116596},
   Abstract = {The brain is organized into networks that reorganize
             dynamically in response to cognitive demands and exogenous
             stimuli. In recent years, repetitive transcranial magnetic
             stimulation (rTMS) has gained increasing use as a
             noninvasive means to modulate cortical physiology, with
             effects both proximal to the stimulation site and in distal
             areas that are intrinsically connected to the proximal
             target. In light of these network-level neuromodulatory
             effects, there has been a rapid growth in studies attempting
             to leverage information about network connectivity to
             improve neuromodulatory control and intervention outcomes.
             However, the mechanisms-of-action of rTMS on network-level
             effects remain poorly understood and is based primarily on
             heuristics from proximal stimulation findings. To help
             bridge this gap, the current paper presents a systematic
             review of 33 rTMS studies with baseline and post-rTMS
             measures of fMRI resting-state functional connectivity
             (RSFC). Literature synthesis revealed variability across
             studies in stimulation parameters, studied populations, and
             connectivity analysis methodology. Despite this variability,
             it is observed that active rTMS induces significant changes
             on RSFC, but the prevalent low-frequency-inhibition/high-frequency-facilitation
             heuristic endorsed for proximal rTMS effects does not fully
             describe distal connectivity findings. This review also
             points towards other important considerations, including
             that the majority of rTMS-induced changes were found outside
             the stimulated functional network, suggesting that rTMS
             effects tend to spread across networks. Future studies may
             therefore wish to adopt conventions and systematic
             frameworks, such as the Yeo functional connectivity
             parcellation atlas adopted here, to better characterize
             network-level effect that contribute to the efficacy of
             these rapidly developing noninvasive interventions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116596},
   Key = {fds366337}
}

@article{fds349549,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Davis, SW and Crowell, CA and Dannhauer, M and Lim, W and Palmer, H and Hilbig, SA and Brito, A and Hile, C and Luber, B and Lisanby,
             SH and Peterchev, AV and Cabeza, R and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Site-Specific Effects of Online rTMS during a Working Memory
             Task in Healthy Older Adults.},
   Journal = {Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {5},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050255},
   Abstract = {The process of manipulating information within working
             memory is central to many cognitive functions, but also
             declines rapidly in old age. Improving this process could
             markedly enhance the health-span in older adults. The
             current pre-registered, randomized and placebo-controlled
             study tested the potential of online repetitive transcranial
             magnetic stimulation (rTMS) applied at 5 Hz over the left
             lateral parietal cortex to enhance working memory
             manipulation in healthy elderly adults. rTMS was applied,
             while participants performed a delayed-response
             alphabetization task with two individually titrated levels
             of difficulty. Coil placement and stimulation amplitude were
             calculated from fMRI activation maps combined with electric
             field modeling on an individual-subject basis in order to
             standardize dosing at the targeted cortical location.
             Contrary to the a priori hypothesis, active rTMS
             significantly decreased accuracy relative to sham, and only
             in the hardest difficulty level. When compared to the
             results from our previous study, in which rTMS was applied
             over the left prefrontal cortex, we found equivalent effect
             sizes but opposite directionality suggesting a site-specific
             effect of rTMS. These results demonstrate engagement of
             cortical working memory processing using a novel TMS
             targeting approach, while also providing prescriptions for
             future studies seeking to enhance memory through
             rTMS.},
   Doi = {10.3390/brainsci10050255},
   Key = {fds349549}
}

@article{fds347371,
   Author = {Burris, K and Liu, S and Appelbaum, L},
   Title = {Visual-motor expertise in athletes: Insights from
             semiparametric modelling of 2317 athletes tested on the Nike
             SPARQ Sensory Station.},
   Journal = {J Sports Sci},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {320-329},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2019.1698090},
   Abstract = {Elite athletes not only run faster, hit harder, and jump
             higher, but also see and react better. However, the specific
             visual-motor skills that differentiate high-achieving
             athletes are still not well understood. In this paper we
             examine 2317 athletes (1871 male) tested on the Nike SPARQ
             Sensory Station, a digital test battery measuring visual,
             perceptual and motor skills relevant for sports performance.
             We develop a multivariate Gaussian transformation model to
             robustly estimate visual-motor differences by level, gender,
             and sport type. Results demonstrate that visual-motor
             performance is superior for athletes at higher levels, with
             males faster at near-far eye movements and females faster at
             eye-hand reaction times. Interestingly, athletes who play
             interceptive sports such as baseball and tennis exhibit
             better measures of visual clarity, contrast sensitivity and
             simple reaction time, while athletes from strategic sports
             like soccer and basketball have higher measures of spatial
             working memory. These findings provide quantitative evidence
             of domain-specific visual expertise in athletes.},
   Doi = {10.1080/02640414.2019.1698090},
   Key = {fds347371}
}

@article{fds351160,
   Author = {Wilkins, L and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {An early review of stroboscopic visual training: insights,
             challenges and accomplishments to guide future
             studies},
   Journal = {International Review of Sport and Exercise
             Psychology},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {65-80},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2019.1582081},
   Abstract = {Stroboscopic visual training (SVT) is a form of training in
             which an individual practices a task under intermittent
             visual conditions with the intention of enhancing subsequent
             performance under normal visual conditions. Training with
             stroboscopic devices is theorized to improve important
             visual, perceptual, and cognitive skills, which in turn
             transfers to enhanced sporting performance. Indeed, while
             there is an abundance of anecdotal evidence suggesting
             benefits of strobe training, empirical evidence is rarer and
             less conclusive. This lack of clarity is due, in part, to
             the challenging methodological issues faced when conducting
             experimental vision training studies in applied contexts.
             The present paper is an early review of the research to date
             with a focus on the key methodological decisions, such as
             the training and testing protocols employed, participant
             samples and control groups used, and practical
             considerations that enable such training in applied
             settings. Whilst still at an early stage, the existing
             studies point to SVT enhancing some aspects of foveal visual
             sensitivity and visual motor control, with notable benefits
             for some athletic tasks. Such improvements could have
             implications not just in sport, but in domains such as
             rehabilitation, education, and motor vehicle
             safety.},
   Doi = {10.1080/1750984X.2019.1582081},
   Key = {fds351160}
}

@article{fds348936,
   Author = {Cox, ML and Deng, Z-D and Palmer, H and Watts, A and Beynel, L and Young,
             JR and Lisanby, SH and Migaly, J and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Utilizing transcranial direct current stimulation to enhance
             laparoscopic technical skills training: A randomized
             controlled trial.},
   Journal = {Brain Stimul},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {863-872},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2020.03.009},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND: Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)
             is a non-invasive brain stimulation technique that delivers
             constant, low electrical current resulting in changes to
             cortical excitability. Prior work suggests it may enhance
             motor learning giving it the potential to augment surgical
             technical skill acquisition. OBJECTIVES: The aim of this
             study was to test the efficacy of tDCS, coupled with motor
             skill training, to accelerate laparoscopic skill acquisition
             in a pre-registered (NCT03083483), double-blind and
             placebo-controlled study. We hypothesized that relative to
             sham tDCS, active tDCS would accelerate the development of
             laparoscopic technical skills, as measured by the
             Fundamentals of Laparoscopic Surgery (FLS) Peg Transfer task
             quantitative metrics. METHODS: In this study, sixty subjects
             (mean age 22.7 years with 42 females) were randomized into
             sham or active tDCS in either bilateral primary motor cortex
             (bM1) or supplementary motor area (SMA) electrode
             configurations. All subjects practiced the FLS Peg Transfer
             Task during six 20-min training blocks, which were preceded
             and followed by a single trial pre-test and post-test. The
             primary outcome was changes in laparoscopic skill
             performance over time, quantified by group differences in
             completion time from pre-test to post-test and learning
             curves developed from a calculated score accounting for
             errors. RESULTS: Learning curves calculated over the six
             20-min training blocks showed significantly greater
             improvement in performance for the bM1 group than the sham
             group (t = 2.07, p = 0.039), with the bM1 group
             achieving approximately the same amount of improvement in 4
             blocks compared to the 6 blocks required of the sham group.
             The SMA group also showed greater mean improvement than
             sham, but exhibited more variable learning performance and
             differences relative to sham were not significant
             (t = 0.85, p = 0.400). A significant main effect was
             present for pre-test versus post-test times (F = 133.2,
             p < 0.001), with lower completion times at post-test,
             however these did not significantly differ for the training
             groups. CONCLUSION: Laparoscopic skill training with active
             bilateral tDCS exhibited significantly greater learning
             relative to sham. The potential for tDCS to enhance the
             training of surgical skills, therefore, merits further
             investigation to determine if these preliminary results may
             be replicated and extended.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.brs.2020.03.009},
   Key = {fds348936}
}

@article{fds349143,
   Author = {Young, JR and Smani, SA and Mischel, NA and Kritzer, MD and Appelbaum,
             LG and Patkar, AA},
   Title = {Non-invasive brain stimulation modalities for the treatment
             and prevention of opioid use disorder: a systematic review
             of the literature.},
   Journal = {J Addict Dis},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {186-199},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10550887.2020.1736756},
   Abstract = {The U.S. is currently facing an unprecedented epidemic of
             opioid-related deaths. Despite the efficacy of the current
             treatments for opioid use disorder (OUD), including
             psychosocial interventions and medication-assisted therapy
             (MAT), many patients remain treatment-resistant and at high
             risk for overdose. A potential augmentation strategy
             includes the use of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS)
             techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS),
             transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), and
             auricular vagus nerve stimulation (aVNS). These approaches
             may have therapeutic benefits by directly or indirectly
             modulating the neurocircuitry affected in OUD. In this
             review, we evaluate the available studies on NIBS in the
             context of OUD withdrawal and detoxification, maintenance,
             and cravings, while also considering analgesia and safety
             concerns. In the context of opioid withdrawal and
             detoxification, a percutaneous form of aVNS has positive
             results in open-label trials, but has not yet been tested
             against sham. No randomized studies have reported on the
             safety and efficacy of NIBS specifically for maintenance
             treatment in OUD. TMS and tDCS have demonstrated effects on
             cravings, although published studies were limited by small
             sample sizes. NIBS may play a role in reducing exposure to
             opioids and the risk of developing OUD, as demonstrated by
             studies using tDCS in an experimental pain condition and TMS
             in a post-operative setting. Overall, while the preliminary
             evidence and safety for NIBS in the prevention and treatment
             of OUD appears promising, further research is needed with
             larger sample sizes, placebo control, and objective
             biomarkers as outcome measures before strong conclusions can
             be drawn.},
   Doi = {10.1080/10550887.2020.1736756},
   Key = {fds349143}
}

@article{fds346303,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Appelbaum, LG and Luber, B and Crowell, CA and Hilbig, SA and Lim, W and Nguyen, D and Chrapliwy, NA and Davis, SW and Cabeza, R and Lisanby, SH and Deng, Z-D},
   Title = {Effects of online repetitive transcranial magnetic
             stimulation (rTMS) on cognitive processing: A meta-analysis
             and recommendations for future studies.},
   Journal = {Neurosci Biobehav Rev},
   Volume = {107},
   Pages = {47-58},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.018},
   Abstract = {Online repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS),
             applied while subjects are performing a task, is widely used
             to disrupt brain regions underlying cognition. However,
             online rTMS has also induced "paradoxical enhancement".
             Given the rapid proliferation of this approach, it is
             crucial to develop a better understanding of how online
             stimulation influences cognition, and the optimal parameters
             to achieve desired effects. To accomplish this goal, a
             quantitative meta-analysis was performed with random-effects
             models fitted to reaction time (RT) and accuracy data. The
             final dataset included 126 studies published between 1998
             and 2016, with 244 total effects for reaction times, and 202
             for accuracy. Meta-analytically, rTMS at 10 Hz and 20 Hz
             disrupted accuracy for attention, executive, language,
             memory, motor, and perception domains, while no effects were
             found with 1 Hz or 5 Hz. Stimulation applied at and 10
             and 20 Hz slowed down RTs in attention and perception
             tasks. No performance enhancement was found. Meta-regression
             analysis showed that fMRI-guided targeting and short
             inter-trial intervals are associated with increased
             disruptive effects with rTMS.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.018},
   Key = {fds346303}
}

@article{fds336042,
   Author = {Teel, EF and Marshall, SW and Appelbaum, LG and Battaglini, CL and Carneiro, KA and Guskiewicz, KM and Register-Mihalik, JK and Mihalik,
             JP},
   Title = {A Randomized Controlled Trial Investigating the Feasibility
             and Adherence to an Aerobic Training Program in Healthy
             Individuals.},
   Journal = {J Sport Rehabil},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {692-698},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsr.2018-0007},
   Abstract = {CONTEXT: Concussion management is moving from passive rest
             strategies to active interventions, including aerobic
             exercise therapy. Little information is available regarding
             the feasibility and adherence of these programs. OBJECTIVES:
             To determine whether an aerobic exercise training program
             intended for rehabilitation in people with concussion is
             feasible. Healthy, nonconcussed subjects were studied in
             this phase 1 trial. DESIGN: Phase 1 parallel-group,
             randomized controlled trial in a sample of healthy
             (nonconcussed), recreationally active university students.
             SETTING: Laboratory. PATIENTS: 40 healthy university
             students. METHODS: Participants were equally randomized to
             acute concussion therapy intervention (ACTIVE) training or
             nontraining groups. All participants completed maximal
             cardiopulmonary exercise tests on a stationary cycle
             ergometer at 2 test sessions approximately 14 days apart.
             During this 2-week study period, ACTIVE training
             participants completed six 30-minute cycling sessions,
             progressing from 60% to 80% of the participant's
             individualized maximal oxygen consumption. A subset of
             participants (NACTIVE = 12, Nnontraining = 11) wore physical
             activity monitors throughout the 2-week study period. MAIN
             OUTCOMES MEASURES: Study protocol and randomization
             effectiveness, exercise safety and adherence, and
             progressive intensity of the ACTIVE training procedures.
             RESULTS: No adverse events occurred during any exercise
             sessions. Twelve ACTIVE training participants (60%)
             completed all training sessions, and every participant
             completed at least 4 sessions. Heart rate increased
             throughout the training period (P < .001), but symptom
             changes and training adherence remained stable despite the
             progressively increasing workload. ACTIVE training
             participants completed approximately 30 additional minutes
             of physical activity on training sessions days, although
             that was not statistically significant (P = .20).
             CONCLUSIONS: University-aged students were adherent to the
             ACTIVE training protocol. Future research should investigate
             the safety and feasibility of aerobic training programs in
             acutely concussed individuals to determine their
             appropriateness as a clinical rehabilitation
             strategy.},
   Doi = {10.1123/jsr.2018-0007},
   Key = {fds336042}
}

@article{fds343408,
   Author = {Addicott, MA and Luber, B and Nguyen, D and Palmer, H and Lisanby, SH and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Low- and High-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic
             Stimulation Effects on Resting-State Functional Connectivity
             Between the Postcentral Gyrus and the Insula.},
   Journal = {Brain Connect},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {322-328},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/brain.2018.0652},
   Abstract = {The insular cortex supports the conscious awareness of
             physical and emotional sensations, and the ability to
             modulate the insula could have important clinical
             applications in psychiatry. Repetitive transcranial magnetic
             stimulation (rTMS) uses transient magnetic fields to induce
             electrical currents in the superficial cortex. Given its
             deep location in the brain, the insula may not be directly
             stimulated by rTMS; however, rTMS may modulate the insula
             via its functional connections with superficial cortical
             regions. Furthermore, low- versus high-frequency rTMS is
             thought to have opposing effects on cortical excitability,
             and the present study investigated these effects on brain
             activity and functional connectivity with the insula.
             Separate groups of healthy participants (n = 14 per
             group) received low (1 Hz)- or high (10 Hz)-frequency
             rTMS in five daily sessions to the right postcentral gyrus,
             a superficial region known to be functionally connected to
             the insula. Resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) was
             measured pre- and post-rTMS. Both 1 and 10 Hz rTMS
             increased RSFC between the right postcentral gyrus and the
             left insula. These results suggest that low- and
             high-frequency rTMS has similar long-term effects on brain
             activity and RSFC. However, given the lack of difference, we
             cannot exclude the possibility that these effects are simply
             due to a nonspecific effect. Given this limitation, these
             unexpected results underscore the need for acoustic- and
             stimulation-matched sham control conditions in rTMS
             research.},
   Doi = {10.1089/brain.2018.0652},
   Key = {fds343408}
}

@article{fds342292,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Davis, SW and Crowell, CA and Hilbig, SA and Lim, W and Nguyen, D and Palmer, H and Brito, A and Peterchev, AV and Luber, B and Lisanby, SH and Cabeza, R and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Online repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation during
             working memory in younger and older adults: A randomized
             within-subject comparison.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e0213707},
   Year = {2019},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213707},
   Abstract = {Working memory is the ability to perform mental operations
             on information that is stored in a flexible, limited
             capacity buffer. The ability to manipulate information in
             working memory is central to many aspects of human
             cognition, but also declines with healthy aging. Given the
             profound importance of such working memory manipulation
             abilities, there is a concerted effort towards developing
             approaches to improve them. The current study tested the
             capacity to enhance working memory manipulation with online
             repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in healthy
             young and older adults. Online high frequency (5Hz)
             repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied
             over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to test the
             hypothesis that active repetitive transcranial magnetic
             stimulation would lead to significant improvements in memory
             recall accuracy compared to sham stimulation, and that these
             effects would be most pronounced in working memory
             manipulation conditions with the highest cognitive demand in
             both young and older adults. Repetitive transcranial
             magnetic stimulation was applied while participants were
             performing a delayed response alphabetization task with
             three individually-titrated levels of difficulty. The left
             dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was identified by combining
             electric field modeling to individualized functional
             magnetic resonance imaging activation maps and was targeted
             during the experiment using stereotactic neuronavigation
             with real-time robotic guidance, allowing optimal coil
             placement during the stimulation. As no accuracy differences
             were found between young and older adults, the results from
             both groups were collapsed. Subsequent analyses revealed
             that active stimulation significantly increased accuracy
             relative to sham stimulation, but only for the hardest
             condition. These results point towards further investigation
             of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for memory
             enhancement focusing on high difficulty conditions as those
             most likely to exhibit benefits.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0213707},
   Key = {fds342292}
}

@article{fds350633,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Deng, L and Crowell, CA and Dannhauer, M and Palmer, H and Hilbig, S and Peterchev, AV and Luber, B and Lisanby, SH and Cabeza, R and Appelbaum, LG and Davis, SW},
   Title = {Structural controllability predicts functional patterns and
             brain stimulation benefits associated with working
             memory},
   Year = {2019},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/794388},
   Abstract = {<h4>Summary</h4> The brain is an inherently dynamic system,
             and much work has focused on the ability to modify neural
             activity through both local perturbations and changes in the
             function of global network ensembles. Network
             controllability is a recent concept in network science that
             purports to predict the influence of individual cortical
             sites on global network states and state changes, thereby
             creating a unifying account of local influences on global
             brain dynamics. Here, we present an integrated set of
             multimodal brain–behavior relationships, acquired from
             functional magnetic resonance imaging during a transcranial
             magnetic stimulation intervention, that demonstrate how
             network controllability influences network function, as well
             as behavior. This work helps to outline a clear technique
             for integrating structural network topology and functional
             activity to predict the influence of a potential stimulation
             target on subsequent behaviors and prescribes next steps
             towards predicting neuromodulatory and behavioral responses
             after brain stimulation. <h4>Highlights</h4> - This study
             tested the strength of network controllability using fMRI
             and rTMS - Controllability correlates with functional
             modulation of working memory demand load - Controllability
             is also correlated with the memory improvement from applied
             rTMS - These findings link network control theory with
             physiology and behavior. <h4>In brief</h4> Beynel et al.
             show that the benefits of functionally targeted brain
             stimulation on working memory performance can be predicted
             by network control properties at the stimulated site.
             Structural controllability and functional activity
             independently predict this cognitive benefit. <h4>Author
             Contributions</h4> Conceptualization & Methodology: L.B,
             S.W.D., B.L., R.C., L.G.A.; Investigation: L.B., L.D.,
             S.W.D., C.A.C., M.D., H.P., S.H.; Writing—Original Draft:
             L.B., L.D., S.W.D.; Writing—Review & Editing: L.B., L.D.,
             S.W.D., L.G.A., A.V.P.; Funding Acquisition: S.W.D., R.C.,
             B.L., S.H.L., A.V.P.; Resources: L.G.A., B.L., R.C.;
             Supervision: L.G.A., S.W.D.},
   Doi = {10.1101/794388},
   Key = {fds350633}
}

@article{fds339602,
   Author = {Addicott, MA and Daughters, SB and Strauman, TJ and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Distress tolerance to auditory feedback and functional
             connectivity with the auditory cortex.},
   Journal = {Psychiatry Research. Neuroimaging},
   Volume = {282},
   Pages = {1-10},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.10.003},
   Abstract = {Distress tolerance is the capacity to withstand negative
             affective states in pursuit of a goal. Low distress
             tolerance may bias an individual to avoid or escape
             experiences that induce affective distress, but the neural
             mechanisms underlying the bottom-up generation of distress
             and its relationship to behavioral avoidance are poorly
             understood. During a neuroimaging scan, healthy participants
             completed a mental arithmetic task with easy and distress
             phases, which differed in cognitive demands and positive
             versus negative auditory feedback. Then, participants were
             given the opportunity to continue playing the distress phase
             for a financial bonus and were allowed to quit at any time.
             The persistence duration was the measure of distress
             tolerance. The easy and distress phases activated auditory
             cortices and fronto-parietal regions. A task-based
             functional connectivity analysis using the left secondary
             auditory cortex (i.e., planum temporale) as the seed region
             revealed stronger connectivity to fronto-parietal regions
             and anterior insula during the distress phase. The
             distress-related connectivity between the seed region and
             the left anterior insula was negatively correlated with
             distress tolerance. The results provide initial evidence of
             the role of the anterior insula as a mediating link between
             the bottom-up generation of affective distress and top-down
             behavioral avoidance of distress.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.10.003},
   Key = {fds339602}
}

@article{fds340441,
   Author = {Davis, SW and Crowell, CA and Beynel, L and Deng, L and Lakhlani, D and Hilbig, SA and Lim, W and Nguyen, D and Peterchev, AV and Luber, BM and Lisanby, SH and Appelbaum, LG and Cabeza, R},
   Title = {Complementary topology of maintenance and manipulation brain
             networks in working memory.},
   Journal = {Scientific Reports},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {17827},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-35887-2},
   Abstract = {Working memory (WM) is assumed to consist of a process that
             sustains memory representations in an active state
             (maintenance) and a process that operates on these activated
             representations (manipulation). We examined evidence for two
             distinct, concurrent cognitive functions supporting
             maintenance and manipulation abilities by testing brain
             activity as participants performed a WM alphabetization
             task. Maintenance was investigated by varying the number of
             letters held in WM and manipulation by varying the number of
             moves required to sort the list alphabetically. We found
             that both maintenance and manipulation demand had
             significant effects on behavior that were associated with
             different cortical regions: maintenance was associated with
             bilateral prefrontal and left parietal cortex, and
             manipulation with right parietal activity, a link that is
             consistent with the role of parietal cortex in symbolic
             computations. Both structural and functional architecture of
             these systems suggested that these cognitive functions are
             supported by two dissociable brain networks. Critically,
             maintenance and manipulation functional networks became
             increasingly segregated with increasing demand, an effect
             that was positively associated with individual WM ability.
             These results provide evidence that network segregation may
             act as a protective mechanism to enable successful
             performance under increasing WM demand.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41598-018-35887-2},
   Key = {fds340441}
}

@article{fds340609,
   Author = {Teel, EF and Register-Mihalik, JK and Appelbaum, LG and Battaglini,
             CL and Carneiro, KA and Guskiewicz, KM and Marshall, SW and Mihalik,
             JP},
   Title = {Randomized Controlled Trial Evaluating Aerobic Training and
             Common Sport-Related Concussion Outcomes in Healthy
             Participants.},
   Journal = {Journal of Athletic Training},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {1156-1165},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4085/1062-6050-7-18},
   Abstract = {CONTEXT: Aerobic exercise interventions are increasingly
             being prescribed for concussion rehabilitation, but whether
             aerobic training protocols influence clinical concussion
             diagnosis and management assessments is unknown. OBJECTIVE:
             To investigate the effects of a brief aerobic exercise
             intervention on clinical concussion outcomes in healthy,
             active participants. DESIGN: Randomized controlled clinical
             trial. SETTING: Laboratory. PATIENTS OR OTHER PARTICIPANTS:
             Healthy (uninjured) participants (n = 40) who exercised ≥3
             times/week. INTERVENTION(S): Participants were randomized
             into the acute concussion therapy intervention (ACTIVE)
             training or nontraining group. All participants completed
             symptom, cognitive, balance, and vision assessments during 2
             test sessions approximately 14 days apart. Participants
             randomized to ACTIVE training completed six 30-minute
             exercise sessions that progressed from 60% to 80% of
             individualized maximal oxygen consumption (V˙o2max) across
             test sessions, while the nontraining group received no
             intervention. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE(S): The CNS Vital Signs
             standardized scores, Vestibular/Ocular Motor Screening
             near-point convergence distance (cm), and Graded Symptom
             Checklist, Balance Error Scoring System, and Standardized
             Assessment of Concussion total scores. RESULTS: An
             interaction effect was found for total symptom score ( P =
             .01); the intervention group had improved symptom scores
             between sessions (session 1: 5.1 ± 5.8; session 2: 1.9 ±
             3.6). Cognitive flexibility, executive functioning,
             reasoning, and total symptom score outcomes were better but
             composite memory, verbal memory, and near-point convergence
             distance scores were worse at the second session (all P
             values < .05). However, few changes exceeded the 80%
             reliable change indices calculated for this study, and
             effect sizes were generally small to negligible.
             CONCLUSIONS: A brief aerobic training protocol had few
             meaningful effects on clinical concussion assessment in
             healthy participants, suggesting that current
             concussion-diagnostic and -assessment tools remain
             clinically stable in response to aerobic exercise training.
             This provides normative data for future researchers, who
             should further evaluate the effect of ACTIVE training on
             clinical outcomes among concussed populations. TRIAL
             REGISTRATION NUMBER: ClinicalTrials.gov :
             NCT02872480.},
   Doi = {10.4085/1062-6050-7-18},
   Key = {fds340609}
}

@article{fds332131,
   Author = {Burris, K and Vittetoe, K and Ramger, B and Suresh, S and Tokdar, ST and Reiter, JP and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Sensorimotor abilities predict on-field performance in
             professional baseball.},
   Journal = {Scientific Reports},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {116},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-18565-7},
   Abstract = {Baseball players must be able to see and react in an
             instant, yet it is hotly debated whether superior
             performance is associated with superior sensorimotor
             abilities. In this study, we compare sensorimotor abilities,
             measured through 8 psychomotor tasks comprising the Nike
             Sensory Station assessment battery, and game statistics in a
             sample of 252 professional baseball players to evaluate the
             links between sensorimotor skills and on-field performance.
             For this purpose, we develop a series of Bayesian
             hierarchical latent variable models enabling us to compare
             statistics across professional baseball leagues. Within this
             framework, we find that sensorimotor abilities are
             significant predictors of on-base percentage, walk rate and
             strikeout rate, accounting for age, position, and league. We
             find no such relationship for either slugging percentage or
             fielder-independent pitching. The pattern of results
             suggests performance contributions from both visual-sensory
             and visual-motor abilities and indicates that sensorimotor
             screenings may be useful for player scouting.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41598-017-18565-7},
   Key = {fds332131}
}

@article{fds336043,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Erickson, G},
   Title = {Sports vision training: A review of the state-of-the-art in
             digital training techniques},
   Journal = {International Review of Sport and Exercise
             Psychology},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {160-189},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2016.1266376},
   Abstract = {Athletes need excellent vision to perform well in their
             sports, and many athletes have turned to vision training
             programs as a way to augment their traditional training
             regimen. The growing practice of ‘sports vision
             training’ relies on the notion that practice with
             demanding visual perceptual, cognitive, or oculomotor tasks
             can improve the ability to process and respond to what is
             seen, thereby improving sport performance. This enterprise
             is not necessarily new, but has been advanced greatly in the
             past few years by new digital technology that can be
             deployed during natural training activities, by
             perceptual-learning-inspired training programs, and by
             virtual reality simulations that can recreate and augment
             sporting contexts to promote certain sports-specific visual
             and cognitive abilities. These improved abilities may, in
             turn, instill a competitive advantage on the playing field,
             underscoring the potential value of these approaches. This
             article reviews emerging approaches, technologies and trends
             in sports vision training. Where available, critical review
             of supporting research is provided.},
   Doi = {10.1080/1750984X.2016.1266376},
   Key = {fds336043}
}

@article{fds324820,
   Author = {Klemish, D and Ramger, B and Vittetoe, K and Reiter, JP and Tokdar, ST and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Visual abilities distinguish pitchers from hitters in
             professional baseball.},
   Journal = {J Sports Sci},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {171-179},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2017.1288296},
   Abstract = {This study aimed to evaluate the possibility that
             differences in sensorimotor abilities exist between hitters
             and pitchers in a large cohort of baseball players of
             varying levels of experience. Secondary data analysis was
             performed on 9 sensorimotor tasks comprising the Nike
             Sensory Station assessment battery. Bayesian hierarchical
             regression modelling was applied to test for differences
             between pitchers and hitters in data from 566 baseball
             players (112 high school, 85 college, 369 professional)
             collected at 20 testing centres. Explanatory variables
             including height, handedness, eye dominance, concussion
             history, and player position were modelled along with age
             curves using basis regression splines. Regression analyses
             revealed better performance for hitters relative to pitchers
             at the professional level in the visual clarity and depth
             perception tasks, but these differences did not exist at the
             high school or college levels. No significant differences
             were observed in the other 7 measures of sensorimotor
             capabilities included in the test battery, and no systematic
             biases were found between the testing centres. These
             findings, indicating that professional-level hitters have
             better visual acuity and depth perception than
             professional-level pitchers, affirm the notion that highly
             experienced athletes have differing perceptual skills.
             Findings are discussed in relation to deliberate practice
             theory.},
   Doi = {10.1080/02640414.2017.1288296},
   Key = {fds324820}
}

@article{fds332130,
   Author = {Rao, HM and Khanna, R and Zielinski, DJ and Lu, Y and Clements, JM and Potter, ND and Sommer, MA and Kopper, R and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Sensorimotor Learning during a Marksmanship Task in
             Immersive Virtual Reality.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {58},
   Pages = {58},
   Publisher = {Frontiers Media},
   Year = {2018},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00058},
   Abstract = {Sensorimotor learning refers to improvements that occur
             through practice in the performance of sensory-guided motor
             behaviors. Leveraging novel technical capabilities of an
             immersive virtual environment, we probed the component
             kinematic processes that mediate sensorimotor learning.
             Twenty naïve subjects performed a simulated marksmanship
             task modeled after Olympic Trap Shooting standards. We
             measured movement kinematics and shooting performance as
             participants practiced 350 trials while receiving
             trial-by-trial feedback about shooting success.
             Spatiotemporal analysis of motion tracking elucidated the
             ballistic and refinement phases of hand movements. We found
             systematic changes in movement kinematics that accompanied
             improvements in shot accuracy during training, though
             reaction and response times did not change over blocks. In
             particular, we observed longer, slower, and more precise
             ballistic movements that replaced effort spent on
             corrections and refinement. Collectively, these results
             leverage developments in immersive virtual reality
             technology to quantify and compare the kinematics of
             movement during early learning of full-body sensorimotor
             orienting.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00058},
   Key = {fds332130}
}

@article{fds320202,
   Author = {Devyatko, D and Appelbaum, LG and Mitroff, SR},
   Title = {A Common Mechanism for Perceptual Reversals in
             Motion-Induced Blindness, the Troxler Effect, and Perceptual
             Filling-In.},
   Journal = {Perception},
   Volume = {46},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {50-77},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0301006616672577},
   Abstract = {Several striking visual phenomena involve a physically
             present stimulus that alternates between being perceived and
             being "invisible." For example, motion-induced blindness,
             the Troxler effect, and perceptual filling-in all consist of
             subjective alternations where an item repeatedly changes
             from being seen to unseen. In the present study, we explored
             whether these three specific visual phenomena share any
             commonalities in their alternation rates and patterns to
             better understand the mechanisms of each. Data from 69
             individuals revealed moderate to strong correlations across
             the three phenomena for the number of perceptual
             disappearances and the accumulated duration of the
             disappearances. Importantly, these effects were not
             correlated with eye movement patterns (saccades) assessed
             through eye tracking, differences in motion sensitivity as
             indexed by dot coherence and speed perception thresholds, or
             simple reaction time abilities. Principal component analyses
             revealed a single component that explained 67% of the
             variance for the number of perceptual reversals and 60% for
             the accumulated duration of the disappearances. The temporal
             dynamics of illusory disappearances was also compared for
             each phenomenon, and normalized durations of disappearances
             were well fit by a gamma distribution with similar shape
             parameters for each phenomenon, suggesting that they may be
             driven by a single oscillatory mechanism.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0301006616672577},
   Key = {fds320202}
}

@article{fds322751,
   Author = {van den Berg, B and Appelbaum, LG and Clark, K and Lorist, MM and Woldorff, MG},
   Title = {Visual search performance is predicted by both prestimulus
             and poststimulus electrical brain activity.},
   Journal = {Scientific Reports},
   Volume = {6},
   Pages = {37718},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep37718},
   Abstract = {An individual's performance on cognitive and perceptual
             tasks varies considerably across time and circumstances. We
             investigated neural mechanisms underlying such performance
             variability using regression-based analyses to examine
             trial-by-trial relationships between response times (RTs)
             and different facets of electrical brain activity. Thirteen
             participants trained five days on a color-popout
             visual-search task, with EEG recorded on days one and five.
             The task was to find a color-popout target ellipse in a
             briefly presented array of ellipses and discriminate its
             orientation. Later within a session, better preparatory
             attention (reflected by less prestimulus Alpha-band
             oscillatory activity) and better poststimulus early visual
             responses (reflected by larger sensory N1 waves) correlated
             with faster RTs. However, N1 amplitudes decreased by half
             throughout each session, suggesting adoption of a more
             efficient search strategy within a session. Additionally,
             fast RTs were preceded by earlier and larger lateralized
             N2pc waves, reflecting faster and stronger attentional
             orienting to the targets. Finally, SPCN waves associated
             with target-orientation discrimination were smaller for fast
             RTs in the first but not the fifth session, suggesting
             optimization with practice. Collectively, these results
             delineate variations in visual search processes that change
             over an experimental session, while also pointing to
             cortical mechanisms underlying performance in visual
             search.},
   Doi = {10.1038/srep37718},
   Key = {fds322751}
}

@article{fds315407,
   Author = {Donohue, SE and Appelbaum, LG and McKay, CC and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {The neural dynamics of stimulus and response conflict
             processing as a function of response complexity and task
             demands.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologia},
   Volume = {84},
   Pages = {14-28},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0028-3932},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12003 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Both stimulus and response conflict can disrupt behavior by
             slowing response times and decreasing accuracy. Although
             several neural activations have been associated with
             conflict processing, it is unclear how specific any of these
             are to the type of stimulus conflict or the amount of
             response conflict. Here, we recorded electrical brain
             activity, while manipulating the type of stimulus conflict
             in the task (spatial [Flanker] versus semantic [Stroop]) and
             the amount of response conflict (two versus four response
             choices). Behaviorally, responses were slower to incongruent
             versus congruent stimuli across all task and response types,
             along with overall slowing for higher response-mapping
             complexity. The earliest incongruency-related neural effect
             was a short-duration frontally-distributed negativity at
             ~200 ms that was only present in the Flanker
             spatial-conflict task. At longer latencies, the classic
             fronto-central incongruency-related negativity 'N(inc)' was
             observed for all conditions, but was larger and ~100 ms
             longer in duration with more response options. Further, the
             onset of the motor-related lateralized readiness potential
             (LRP) was earlier for the two vs. four response sets,
             indicating that smaller response sets enabled faster
             motor-response preparation. The late positive complex (LPC)
             was present in all conditions except the two-response Stroop
             task, suggesting this late conflict-related activity is not
             specifically related to task type or response-mapping
             complexity. Importantly, across tasks and conditions, the
             LRP onset at or before the conflict-related N(inc),
             indicating that motor preparation is a rapid, automatic
             process that interacts with the conflict-detection processes
             after it has begun. Together, these data highlight how
             different conflict-related processes operate in parallel and
             depend on both the cognitive demands of the task and the
             number of response options.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.035},
   Key = {fds315407}
}

@article{fds270437,
   Author = {San Martín and R and Appelbaum, LG and Huettel, SA and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Cortical Brain Activity Reflecting Attentional Biasing
             Toward Reward-Predicting Cues Covaries with Economic
             Decision-Making Performance.},
   Journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-11},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {1047-3211},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/12006 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Adaptive choice behavior depends critically on identifying
             and learning from outcome-predicting cues. We hypothesized
             that attention may be preferentially directed toward certain
             outcome-predicting cues. We studied this possibility by
             analyzing event-related potential (ERP) responses in humans
             during a probabilistic decision-making task. Participants
             viewed pairs of outcome-predicting visual cues and then
             chose to wager either a small (i.e., loss-minimizing) or
             large (i.e., gain-maximizing) amount of money. The cues were
             bilaterally presented, which allowed us to extract the
             relative neural responses to each cue by using a
             contralateral-versus-ipsilateral ERP contrast. We found an
             early lateralized ERP response, whose features matched the
             attention-shift-related N2pc component and whose amplitude
             scaled with the learned reward-predicting value of the cues
             as predicted by an attention-for-reward model. Consistently,
             we found a double dissociation involving the N2pc. Across
             participants, gain-maximization positively correlated with
             the N2pc amplitude to the most reliable gain-predicting cue,
             suggesting an attentional bias toward such cues. Conversely,
             loss-minimization was negatively correlated with the N2pc
             amplitude to the most reliable loss-predicting cue,
             suggesting an attentional avoidance toward such stimuli.
             These results indicate that learned stimulus-reward
             associations can influence rapid attention allocation, and
             that differences in this process are associated with
             individual differences in economic decision-making
             performance.},
   Doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhu160},
   Key = {fds270437}
}

@article{fds320203,
   Author = {Krasich, K and Ramger, B and Holton, L and Wang, L and Mitroff, SR and Gregory Appelbaum and L},
   Title = {Sensorimotor Learning in a Computerized Athletic Training
             Battery.},
   Journal = {J Mot Behav},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {401-412},
   Year = {2016},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2015.1113918},
   Abstract = {Sensorimotor abilities are crucial for performance in
             athletic, military, and other occupational activities, and
             there is great interest in understanding learning in these
             skills. Here, behavioral performance was measured over three
             days as twenty-seven participants practiced multiple
             sessions on the Nike SPARQ Sensory Station (Nike, Inc.,
             Beaverton, Oregon), a computerized visual and motor
             assessment battery. Wrist-worn actigraphy was recorded to
             monitor sleep-wake cycles. Significant learning was observed
             in tasks with high visuomotor control demands but not in
             tasks of visual sensitivity. Learning was primarily linear,
             with up to 60% improvement, but did not relate to sleep
             quality in this normal-sleeping population. These results
             demonstrate differences in the rate and capacity for
             learning across perceptual and motor domains, indicating
             potential targets for sensorimotor training
             interventions.},
   Doi = {10.1080/00222895.2015.1113918},
   Key = {fds320203}
}

@article{fds315408,
   Author = {Zielinski, DJ and Rao, HM and Potter, ND and Sommer, MA and Appelbaum,
             LG and Kopper, R},
   Title = {Evaluating the Effects of Image Persistence on Dynamic
             Target Acquisition in Low Frame Rate Virtual
             Environments},
   Journal = {3d User Interfaces (3dui), 2016 Ieee Symposium
             On},
   Pages = {133-140},
   Publisher = {IEEE},
   Editor = {BH Thomas and R Lindeman and M Marchal},
   Year = {2016},
   url = {http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/xpl/mostRecentIssue.jsp?punumber=7454633},
   Abstract = {User performance in virtual environments with degraded
             visual conditions due to low frame rates is an interesting
             area of inquiry. Visual content shown in a low frame rate
             simulation has the quality of the original image, but
             persists for an extended period until the next frame is
             displayed (so-called high persistence-HP). An alternative,
             called low persistence (LP), involves displaying the
             rendered frame for a single display frame and blanking the
             screen while waiting for the next frame to be generated.
             Previous research has evaluated the usefulness of the LP
             technique in low frame rate simulations during a static
             target acquisition task. To gain greater knowledge about the
             LP technique, we have conducted a user study to evaluate
             user performance and learning during a dynamic target
             acquisition task. The acquisition task was evaluated under a
             high frame rate, (60 fps) condition, a traditional low frame
             rate HP condition (10 fps), and the experimental low frame
             rate LP technique. The task involved the acquisition of
             targets moving along several different trajectories, modeled
             after a shotgun trap shooting task. The results of our study
             indicate the LP condition approaches high frame rate
             performance within certain classes of target trajectories.
             Interestingly we also see that learning is consistent across
             conditions, indicating that it may not always be necessary
             to train under a visually high frame rate system to learn a
             particular task. We discuss implications of using the LP
             technique to mitigate low frame rate issues as well as its
             potential usefulness for training in low frame rate virtual
             environments.},
   Doi = {10.1109/3DUI.2016.7460043},
   Key = {fds315408}
}

@article{fds270436,
   Author = {Wang, L and Krasich, K and Bel-Bahar, T and Hughes, L and Mitroff, SR and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {Mapping the structure of perceptual and visual-motor
             abilities in healthy young adults.},
   Journal = {Acta Psychol (Amst)},
   Volume = {157},
   Pages = {74-84},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0001-6918},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10643 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The ability to quickly detect and respond to visual stimuli
             in the environment is critical to many human activities.
             While such perceptual and visual-motor skills are important
             in a myriad of contexts, considerable variability exists
             between individuals in these abilities. To better understand
             the sources of this variability, we assessed perceptual and
             visual-motor skills in a large sample of 230 healthy
             individuals via the Nike SPARQ Sensory Station, and compared
             variability in their behavioral performance to demographic,
             state, sleep and consumption characteristics. Dimension
             reduction and regression analyses indicated three underlying
             factors: Visual-Motor Control, Visual Sensitivity, and Eye
             Quickness, which accounted for roughly half of the overall
             population variance in performance on this battery.
             Inter-individual variability in Visual-Motor Control was
             correlated with gender and circadian patters such that
             performance on this factor was better for males and for
             those who had been awake for a longer period of time before
             assessment. The current findings indicate that abilities
             involving coordinated hand movements in response to stimuli
             are subject to greater individual variability, while visual
             sensitivity and occulomotor control are largely stable
             across individuals.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.02.005},
   Key = {fds270436}
}

@article{fds270435,
   Author = {Clark, K and Appelbaum, LG and van den Berg, B and Mitroff, SR and Woldorff, MG},
   Title = {Improvement in visual search with practice: mapping
             learning-related changes in neurocognitive stages of
             processing.},
   Journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {13},
   Pages = {5351-5359},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10641 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Practice can improve performance on visual search tasks; the
             neural mechanisms underlying such improvements, however, are
             not clear. Response time typically shortens with practice,
             but which components of the stimulus-response processing
             chain facilitate this behavioral change? Improved search
             performance could result from enhancements in various
             cognitive processing stages, including (1) sensory
             processing, (2) attentional allocation, (3) target
             discrimination, (4) motor-response preparation, and/or (5)
             response execution. We measured event-related potentials
             (ERPs) as human participants completed a five-day
             visual-search protocol in which they reported the
             orientation of a color popout target within an array of
             ellipses. We assessed changes in behavioral performance and
             in ERP components associated with various stages of
             processing. After practice, response time decreased in all
             participants (while accuracy remained consistent), and
             electrophysiological measures revealed modulation of several
             ERP components. First, amplitudes of the early
             sensory-evoked N1 component at 150 ms increased bilaterally,
             indicating enhanced visual sensory processing of the array.
             Second, the negative-polarity posterior-contralateral
             component (N2pc, 170-250 ms) was earlier and larger,
             demonstrating enhanced attentional orienting. Third, the
             amplitude of the sustained posterior contralateral
             negativity component (SPCN, 300-400 ms) decreased,
             indicating facilitated target discrimination. Finally,
             faster motor-response preparation and execution were
             observed after practice, as indicated by latency changes in
             both the stimulus-locked and response-locked lateralized
             readiness potentials (LRPs). These electrophysiological
             results delineate the functional plasticity in key
             mechanisms underlying visual search with high temporal
             resolution and illustrate how practice influences various
             cognitive and neural processing stages leading to enhanced
             behavioral performance.},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1152-14.2015},
   Key = {fds270435}
}

@article{fds270434,
   Author = {Norcia, AM and Appelbaum, LG and Ales, JM and Cottereau, BR and Rossion,
             B},
   Title = {The steady-state visual evoked potential in vision research:
             A review.},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {4},
   Year = {2015},
   ISSN = {1534-7362},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10640 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Periodic visual stimulation and analysis of the resulting
             steady-state visual evoked potentials were first introduced
             over 80 years ago as a means to study visual sensation and
             perception. From the first single-channel recording of
             responses to modulated light to the present use of
             sophisticated digital displays composed of complex visual
             stimuli and high-density recording arrays, steady-state
             methods have been applied in a broad range of scientific and
             applied settings.The purpose of this article is to describe
             the fundamental stimulation paradigms for steady-state
             visual evoked potentials and to illustrate these principles
             through research findings across a range of applications in
             vision science.},
   Doi = {10.1167/15.6.4},
   Key = {fds270434}
}

@article{fds270440,
   Author = {Beam, E and Appelbaum, LG and Jack, J and Moody, J and Huettel,
             SA},
   Title = {Mapping the semantic structure of cognitive
             neuroscience.},
   Journal = {J Cogn Neurosci},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1949-1965},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10645 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {Cognitive neuroscience, as a discipline, links the
             biological systems studied by neuroscience to the processing
             constructs studied by psychology. By mapping these relations
             throughout the literature of cognitive neuroscience, we
             visualize the semantic structure of the discipline and point
             to directions for future research that will advance its
             integrative goal. For this purpose, network text analyses
             were applied to an exhaustive corpus of abstracts collected
             from five major journals over a 30-month period, including
             every study that used fMRI to investigate psychological
             processes. From this, we generate network maps that
             illustrate the relationships among psychological and
             anatomical terms, along with centrality statistics that
             guide inferences about network structure. Three
             terms--prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and anterior cingulate
             cortex--dominate the network structure with their high
             frequency in the literature and the density of their
             connections with other neuroanatomical terms. From network
             statistics, we identify terms that are understudied compared
             with their importance in the network (e.g., insula and
             thalamus), are underspecified in the language of the
             discipline (e.g., terms associated with executive function),
             or are imperfectly integrated with other concepts (e.g.,
             subdisciplines like decision neuroscience that are
             disconnected from the main network). Taking these results as
             the basis for prescriptive recommendations, we conclude that
             semantic analyses provide useful guidance for cognitive
             neuroscience as a discipline, both by illustrating
             systematic biases in the conduct and presentation of
             research and by identifying directions that may be most
             productive for future research.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_00604},
   Key = {fds270440}
}

@article{fds270439,
   Author = {McClintock, SM and Choi, J and Deng, Z-D and Appelbaum, LG and Krystal,
             AD and Lisanby, SH},
   Title = {Multifactorial determinants of the neurocognitive effects of
             electroconvulsive therapy.},
   Journal = {J Ect},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {165-176},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {1095-0680},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10644 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {For many patients with neuropsychiatric illnesses, standard
             psychiatric treatments with mono or combination
             pharmacotherapy, psychotherapy, and transcranial magnetic
             stimulation are ineffective. For these patients with
             treatment-resistant neuropsychiatric illnesses, a main
             therapeutic option is electroconvulsive therapy (ECT).
             Decades of research have found ECT to be highly effective;
             however, it can also result in adverse neurocognitive
             effects. Specifically, ECT results in disorientation after
             each session, anterograde amnesia for recently learned
             information, and retrograde amnesia for previously learned
             information. Unfortunately, the neurocognitive effects and
             underlying mechanisms of action of ECT remain poorly
             understood. The purpose of this paper was to synthesize the
             multiple moderating and mediating factors that are thought
             to underlie the neurocognitive effects of ECT into a
             coherent model. Such factors include demographic and
             neuropsychological characteristics, neuropsychiatric
             symptoms, ECT technical parameters, and ECT-associated
             neurophysiological changes. Future research is warranted to
             evaluate and test this model, so that these findings may
             support the development of more refined clinical seizure
             therapy delivery approaches and efficacious cognitive
             remediation strategies to improve the use of this important
             and widely used intervention tool for neuropsychiatric
             diseases.},
   Doi = {10.1097/YCT.0000000000000137},
   Key = {fds270439}
}

@article{fds270441,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Boehler, CN and Davis, LA and Won, RJ and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {The dynamics of proactive and reactive cognitive control
             processes in the human brain.},
   Journal = {J Cogn Neurosci},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1021-1038},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24345171},
   Abstract = {In this study, we leveraged the high temporal resolution of
             EEG to examine the neural mechanisms underlying the flexible
             regulation of cognitive control that unfolds over different
             timescales. We measured behavioral and neural effects of
             color-word incongruency, as different groups of participants
             performed three different versions of color-word Stroop
             tasks in which the relative timing of the color and word
             features varied from trial to trial. For this purpose, we
             used a standard Stroop color identification task with equal
             congruent-to-incongruent proportions (50%/50%), along with
             two versions of the "Reverse Stroop" word identification
             tasks, for which we manipulated the incongruency proportion
             (50%/50% and 80%/20%). Two canonical ERP markers of neural
             processing of stimulus incongruency, the frontocentral
             negative polarity incongruency wave (NINC) and the late
             positive component (LPC), were evoked across the various
             conditions. Results indicated that color-word incongruency
             interacted with the relative feature timing, producing
             greater neural and behavioral effects when the
             task-irrelevant stimulus preceded the target, but still
             significant effects when it followed. Additionally, both
             behavioral and neural incongruency effects were reduced by
             nearly half in the word identification task (Reverse Stroop
             50/50) relative to the color identification task (Stroop
             50/50), with these effects essentially fully recovering when
             incongruent trials appeared only infrequently (Reverse
             Stroop 80/20). Across the conditions, NINC amplitudes
             closely paralleled RTs, indicating this component is
             sensitive to the overall level of stimulus conflict. In
             contrast, LPC amplitudes were largest with infrequent
             incongruent trials, suggesting a possible readjustment role
             when proactive control is reduced. These findings thus
             unveil distinct control mechanisms that unfold over time in
             response to conflicting stimulus input under different
             contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_00542},
   Key = {fds270441}
}

@article{fds270444,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Cain, MS and Darling, EF and Mitroff,
             SR},
   Title = {Action video game playing is associated with improved visual
             sensitivity, but not alterations in visual sensory
             memory.},
   Journal = {Atten Percept Psychophys},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1161-1167},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23709062},
   Abstract = {Action video game playing has been experimentally linked to
             a number of perceptual and cognitive improvements. These
             benefits are captured through a wide range of psychometric
             tasks and have led to the proposition that action video game
             experience may promote the ability to extract statistical
             evidence from sensory stimuli. Such an advantage could arise
             from a number of possible mechanisms: improvements in visual
             sensitivity, enhancements in the capacity or duration for
             which information is retained in visual memory, or
             higher-level strategic use of information for decision
             making. The present study measured the capacity and time
             course of visual sensory memory using a partial report
             performance task as a means to distinguish between these
             three possible mechanisms. Sensitivity measures and
             parameter estimates that describe sensory memory capacity
             and the rate of memory decay were compared between
             individuals who reported high evels and low levels of action
             video game experience. Our results revealed a uniform
             increase in partial report accuracy at all stimulus-to-cue
             delays for action video game players but no difference in
             the rate or time course of the memory decay. The present
             findings suggest that action video game playing may be
             related to enhancements in the initial sensitivity to visual
             stimuli, but not to a greater retention of information in
             iconic memory buffers.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13414-013-0472-7},
   Key = {fds270444}
}

@article{fds270448,
   Author = {San Martín and R and Appelbaum, LG and Pearson, JM and Huettel, SA and Woldorff, MG},
   Title = {Rapid brain responses independently predict gain
             maximization and loss minimization during economic decision
             making.},
   Journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {16},
   Pages = {7011-7019},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23595758},
   Abstract = {Success in many decision-making scenarios depends on the
             ability to maximize gains and minimize losses. Even if an
             agent knows which cues lead to gains and which lead to
             losses, that agent could still make choices yielding
             suboptimal rewards. Here, by analyzing event-related
             potentials (ERPs) recorded in humans during a probabilistic
             gambling task, we show that individuals' behavioral
             tendencies to maximize gains and to minimize losses are
             associated with their ERP responses to the receipt of those
             gains and losses, respectively. We focused our analyses on
             ERP signals that predict behavioral adjustment: the
             frontocentral feedback-related negativity (FRN) and two P300
             (P3) subcomponents, the frontocentral P3a and the parietal
             P3b. We found that, across participants, gain maximization
             was predicted by differences in amplitude of the P3b for
             suboptimal versus optimal gains (i.e., P3b amplitude
             difference between the least good and the best gains).
             Conversely, loss minimization was predicted by differences
             in the P3b amplitude to suboptimal versus optimal losses
             (i.e., difference between the worst and the least bad
             losses). Finally, we observed that the P3a and P3b, but not
             the FRN, predicted behavioral adjustment on subsequent
             trials, suggesting a specific adaptive mechanism by which
             prior experience may alter ensuing behavior. These findings
             indicate that individual differences in gain maximization
             and loss minimization are linked to individual differences
             in rapid neural responses to monetary outcomes.},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4242-12.2013},
   Key = {fds270448}
}

@article{fds270458,
   Author = {Ales, JM and Appelbaum, LG and Cottereau, BR and Norcia,
             AM},
   Title = {The time course of shape discrimination in the human
             brain.},
   Journal = {Neuroimage},
   Volume = {67},
   Pages = {77-88},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23116814},
   Abstract = {The lateral occipital cortex (LOC) activates selectively to
             images of intact objects versus scrambled controls, is
             selective for the figure-ground relationship of a scene, and
             exhibits at least some degree of invariance for size and
             position. Because of these attributes, it is considered to
             be a crucial part of the object recognition pathway. Here we
             show that human LOC is critically involved in perceptual
             decisions about object shape. High-density EEG was recorded
             while subjects performed a threshold-level shape
             discrimination task on texture-defined figures segmented by
             either phase or orientation cues. The appearance or
             disappearance of a figure region from a uniform background
             generated robust visual evoked potentials throughout
             retinotopic cortex as determined by inverse modeling of the
             scalp voltage distribution. Contrasting responses from
             trials containing shape changes that were correctly detected
             (hits) with trials in which no change occurred (correct
             rejects) revealed stimulus-locked, target-selective activity
             in the occipital visual areas LOC and V4 preceding the
             subject's response. Activity that was locked to the
             subjects' reaction time was present in the LOC.
             Response-locked activity in the LOC was determined to be
             related to shape discrimination for several reasons:
             shape-selective responses were silenced when subjects viewed
             identical stimuli but their attention was directed away from
             the shapes to a demanding letter discrimination task;
             shape-selectivity was present across four different stimulus
             configurations used to define the figure; LOC responses
             correlated with participants' reaction times. These results
             indicate that decision-related activity is present in the
             LOC when subjects are engaged in threshold-level shape
             discriminations.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.044},
   Key = {fds270458}
}

@article{fds270442,
   Author = {Mills, DL and Dai, L and Fishman, I and Yam, A and Appelbaum, LG and St
             George, M and Galaburda, A and Bellugi, U and Korenberg,
             JR},
   Title = {Genetic mapping of brain plasticity across development in
             Williams syndrome: ERP markers of face and language
             processing.},
   Journal = {Dev Neuropsychol},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {613-642},
   Year = {2013},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24219698},
   Abstract = {In Williams Syndrome (WS), a known genetic deletion results
             in atypical brain function with strengths in face and
             language processing. We examined how genetic influences on
             brain activity change with development. In three studies,
             event-related potentials (ERPs) from large samples of
             children, adolescents, and adults with the full genetic
             deletion for WS were compared to typically developing
             controls, and two adults with partial deletions for WS.
             Studies 1 and 2 identified ERP markers of brain plasticity
             in WS across development. Study 3 suggested that, in adults
             with partial deletions for WS, specific genes may be
             differentially implicated in face and language
             processing.},
   Doi = {10.1080/87565641.2013.825617},
   Key = {fds270442}
}

@article{fds270443,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Donohue, SE and Park, CJ and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Is one enough? The case for non-additive influences of
             visual features on crossmodal Stroop interference.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {4},
   Pages = {799},
   Year = {2013},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24198800},
   Abstract = {When different perceptual signals arising from the same
             physical entity are integrated, they form a more reliable
             sensory estimate. When such repetitive sensory signals are
             pitted against other competing stimuli, such as in a Stroop
             Task, this redundancy may lead to stronger processing that
             biases behavior toward reporting the redundant stimuli. This
             bias would therefore, be expected to evoke greater
             incongruency effects than if these stimuli did not contain
             redundant sensory features. In the present paper we report
             that this is not the case for a set of three crossmodal,
             auditory-visual Stroop tasks. In these tasks participants
             attended to, and reported, either the visual or the auditory
             stimulus (in separate blocks) while ignoring the other,
             unattended modality. The visual component of these stimuli
             could be purely semantic (words), purely perceptual
             (colors), or the combination of both. Based on previous work
             showing enhanced crossmodal integration and visual search
             gains for redundantly coded stimuli, we had expected that
             relative to the single features, redundant visual features
             would have induced both greater visual distracter
             incongruency effects for attended auditory targets, and been
             less influenced by auditory distracters for attended visual
             targets. Overall, reaction times were faster for visual
             targets and were dominated by behavioral facilitation for
             the cross-modal interactions (relative to interference), but
             showed surprisingly little influence of visual feature
             redundancy. Post-hoc analyses revealed modest and trending
             evidence for possible increases in behavioral interference
             for redundant visual distracters on auditory targets,
             however, these effects were substantially smaller than
             anticipated and were not accompanied by a redundancy effect
             for behavioral facilitation or for attended visual
             targets.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00799},
   Key = {fds270443}
}

@article{fds270447,
   Author = {Donohue, SE and Appelbaum, LG and Park, CJ and Roberts, KC and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Cross-modal stimulus conflict: the behavioral effects of
             stimulus input timing in a visual-auditory Stroop
             task.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {e62802},
   Year = {2013},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23638149},
   Abstract = {Cross-modal processing depends strongly on the compatibility
             between different sensory inputs, the relative timing of
             their arrival to brain processing components, and on how
             attention is allocated. In this behavioral study, we
             employed a cross-modal audio-visual Stroop task in which we
             manipulated the within-trial stimulus-onset-asynchronies
             (SOAs) of the stimulus-component inputs, the grouping of the
             SOAs (blocked vs. random), the attended modality (auditory
             or visual), and the congruency of the Stroop color-word
             stimuli (congruent, incongruent, neutral) to assess how
             these factors interact within a multisensory context. One
             main result was that visual distractors produced larger
             incongruency effects on auditory targets than vice versa.
             Moreover, as revealed by both overall shorter response times
             (RTs) and relative shifts in the psychometric
             incongruency-effect functions, visual-information processing
             was faster and produced stronger and longer-lasting
             incongruency effects than did auditory. When attending to
             either modality, stimulus incongruency from the other
             modality interacted with SOA, yielding larger effects when
             the irrelevant distractor occurred prior to the attended
             target, but no interaction with SOA grouping. Finally,
             relative to neutral-stimuli, and across the wide range of
             the SOAs employed, congruency led to substantially more
             behavioral facilitation than did incongruency to
             interference, in contrast to findings that within-modality
             stimulus-compatibility effects tend to be more evenly split
             between facilitation and interference. In sum, the present
             findings reveal several key characteristics of how we
             process the stimulus compatibility of cross-modal sensory
             inputs, reflecting stimulus processing patterns that are
             critical for successfully navigating our complex
             multisensory world.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0062802},
   Key = {fds270447}
}

@article{fds270461,
   Author = {Krebs, RM and Boehler, CN and Appelbaum, LG and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Reward associations reduce behavioral interference by
             changing the temporal dynamics of conflict
             processing.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e53894},
   Year = {2013},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23326530},
   Abstract = {Associating stimuli with the prospect of reward typically
             facilitates responses to those stimuli due to an enhancement
             of attentional and cognitive-control processes. Such
             reward-induced facilitation might be especially helpful when
             cognitive-control mechanisms are challenged, as when one
             must overcome interference from irrelevant inputs. Here, we
             investigated the neural dynamics of reward effects in a
             color-naming Stroop task by employing event-related
             potentials (ERPs). We found that behavioral facilitation in
             potential-reward trials, as compared to no-reward trials,
             was paralleled by early ERP modulations likely indexing
             increased attention to the reward-predictive stimulus.
             Moreover, reward changed the temporal dynamics of
             conflict-related ERP components, which may be a consequence
             of an early access to the various stimulus features and
             their relationships. Finally, although word meanings
             referring to potential-reward colors were always
             task-irrelevant, they caused greater interference compared
             to words referring to no-reward colors, an effect that was
             accompanied by a relatively early fronto-central ERP
             modulation. This latter observation suggests that
             task-irrelevant reward information can undermine
             goal-directed behavior at an early processing stage,
             presumably reflecting priming of a goal-incompatible
             response. Yet, these detrimental effects of incongruent
             reward-related words were absent in potential-reward trials,
             apparently due to the prioritized processing of
             task-relevant reward information. Taken together, the
             present data demonstrate that reward associations can
             influence conflict processing by changing the temporal
             dynamics of stimulus processing and subsequent
             cognitive-control mechanisms.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0053894},
   Key = {fds270461}
}

@article{fds270459,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Cain, MS and Schroeder, JE and Darling, EF and Mitroff, SR},
   Title = {Stroboscopic visual training improves information encoding
             in short-term memory.},
   Journal = {Atten Percept Psychophys},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1681-1691},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22810559},
   Abstract = {The visual system has developed to transform an
             undifferentiated and continuous flow of information into
             discrete and manageable representations, and this ability
             rests primarily on the uninterrupted nature of the input.
             Here we explore the impact of altering how visual
             information is accumulated over time by assessing how
             intermittent vision influences memory retention. Previous
             work has shown that intermittent, or stroboscopic, visual
             training (i.e., practicing while only experiencing snapshots
             of vision) can enhance visual-motor control and visual
             cognition, yet many questions remain unanswered about the
             mechanisms that are altered. In the present study, we used a
             partial-report memory paradigm to assess the possible
             changes in visual memory following training under
             stroboscopic conditions. In Experiment 1, the memory task
             was completed before and immediately after a training phase,
             wherein participants engaged in physical activities (e.g.,
             playing catch) while wearing either specialized stroboscopic
             eyewear or transparent control eyewear. In Experiment 2, an
             additional group of participants underwent the same
             stroboscopic protocol but were delayed 24 h between
             training and assessment, so as to measure retention. In
             comparison to the control group, both stroboscopic groups
             (immediate and delayed retest) revealed enhanced retention
             of information in short-term memory, leading to better
             recall at longer stimulus-to-cue delays (640-2,560 ms).
             These results demonstrate that training under stroboscopic
             conditions has the capacity to enhance some aspects of
             visual memory, that these faculties generalize beyond the
             specific tasks that were trained, and that trained
             improvements can be maintained for at least a
             day.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13414-012-0344-6},
   Key = {fds270459}
}

@article{fds270463,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Boehler, CN and Won, R and Davis, L and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Strategic allocation of attention reduces temporally
             predictable stimulus conflict.},
   Journal = {J Cogn Neurosci},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1834-1848},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22360623},
   Abstract = {Humans are able to continuously monitor environmental
             situations and adjust their behavioral strategies to
             optimize performance. Here we investigate the behavioral and
             brain adjustments that occur when conflicting stimulus
             elements are, or are not, temporally predictable. ERPs were
             collected while manual response variants of the Stroop task
             were performed in which the SOAs between the relevant color
             and irrelevant word stimulus components were either randomly
             intermixed or held constant within each experimental run.
             Results indicated that the size of both the neural and
             behavioral effects of stimulus incongruency varied with the
             temporal arrangement of the stimulus components, such that
             the random-SOA arrangements produced the greatest
             incongruency effects at the earliest irrelevant first SOA
             (-200 msec) and the constant-SOA arrangements produced the
             greatest effects with simultaneous presentation. These
             differences in conflict processing were accompanied by rapid
             (∼150 msec) modulations of the sensory ERPs to the
             irrelevant distractor components when they occurred
             consistently first. These effects suggest that individuals
             are able to strategically allocate attention in time to
             mitigate the influence of a temporally predictable
             distractor. As these adjustments are instantiated by the
             participants without instruction, they reveal a form of
             rapid strategic learning for dealing with temporally
             predictable stimulus incongruency.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_00209},
   Key = {fds270463}
}

@article{fds270456,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Cain, MS and Darling, EF and Stanton, SJ and Nguyen,
             MT and Mitroff, SR},
   Title = {Corrigendum to " What is the identity of a sports
             spectator?" [Personality and Individual Differences 52
             (2012) 422-427]},
   Journal = {Personality and Individual Differences},
   Volume = {52},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {862},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0191-8869},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.021},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.021},
   Key = {fds270456}
}

@article{fds270462,
   Author = {Boehler, CN and Appelbaum, LG and Krebs, RM and Hopf, J-M and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {The influence of different Stop-signal response time
             estimation procedures on behavior-behavior and
             brain-behavior correlations.},
   Journal = {Behav Brain Res},
   Volume = {229},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {123-130},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22245527},
   Abstract = {The fundamental cognitive-control function of inhibitory
             control over motor behavior has been extensively
             investigated using the Stop-signal task. The critical
             behavioral parameter describing stopping efficacy is the
             Stop-signal response time (SSRT), and correlations with
             estimates of this parameter are commonly used to establish
             that other variables (e.g., other behavioral measures or
             brain activity measures) are closely related to inhibitory
             motor control. Recently, however, it has been argued that
             SSRT estimates can be strongly distorted if participants
             strategically slow down their responses over the course of
             the experiment, resulting in the SSRT no longer reliably
             representing response-inhibition efficacy. Here, we
             performed new analyses on behavioral and functional data
             from an fMRI version of the Stop-signal task to gauge the
             consequences of using different SSRT estimation approaches
             that are differentially prone to the influence of strategic
             response slowing. The results indicate that the SSRT
             estimation approach can dramatically change
             behavior-behavior correlations. Specifically, a correlation
             between the SSRT and Go-trial accuracy that was highly
             significant with one estimation approach, virtually
             disappeared for the other. Additional analyses indeed
             supported that this effect was related to strategic response
             slowing. Concerning brain-behavior correlations, only the
             left anterior insula was found to be significantly
             correlated with the SSRT within the set of areas tested
             here. Interestingly, this brain-behavior correlation
             differed little for the different SSRT-estimation
             procedures. In sum, the current results highlight that
             different SSRT-estimation procedures can strongly influence
             the distribution of SSRT values across subjects, which in
             turn can ramify into correlational analyses with other
             parameters.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.003},
   Key = {fds270462}
}

@article{fds270454,
   Author = {Gregory Appelbaum and L and Cain, MS and Darling, EF and Stanton, SJ and Nguyen, MT and Mitroff, SR},
   Title = {What is the identity of a sports spectator?},
   Journal = {Personality and Individual Differences},
   Volume = {52},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {422-427},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0191-8869},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.048},
   Abstract = {Despite the prominence of sports in contemporary society,
             little is known about the identity and personality traits of
             sports spectators. With a sample of 293 individuals, we
             examine four broad categories of factors that may explain
             variability in the reported amount of time spent watching
             sports. Using individual difference regression techniques,
             we explore the relationship between sports spectating and
             physiological measures (e.g., testosterone and cortisol),
             clinical self-report scales (ADHD and autism), personality
             traits (e.g., NEO "Big Five"), and pastime activities (e.g.,
             video game playing). Our results indicate that individuals
             who report higher levels of sports spectating tend to have
             higher levels of extraversion, and in particular excitement
             seeking and gregariousness. These individuals also engage
             more in complementary pastime activities, including
             participating in sports and exercise activities, watching
             TV/movies, and playing video games. Notably, no differences
             were observed in the clinical self-report scales, indicating
             no differences in reported symptoms of ADHD or autism for
             spectators and non-spectators. Likewise, no relationship was
             seen between baseline concentrations of testosterone or
             cortisol and sports spectating in our sample. These results
             provide an assessment of the descriptive personality
             dimensions of frequent sports spectators and provide a basic
             taxonomy of how these traits are expressed across the
             population. © 2011 Elsevier Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.048},
   Key = {fds270454}
}

@article{fds270460,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Ales, JM and Norcia, AM},
   Title = {The time course of segmentation and cue-selectivity in the
             human visual cortex.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e34205},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22479566},
   Abstract = {Texture discontinuities are a fundamental cue by which the
             visual system segments objects from their background. The
             neural mechanisms supporting texture-based segmentation are
             therefore critical to visual perception and cognition. In
             the present experiment we employ an EEG source-imaging
             approach in order to study the time course of texture-based
             segmentation in the human brain. Visual Evoked Potentials
             were recorded to four types of stimuli in which periodic
             temporal modulation of a central 3° figure region could
             either support figure-ground segmentation, or have identical
             local texture modulations but not produce changes in global
             image segmentation. The image discontinuities were defined
             either by orientation or phase differences across image
             regions. Evoked responses to these four stimuli were
             analyzed both at the scalp and on the cortical surface in
             retinotopic and functional regions-of-interest (ROIs)
             defined separately using fMRI on a subject-by-subject basis.
             Texture segmentation (tsVEP: segmenting versus
             non-segmenting) and cue-specific (csVEP: orientation versus
             phase) responses exhibited distinctive patterns of activity.
             Alternations between uniform and segmented images produced
             highly asymmetric responses that were larger after
             transitions from the uniform to the segmented state. Texture
             modulations that signaled the appearance of a figure evoked
             a pattern of increased activity starting at ∼143 ms that
             was larger in V1 and LOC ROIs, relative to identical
             modulations that didn't signal figure-ground segmentation.
             This segmentation-related activity occurred after an initial
             response phase that did not depend on the global
             segmentation structure of the image. The two cue types
             evoked similar tsVEPs up to 230 ms when they differed in the
             V4 and LOC ROIs. The evolution of the response proceeded
             largely in the feed-forward direction, with only weak
             evidence for feedback-related activity.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0034205},
   Key = {fds270460}
}

@article{fds270471,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Smith, DV and Boehler, CN and Chen, WD and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Rapid modulation of sensory processing induced by stimulus
             conflict.},
   Journal = {J Cogn Neurosci},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {2620-2628},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {1530-8898},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20849233},
   Keywords = {Adult • Analysis of Variance • Brain • Brain
             Mapping* • Conflict (Psychology)* •
             Electroencephalography • Evoked Potentials, Visual
             • Female • Functional Laterality • Humans
             • Male • Photic Stimulation • Reaction Time
             • Visual Pathways • Young Adult • methods
             • physiology • physiology*},
   Abstract = {Humans are constantly confronted with environmental stimuli
             that conflict with task goals and can interfere with
             successful behavior. Prevailing theories propose the
             existence of cognitive control mechanisms that can suppress
             the processing of conflicting input and enhance that of the
             relevant input. However, the temporal cascade of brain
             processes invoked in response to conflicting stimuli remains
             poorly understood. By examining evoked electrical brain
             responses in a novel, hemifield-specific, visual-flanker
             task, we demonstrate that task-irrelevant conflicting
             stimulus input is quickly detected in higher level executive
             regions while simultaneously inducing rapid, recurrent
             modulation of sensory processing in the visual cortex.
             Importantly, however, both of these effects are larger for
             individuals with greater incongruency-related RT slowing.
             The combination of neural activation patterns and behavioral
             interference effects suggest that this initial sensory
             modulation induced by conflicting stimulus inputs reflects
             performance-degrading attentional distraction because of
             their incompatibility rather than any rapid task-enhancing
             cognitive control mechanisms. The present findings thus
             provide neural evidence for a model in which attentional
             distraction is the key initial trigger for the temporal
             cascade of processes by which the human brain responds to
             conflicting stimulus input in the environment.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn.2010.21575},
   Key = {fds270471}
}

@article{fds270457,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Schroeder, JE and Cain, MS and Mitroff,
             SR},
   Title = {Improved Visual Cognition through Stroboscopic
             Training.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {2},
   Pages = {276},
   Year = {2011},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22059078},
   Abstract = {Humans have a remarkable capacity to learn and adapt, but
             surprisingly little research has demonstrated generalized
             learning in which new skills and strategies can be used
             flexibly across a range of tasks and contexts. In the
             present work we examined whether generalized learning could
             result from visual-motor training under stroboscopic visual
             conditions. Individuals were assigned to either an
             experimental condition that trained with stroboscopic
             eyewear or to a control condition that underwent identical
             training with non-stroboscopic eyewear. The training
             consisted of multiple sessions of athletic activities during
             which participants performed simple drills such as throwing
             and catching. To determine if training led to generalized
             benefits, we used computerized measures to assess perceptual
             and cognitive abilities on a variety of tasks before and
             after training. Computer-based assessments included measures
             of visual sensitivity (central and peripheral motion
             coherence thresholds), transient spatial attention (a useful
             field of view - dual task paradigm), and sustained attention
             (multiple-object tracking). Results revealed that
             stroboscopic training led to significantly greater re-test
             improvement in central visual field motion sensitivity and
             transient attention abilities. No training benefits were
             observed for peripheral motion sensitivity or peripheral
             transient attention abilities, nor were benefits seen for
             sustained attention during multiple-object tracking. These
             findings suggest that stroboscopic training can effectively
             improve some, but not all aspects of visual perception and
             attention.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00276},
   Key = {fds270457}
}

@article{fds270465,
   Author = {Boehler, CN and Appelbaum, LG and Krebs, RM and Chen, L-C and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {The role of stimulus salience and attentional capture across
             the neural hierarchy in a stop-signal task.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {e26386},
   Year = {2011},
   ISSN = {1932-6203},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22022611},
   Keywords = {Attention • Behavior • Brain • Brain Mapping
             • Female • Humans • Magnetic Resonance
             Imaging • Male • Nerve Net • Physical
             Stimulation • Task Performance and Analysis* •
             Young Adult • anatomy & histology • physiology
             • physiology*},
   Abstract = {Inhibitory motor control is a core function of cognitive
             control. Evidence from diverse experimental approaches has
             linked this function to a mostly right-lateralized network
             of cortical and subcortical areas, wherein a signal from the
             frontal cortex to the basal ganglia is believed to trigger
             motor-response cancellation. Recently, however, it has been
             recognized that in the context of typical motor-control
             paradigms those processes related to actual response
             inhibition and those related to the attentional processing
             of the relevant stimuli are highly interrelated and thus
             difficult to distinguish. Here, we used fMRI and a modified
             Stop-signal task to specifically examine the role of
             perceptual and attentional processes triggered by the
             different stimuli in such tasks, thus seeking to further
             distinguish other cognitive processes that may precede or
             otherwise accompany the implementation of response
             inhibition. In order to establish which brain areas respond
             to sensory stimulation differences by rare Stop-stimuli, as
             well as to the associated attentional capture that these may
             trigger irrespective of their task-relevance, we compared
             brain activity evoked by Stop-trials to that evoked by
             Go-trials in task blocks where Stop-stimuli were to be
             ignored. In addition, region-of-interest analyses comparing
             the responses to these task-irrelevant Stop-trials, with
             those to typical relevant Stop-trials, identified separable
             activity profiles as a function of the task-relevance of the
             Stop-signal. While occipital areas were mostly blind to the
             task-relevance of Stop-stimuli, activity in temporo-parietal
             areas dissociated between task-irrelevant and task-relevant
             ones. Activity profiles in frontal areas, in turn, were
             activated mainly by task-relevant Stop-trials, presumably
             reflecting a combination of triggered top-down attentional
             influences and inhibitory motor-control processes.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0026386},
   Key = {fds270465}
}

@article{fds270451,
   Author = {Jack, J and Appelbaum, LG},
   Title = {"This is your brain on rhetoric": Research directions for
             neurorhetorics},
   Journal = {Rhetoric Society Quarterly},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {411-437},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {0277-3945},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000284226900003&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Neuroscience research findings yield fascinating new
             insights into human cognition and communication.
             Rhetoricians may be attracted to neuroscience research that
             uses imaging tools (such as fMRI) to draw inferences about
             rhetorical concepts, such as emotion, reason, or empathy.
             Yet this interdisciplinary effort poses challenges to
             rhetorical scholars. Accordingly, research in neurorhetorics
             should be two-sided: Not only should researchers question
             the neuroscience of rhetoric (the brain functions related to
             persuasion and argument), but they should also inquire into
             the rhetoric of neuroscience (how neuroscience research
             findings are framed rhetorically). This two-sided approach
             can help rhetoric scholars to use neuroscience insights in a
             responsible manner, minimizing analytical pitfalls. These
             two approaches can be combined to examine neuroscience
             discussions about methodology, research, and emotion, and
             studies of autism and empathy, with a rhetorical as well as
             scientific lens. Such an approach yields productive insights
             into rhetoric while minimizing potential pitfalls of
             interdisciplinary work. © 2010 The Rhetoric Society of
             America.},
   Doi = {10.1080/02773945.2010.516303},
   Key = {fds270451}
}

@article{fds270470,
   Author = {Boehler, CN and Appelbaum, LG and Krebs, RM and Hopf, JM and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {Pinning down response inhibition in the brain--conjunction
             analyses of the Stop-signal task.},
   Journal = {Neuroimage},
   Volume = {52},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1621-1632},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {1095-9572},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20452445},
   Keywords = {Adult • Cerebral Cortex • Cognition • Cues*
             • Female • Humans • Male • Movement
             • Neural Inhibition • Visual Perception •
             physiology*},
   Abstract = {Successful behavior requires a finely-tuned interplay of
             initiating and inhibiting motor programs to react
             effectively to constantly changing environmental demands.
             One particularly useful paradigm for investigating
             inhibitory motor control is the Stop-signal task, where
             already-initiated responses to Go-stimuli are to be
             inhibited upon the rapid subsequent presentation of a
             Stop-stimulus (yielding successful and unsuccessful
             Stop-trials). Despite the extensive use of this paradigm in
             functional neuroimaging, there is no consensus on which
             functional comparison to use to characterize
             response-inhibition-related brain activity. Here, we utilize
             conjunction analyses of successful and unsuccessful
             Stop-trials that are each contrasted against a reference
             condition. This conjunction approach identifies processes
             common to both Stop-trial types while excluding processes
             specific to either, thereby capitalizing on the presence of
             some response-inhibition-related activity in both
             conditions. Using this approach on fMRI data from human
             subjects, we identify a network of brain structures that was
             linked to both types of Stop-trials, including
             lateral-inferior frontal and medial frontal cortical areas
             and the caudate nucleus. In addition, comparisons with a
             reference condition matched for visual stimulation
             identified additional activity in the right inferior
             parietal cortex that may play a role in enhancing the
             processing of the Stop-stimuli. Finally, differences in
             stopping efficacy across subjects were associated with
             variations in activity in the left anterior insula. However,
             this region was also associated with general task accuracy
             (which furthermore correlated directly with stopping
             efficacy), suggesting that it might actually reflect a more
             general mechanism of performance control that supports
             response inhibition in a relatively nonspecific
             way.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.276},
   Key = {fds270470}
}

@article{fds270475,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Ales, JM and Cottereau, B and Norcia,
             AM},
   Title = {Configural specificity of the lateral occipital
             cortex.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologia},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {3323-3328},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {1873-3514},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20638395},
   Keywords = {Adult • Cues • Electroencephalography •
             Evoked Potentials, Visual • Female • Humans •
             Image Processing, Computer-Assisted • Magnetic
             Resonance Imaging • Male • Occipital Lobe •
             Photic Stimulation • Visual Cortex • Visual
             Perception • physiology • physiology*},
   Abstract = {While regions of the lateral occipital cortex (LOC) are
             known to be selective for objects relative to
             feature-matched controls, it is not known what set of cues
             or configurations are used to promote this selectivity. Many
             theories of perceptual organization have emphasized the
             figure-ground relationship as being especially important in
             object-level processing. In the present work we studied the
             role of perceptual organization in eliciting visual evoked
             potentials from the object selective LOC. To do this, we
             used two-region stimuli in which the regions were modulated
             at different temporal frequencies and were comprised of
             either symmetric or asymmetric arrangements. The asymmetric
             arrangement produced an unambiguous figure-ground
             relationship consistent with a smaller figure region
             surrounded by a larger background, while four different
             symmetric arrangements resulted in ambiguous figure-ground
             relationships but still possessed strong kinetic boundaries
             between the regions. The surrounded figure-ground
             arrangement evoked greater activity in the LOC relative to
             first-tier visual areas (V1-V3). Response selectivity in the
             LOC, however, was not present for the four different types
             of symmetric stimuli. These results suggest that kinetic
             texture boundaries alone are not sufficient to trigger
             selective processing in the LOC, but that the spatial
             configuration of a figure that is surrounded by a larger
             background is both necessary and sufficient to selectively
             activate the LOC.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.016},
   Key = {fds270475}
}

@article{fds270472,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Meyerhoff, KL and Woldorff, MG},
   Title = {Priming and backward influences in the human brain:
             processing interactions during the stroop interference
             effect.},
   Journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {2508-2521},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {1460-2199},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19321654},
   Keywords = {Adolescent • Adult • Attention • Brain •
             Brain Mapping • Choice Behavior • Conflict
             (Psychology)* • Contingent Negative Variation •
             Female • Humans • Male • Middle Aged •
             Problem Solving • Stroop Test • Young Adult •
             methods* • physiology*},
   Abstract = {This study investigated neural processing interactions
             during Stroop interference by varying the temporal
             separation of relevant and irrelevant features of congruent,
             neutral, and incongruent colored-bar/color-word stimulus
             components. High-density event-related potentials (ERPs) and
             behavioral performance were measured as participants
             reported the bar color as quickly as possible, while
             ignoring the color words. The task-irrelevant color words
             could appear at 1 of 5 stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs)
             relative to the task-relevant bar-color occurrence: -200 or
             -100 ms before, +100 or +200 ms after, or simultaneously.
             Incongruent relative to congruent presentations elicited
             slower reaction times and higher error rates (with neutral
             in between), and ERP difference waves containing both an
             early, negative-polarity, central-parietal deflection, and a
             later, more left-sided, positive-polarity component. These
             congruency-related differences interacted with SOA, showing
             the greatest behavioral and electrophysiological effects
             when irrelevant stimulus information preceded the
             task-relevant target and reduced effects when the irrelevant
             information followed the relevant target. We interpret these
             data as reflecting 2 separate processes: 1) a 'priming
             influence' that enhances the magnitude of conflict-related
             facilitation and conflict-related interference when a
             task-relevant target is preceded by an irrelevant
             distractor; and 2) a reduced 'backward influence' of
             stimulus conflict when the irrelevant distractor information
             follows the task-relevant target.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhp036},
   Key = {fds270472}
}

@article{fds270469,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Norcia, AM},
   Title = {Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural
             processing.},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {18.1-1812},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {1534-7362},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20053081},
   Keywords = {Adult • Attention • Electroencephalography •
             Evoked Potentials, Visual • Form Perception •
             Fourier Analysis • Frontal Lobe • Humans •
             Middle Aged • Occipital Lobe • Photic Stimulation
             • methods • physiology*},
   Abstract = {Here we use the steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP)
             to study attentive versus non-attentive processing of simple
             texture-defined shapes. By "tagging" the figure and
             background regions with different temporal frequencies, the
             method isolates response components associated with the
             figure region, the background region, and with non-linear
             spatio-temporal interactions between regions. Each of these
             response classes has a distinct scalp topography that is
             preserved under differing attentional task demands. In one
             task, attention was directed to discrimination of shape
             changes in the figure region. In the other task, a difficult
             letter discrimination was used to divert attentive
             processing resources away from the texture-defined form.
             Larger task-dependent effects were observed for figure
             responses and for the figure/background interaction than for
             the background responses. The figure region responses were
             delayed in occipital areas in the shape versus letter task
             conditions, while the region interactions were enhanced,
             especially in frontal areas. While a basic differentiation
             of figure from background processing occurred independent of
             task, attentive processing of elementary shapes recruited
             later occipital activity for figure processing and sustained
             non-linear figure/background interaction in frontal areas.
             Collectively, these results indicate that basic aspects of
             scene segmentation proceed pre-attentively, but that
             directed attention to the object shape engages a widely
             distributed network of brain areas including frontal and
             occipital regions.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1167/9.11.18},
   Key = {fds270469}
}

@article{fds350011,
   Author = {Haberman, J and Harp, T and Whitney, D},
   Title = {Averaging facial expression over time.},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {1.1-113},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/9.11.1},
   Abstract = {The visual system groups similar features, objects, and
             motion (e.g., Gestalt grouping). Recent work suggests that
             the computation underlying perceptual grouping may be one of
             summary statistical representation. Summary representation
             occurs for low-level features, such as size, motion, and
             position, and even for high level stimuli, including faces;
             for example, observers accurately perceive the average
             expression in a group of faces (J. Haberman & D. Whitney,
             2007, 2009). The purpose of the present experiments was to
             characterize the time-course of this facial integration
             mechanism. In a series of three experiments, we measured
             observers' abilities to recognize the average expression of
             a temporal sequence of distinct faces. Faces were presented
             in sets of 4, 12, or 20, at temporal frequencies ranging
             from 1.6 to 21.3 Hz. The results revealed that observers
             perceived the average expression in a temporal sequence of
             different faces as precisely as they perceived a single face
             presented repeatedly. The facial averaging was independent
             of temporal frequency or set size, but depended on the total
             duration of exposed faces, with a time constant of
             approximately 800 ms. These experiments provide evidence
             that the visual system is sensitive to the ensemble
             characteristics of complex objects presented over
             time.},
   Doi = {10.1167/9.11.1},
   Key = {fds350011}
}

@article{fds270466,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Liotti, M and Perez, R and Fox, SP and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {The temporal dynamics of implicit processing of non-letter,
             letter, and word-forms in the human visual
             cortex.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Human Neuroscience},
   Volume = {3},
   Pages = {56},
   Year = {2009},
   ISSN = {1662-5161},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20046826},
   Abstract = {The decoding of visually presented line segments into
             letters, and letters into words, is critical to fluent
             reading abilities. Here we investigate the temporal dynamics
             of visual orthographic processes, focusing specifically on
             right hemisphere contributions and interactions between the
             hemispheres involved in the implicit processing of visually
             presented words, consonants, false fonts, and symbolic
             strings. High-density EEG was recorded while participants
             detected infrequent, simple, perceptual targets (dot
             strings) embedded amongst a of character strings. Beginning
             at 130 ms, orthographic and non-orthographic stimuli were
             distinguished by a sequence of ERP effects over occipital
             recording sites. These early latency occipital effects were
             dominated by enhanced right-sided negative-polarity
             activation for non-orthographic stimuli that peaked at
             around 180 ms. This right-sided effect was followed by
             bilateral positive occipital activity for false-fonts, but
             not symbol strings. Moreover the size of components of this
             later positive occipital wave was inversely correlated with
             the right-sided ROcc180 wave, suggesting that subjects who
             had larger early right-sided activation for non-orthographic
             stimuli had less need for more extended bilateral (e.g.,
             interhemispheric) processing of those stimuli shortly later.
             Additional early (130-150 ms) negative-polarity activity
             over left occipital cortex and longer-latency centrally
             distributed responses (>300 ms) were present, likely
             reflecting implicit activation of the previously reported
             'visual-word-form' area and N400-related responses,
             respectively. Collectively, these results provide a close
             look at some relatively unexplored portions of the temporal
             flow of information processing in the brain related to the
             implicit processing of potentially linguistic information
             and provide valuable information about the interactions
             between hemispheres supporting visual orthographic
             processing.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.3389/neuro.09.056.2009},
   Key = {fds270466}
}

@article{fds270467,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Wade, AR and Pettet, MW and Vildavski, VY and Norcia,
             AM},
   Title = {Figure-ground interaction in the human visual
             cortex.},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {8.1-819},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {1534-7362},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18831644},
   Keywords = {Adult • Brain Mapping* • Electroencephalography
             • Form Perception • Humans • Magnetic
             Resonance Imaging • Models, Neurological* •
             Multivariate Analysis • Nonlinear Dynamics •
             Photic Stimulation • Retina • Space Perception
             • Visual Cortex • physiology •
             physiology*},
   Abstract = {Discontinuities in feature maps serve as important cues for
             the location of object boundaries. Here we used multi-input
             nonlinear analysis methods and EEG source imaging to assess
             the role of several different boundary cues in visual scene
             segmentation. Synthetic figure/ground displays portraying a
             circular figure region were defined solely by differences in
             the temporal frequency of the figure and background regions
             in the limiting case and by the addition of orientation or
             relative alignment cues in other cases. The use of distinct
             temporal frequencies made it possible to separately record
             responses arising from each region and to characterize the
             nature of nonlinear interactions between the two regions as
             measured in a set of retinotopically and functionally
             defined cortical areas. Figure/background interactions were
             prominent in retinotopic areas, and in an extra-striate
             region lying dorsal and anterior to area MT+.
             Figure/background interaction was greatly diminished by the
             elimination of orientation cues, the introduction of small
             gaps between the two regions, or by the presence of a
             constant second-order border between regions. Nonlinear
             figure/background interactions therefore carry spatially
             precise, time-locked information about the
             continuity/discontinuity of oriented texture fields. This
             information is widely distributed throughout occipital
             areas, including areas that do not display strong
             retinotopy.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1167/8.9.8},
   Key = {fds270467}
}

@article{fds270474,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Lu, Z-L and Sperling, G},
   Title = {Contrast amplification in global texture orientation
             discrimination.},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {13.1-1319},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {1534-7362},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17997682},
   Keywords = {Contrast Sensitivity* • Discrimination (Psychology)*
             • Fourier Analysis • Humans • Models,
             Psychological • Orientation • Perceptual Masking
             • Photic Stimulation • Sensory Thresholds •
             methods*},
   Abstract = {We show that adding a low-contrast texture stimulus that is
             far below its own detection threshold to an ambiguously
             oriented high-contrast texture can produce an easily
             perceived global orientation. When such a low-contrast
             (e.g., 0.1%) test texture and a high-contrast (e.g., 2%)
             amplifier texture are interleaved, the effective strength
             for global orientation detection closely approximates the
             product of the two contrasts. Therefore, adding two
             ambiguous textures, an amplifier texture at 5x its threshold
             contrast for global orientation discrimination and a test
             texture at 1/5x its threshold contrast, produces threshold
             global orientation discrimination, that is, 5x amplification
             of the below-threshold test texture. The observed 5x
             amplification factors are larger than facilitation effects
             reported in other pattern tasks. Amplification is 11x when
             orientation discrimination thresholds are compared to
             absolute detection thresholds. For second-order textures,
             maximum contrast amplification is about 2.5x. A contrast
             gain control model is presented that accounts for 90% of the
             variance in observed d' for texture patterns of differing
             geometries, exposure durations, and component contrasts. In
             the model, very low-contrast orientations are represented by
             power functions of their contrasts, with an exponent greater
             than two. As the contrast of an amplifier texture increases
             beyond about 4%, feed-forward gain control exerted by the
             amplifier ultimately nullifies the amplification effect and
             produces masking.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1167/7.10.13},
   Key = {fds270474}
}

@article{fds270449,
   Author = {Norcia, AM and Wade, AR and Vildavski, VY and Pettet, MW and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Anticipatory responses in human visual cortex to predictable
             stimuli: An EEG source-imaging study},
   Journal = {Perception},
   Volume = {36},
   Pages = {111-111},
   Publisher = {SAGE PUBLICATIONS LTD},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0301-0066},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000250594600395&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds270449}
}

@article{fds270464,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Wade, AR and Vildavski, VY and Pettet, MW and Norcia,
             AM},
   Title = {Cue-invariant networks for figure and background processing
             in human visual cortex.},
   Journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {45},
   Pages = {11695-11708},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {1529-2401},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17093091},
   Keywords = {Adult • Attention • Brain Mapping* • Contrast
             Sensitivity • Cues* • Electroencephalography
             • Humans • Image Processing, Computer-Assisted
             • Magnetic Resonance Imaging • Multivariate
             Analysis • Orientation • Oxygen • Pattern
             Recognition, Visual • Photic Stimulation • Size
             Perception • Space Perception • Spectrum Analysis
             • Visual Cortex • Visual Fields • blood
             • blood supply • methods • physiology •
             physiology*},
   Abstract = {Lateral occipital cortical areas are involved in the
             perception of objects, but it is not clear how these areas
             interact with first tier visual areas. Using synthetic
             images portraying a simple texture-defined figure and an
             electrophysiological paradigm that allows us to monitor
             cortical responses to figure and background regions
             separately, we found distinct neuronal networks responsible
             for the processing of each region. The figure region of our
             displays was tagged with one temporal frequency (3.0 Hz) and
             the background region with another (3.6 Hz). Spectral
             analysis was used to separate the responses to the two
             regions during their simultaneous presentation. Distributed
             source reconstructions were made by using the minimum norm
             method, and cortical current density was measured in a set
             of visual areas defined on retinotopic and functional
             criteria with the use of functional magnetic resonance
             imaging. The results of the main experiments, combined with
             a set of control experiments, indicate that the figure
             region, but not the background, was routed preferentially to
             lateral cortex. A separate network extending from first tier
             through more dorsal areas responded preferentially to the
             background region. The figure-related responses were mostly
             invariant with respect to the texture types used to define
             the figure, did not depend on its spatial location or size,
             and mostly were unaffected by attentional instructions.
             Because of the emergent nature of a segmented figure in our
             displays, feedback from higher cortical areas is a likely
             candidate for the selection mechanism by which the figure
             region is routed to lateral occipital cortex.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2741-06.2006},
   Key = {fds270464}
}

@article{fds270452,
   Author = {Sperling, G and Appelbaum, LG and Lu, ZL},
   Title = {Amplifying the effective perceptual contrast of a
             grating},
   Journal = {Perception},
   Volume = {34},
   Pages = {22-22},
   Publisher = {PION LTD},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0301-0066},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000232726000052&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds270452}
}

@article{fds270473,
   Author = {Mervis, CB and Morris, CA and Klein-Tasman, BP and Bertrand, J and Kwitny, S and Appelbaum, LG and Rice, CE},
   Title = {Attentional characteristics of infants and toddlers with
             Williams syndrome during triadic interactions.},
   Journal = {Developmental Neuropsychology},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1-2},
   Pages = {243-268},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2003},
   ISSN = {8756-5641},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12730027},
   Keywords = {Attention* • Case-Control Studies • Child Behavior
             • Child Development • Child, Preschool •
             Female • Humans • Infant • Male •
             Temperament* • Visual Perception* • Williams
             Syndrome • genetics • physiopathology •
             psychology*},
   Abstract = {Two studies were conducted to consider the looking behavior
             of infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome (WS). In
             Study 1,the looking behavior of a 10-month-old girl with WS
             during play sessions with her mother and with a stranger was
             compared to that of 2 groups of infants who were developing
             normally (ND),1 matched for chronological age and the other
             for developmental age. The infant with WS spent more than
             twice as much time looking at her mother as the infants in
             either contrast group did. She also spent twice as much time
             looking at the stranger. In addition, during 78%of this
             time, her gaze at the stranger was coded as extremely
             intense. Looks of this intensity were virtually never made
             by the ND infants. In Study 2,the looking behavior of 31
             individuals with WS ages 8 to 43 months during a genetics
             evaluation was compared to that of 319 control children in
             the same age range (242 with developmental delay due to
             causes other than WS).Twenty-three of the 25 participants
             with WS aged 33 months or younger demonstrated extended and
             intense looking at the geneticist. In contrast, none of the
             control participants looked extensively or intently at the
             geneticist. Findings are discussed in the context of
             previous research on arousal and focused attention during
             normal development and on temperament and personality of
             older children and adults with WS. It is argued that the
             unusual looking patterns evidenced by infants and toddlers
             with WS presage the unusual temperament and personality of
             older individuals with WS, and the possibility of a genetic
             basis for these behaviors is addressed.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1080/87565641.2003.9651894},
   Key = {fds270473}
}

@article{fds270450,
   Author = {Appelbaum, LG and Lu, ZL and Sperling, G},
   Title = {Contrast amplification in a texture discrimination
             task.},
   Journal = {Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {S315-S315},
   Publisher = {ASSOC RESEARCH VISION OPHTHALMOLOGY INC},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0146-0404},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000168392101679&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds270450}
}

@article{fds270468,
   Author = {Mills, DL and Alvarez, TD and St George and M and Appelbaum, LG and Bellugi, U and Neville, H},
   Title = {III. Electrophysiological studies of face processing in
             Williams syndrome.},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Volume = {12 Suppl 1},
   Pages = {47-64},
   Year = {2000},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10953233},
   Keywords = {Adolescent • Adult • Electroencephalography •
             Electrophysiology* • Evoked Potentials • Face*
             • Humans • Occipital Lobe • Space Perception
             • Williams Syndrome • physiology •
             physiopathology • physiopathology*},
   Abstract = {Williams Syndrome (WMS) is a genetically based disorder
             characterized by pronounced variability in performance
             across different domains of cognitive functioning. This
             study examined brain activity linked to face-processing
             abilities, which are typically spared in individuals with
             WMS. Subjects watched photographic pairs of upright or
             inverted faces and indicated if the second face matched or
             did not match the first face. Results from a previous study
             with normal adults showed dramatic differences in the timing
             and distribution of ERP effects linked to recognition of
             upright and inverted faces. In normal adults, upright faces
             elicited ERP differences to matched vs. mismatched faces at
             approximately 320 msec (N320) after the onset of the second
             stimulus. This "N320" effect was largest over anterior
             regions of the right hemisphere. In contrast, the
             mismatch/match effect for inverted faces consisted of a
             large positive component between 400 and 1000 msec (P500)
             that was largest over parietal regions and was symmetrical.
             In contrast to normal adults, WMS subjects showed an
             N320-mismatch effect for both upright and inverted faces.
             Additionally, the WMS subjects did not display the N320
             right-hemisphere asymmetry observed in the normal adults.
             WMS subjects also displayed an abnormally small negativity
             at 100 msec (N100) and an abnormally large negativity at 200
             msec (N200) to both upright and inverted faces. This ERP
             pattern was observed in all subjects with WMS but was not
             observed in the normal controls. These results may be linked
             to increased attention to faces in subjects with WMS and
             might be specific to the disorder. These results were
             consistent with our ERP studies of language processing in
             WMS, which suggested abnormal cerebral specialization for
             spared cognitive functions in individuals with
             WMS.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1162/089892900561977},
   Key = {fds270468}
}


%% Chapters in Books   
@misc{fds353026,
   Author = {Beynel, L and Appelbaum, LG and Kimbrel, NA},
   Title = {Neurobiology and neuromodulation of emotion in
             PTSD},
   Pages = {175-210},
   Booktitle = {Emotion in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Etiology,
             Assessment, Neurobiology, and Treatment},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9780128160220},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-816022-0.00007-7},
   Abstract = {PTSD is a chronic, highly debilitating condition that can
             develop following exposure to traumatic events.
             Unfortunately, less than 20%-30% of patients with PTSD
             achieve full remission following pharmacological treatment.
             As such, there is a need for novel, nonpharmacological
             alternatives that can facilitate recovery from PTSD. Brain
             stimulation approaches have been widely used as treatments
             for mood disorders, and a growing literature is
             investigating the capacity of these approaches to treat PTSD
             symptoms. Results from these studies demonstrate feasibility
             and preliminary efficacy; however, optimizing brain
             stimulation efficacy remains a crucial goal that requires a
             better understanding of the neurobiology associated with
             this disorder. The present chapter describes neurocircuitry
             models of PTSD as they relate to systems that are
             specifically accessible through brain stimulation. We then
             review existing brain stimulation studies that have
             addressed PTSD and associated outcomes before concluding
             with proposals for optimizing rTMS efficacy in PTSD
             treatment.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-816022-0.00007-7},
   Key = {fds353026}
}

@misc{fds338342,
   Author = {Clements, JM and Kopper, R and Zielinski, DJ and Rao, H and Sommer, MA and Kirsch, E and Mainsah, BO and Collins, LM and Appelbaum,
             LG},
   Title = {Neurophysiology of Visual-Motor Learning during a Simulated
             Marksmanship Task in Immersive Virtual Reality},
   Journal = {25th Ieee Conference on Virtual Reality and 3d User
             Interfaces, Vr 2018 Proceedings},
   Pages = {451-458},
   Publisher = {IEEE},
   Editor = {Kiyokawa, K and Steinicke, F and Thomas, BH and Welch,
             G},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9781538633656},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/VR.2018.8446068},
   Abstract = {Immersive virtual reality (VR) systems offer flexible
             control of an interactive environment, along with precise
             position and orientation tracking of realistic movements.
             Immersive VR can also be used in conjunction with
             neurophysiological monitoring techniques, such as
             electroencephalography (EEG), to record neural activity as
             users perform complex tasks. As such, the fusion of VR,
             kinematic tracking, and EEG offers a powerful testbed for
             naturalistic neuroscience research. In this study, we
             combine these elements to investigate the cognitive and
             neural mechanisms that underlie motor skill learning during
             a multi-day simulated marksmanship training regimen
             conducted with 20 participants. On each of 3 days,
             participants performed 8 blocks of 60 trials in which a
             simulated clay pigeon was launched from behind a trap house.
             Participants attempted to shoot the moving target with a
             firearm game controller, receiving immediate positional
             feedback and running scores after each shot. Over the course
             of the 3 days that individuals practiced this protocol, shot
             accuracy and precision improved significantly while reaction
             times got significantly faster. Furthermore, results
             demonstrate that more negative EEG amplitudes produced over
             the visual cortices correlate with better shooting
             performance measured by accuracy, reaction times, and
             response times, indicating that early visual system
             plasticity underlies behavioral learning in this task. These
             findings point towards a naturalistic neuroscience approach
             that can be used to identify neural markers of marksmanship
             performance.},
   Doi = {10.1109/VR.2018.8446068},
   Key = {fds338342}
}

@misc{fds330473,
   Author = {Jack, J and Appelbaum, LG and Beam, E and Moody, J and Huettel,
             SA},
   Title = {Mapping rhetorical topologies in cognitive
             neuroscience},
   Pages = {125-150},
   Booktitle = {Topologies as Techniques for a Post-Critical
             Rhetoric},
   Publisher = {Springer International Publishing},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9783319512679},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-51268-6_7},
   Abstract = {Many tools that neuroscientists use to trace the complex
             topography of the human brain draw on the neuroscience
             literature to yield “metanalyses” or “syntheses of
             data.” These approaches conflate rhetorical connections in
             the literature with physical connections in the brain. By
             contrast, the model presented in this chapter seeks not a
             topography of the brain but a topology of neuroscience. A
             social network analysis of titles and abstracts for
             cognitive neuroscience articles yields a topology of brain
             regions and functions. This map can help researchers
             identify underresearched areas (e.g., the thalamus) or areas
             that are oversaturated (e.g., the amygdala). The map also
             helps researchers identify subdisciplines, such as
             “neuroeconomics,” that have not yet integrated with the
             broader field–“islands” where rhetorical work could
             yield benefits.},
   Doi = {10.1007/978-3-319-51268-6_7},
   Key = {fds330473}
}

@misc{fds320204,
   Author = {Zielinski, DJ and Rao, HM and Potter, ND and Appelbaum, LG and Kopper,
             R},
   Title = {Evaluating the Effects of Image Persistence on Dynamic
             Target Acquisition in Low Frame Rate Virtual
             Environments},
   Journal = {2016 Ieee Virtual Reality Posters},
   Volume = {2016-July},
   Pages = {319-320},
   Publisher = {IEEE},
   Year = {2016},
   ISBN = {9781509008360},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/VR.2016.7504782},
   Abstract = {Here we explore a visual display technique for low frame
             rate virtual environments called low persistence (LP). This
             involves displaying the rendered frame for a single display
             frame and blanking the screen while waiting for the next
             frame to be generated. To gain greater knowledge about the
             LP technique, we have conducted a user study to evaluate
             user performance and learning during a dynamic target
             acquisition task. The task involved the acquisition of
             targets moving along several different trajectories, modeled
             after a shotgun trap shooting task. The results of our study
             indicate the LP condition approaches high frame rate
             performance within certain classes of target trajectories.
             Interestingly we also see that learning is consistent across
             conditions, indicating that it may not always be necessary
             to train under a visually high frame rate
             system.},
   Doi = {10.1109/VR.2016.7504782},
   Key = {fds320204}
}

@misc{fds270445,
   Author = {Martin, RS and Appelbaum, LG and Huettel, SA and Woldorff,
             MG},
   Title = {NEURAL SIGNATURES OF VALUE-DRIVEN ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE
             PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN ECONOMIC
             CHOICE},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Pages = {208-208},
   Publisher = {M I T PRESS},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000317030501166&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds270445}
}

@misc{fds270446,
   Author = {Donohue, SE and Appelbaum, LG and Woldorff, MG},
   Title = {THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSE NUMBER AND TASK ON THE
             ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF CONFLICT
             PROCESSING},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Pages = {102-102},
   Publisher = {M I T PRESS},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000317030500377&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Key = {fds270446}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Postdocs * Reload * Login