Psychology and Neuroscience Faculty Database
Psychology and Neuroscience
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > pn > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Sarah E. Gaither    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Journal Articles   
@article{fds376854,
   Author = {Park, LE and Aknin, LB and Gaither, SE and Impett, EA and Whillans,
             AV},
   Title = {Starting and sustaining fruitful collaborations in
             psychology},
   Journal = {Social and Personality Psychology Compass},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {4},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12950},
   Abstract = {Much of psychological science relies on collaboration—from
             generating new theories and study ideas, to collecting and
             analyzing data, to writing and sharing results with the
             broader community. Learning how to collaborate with others
             is an important skill, yet this process is not often
             explicitly discussed in academia. Here, five researchers
             from diverse backgrounds share their experiences and advice
             on starting and sustaining collaborations. In doing so, they
             reflect on aspects of both successful (and failed)
             collaborations with students, colleagues within and outside
             of psychology, and members of industry and organizational
             partners beyond academia. Recommendations and challenges of
             productive collaborations are discussed, along with examples
             of how collaborative teams can contribute to psychological
             science, address real-world issues, and make the process of
             conducting research more enjoyable and rewarding.},
   Doi = {10.1111/spc3.12950},
   Key = {fds376854}
}

@article{fds372782,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and Muñoz, M and Kinzler, K and Woodward, A and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {Hypodescent or ingroup overexclusion?: Children's and
             adults' racial categorization of ambiguous black/white
             biracial faces.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {e13450},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13450},
   Abstract = {Two processes describe racially ambiguous Black/White
             Biracial categorization-the one-drop rule, or hypodescent,
             whereby racially ambiguous people are categorized as members
             of their socially subordinated racial group (i.e.,
             Black/White Biracial faces categorized as Black) and the
             ingroup overexclusion effect, whereby racially ambiguous
             people are categorized as members of a salient outgroup,
             regardless of the group's status. Without developmental
             research with racially diverse samples, it is unclear when
             these categorization patterns emerge. Study 1 included
             White, Black, and racially diverse Biracial children (aged
             3- to 7-years) and their parents to test how racial group
             membership and social context influence face categorization
             biases. To provide the clearest test of hypodescent and
             ingroup overexclusion, White participants came from majority
             White neighborhoods and Black participants from majority
             Black neighborhoods (with Biracial participants from more
             racially diverse neighborhoods)-two samples with prominent
             racial ingroups. Study 2 aimed to replicate the parent
             findings with a separate sample of White, Black, Black/White
             Biracial, and Asian adults. Results suggest the ingroup
             overexclusion effect is present across populations early in
             development and persists into adulthood. Additionally,
             categorization was meaningfully related to parental context,
             pinpointing a pathway that potentially contributes to
             ingroup overexclusion. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: White, Black,
             and racially diverse Biracial children and adults tended to
             categorize racially ambiguous Black/White Biracial faces as
             racial outgroup members, even if the outgroup was White.
             This contradicts most work arguing Black/White Biracial
             racially ambiguous people are more often seen as Black.
             Children and parents' categorizations were related, though
             children's categorizations were not related to socialization
             above and beyond parents' categorizations. Children showed
             similar categorization patterns across dichotomous and
             continuous measures.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.13450},
   Key = {fds372782}
}

@article{fds374966,
   Author = {Straka, BC and Albuja, A and Leer, J and Brauher, K and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {The rich get richer? Children's reasoning about
             socioeconomic status predicts inclusion and resource
             bias.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {60},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {505-521},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001655},
   Abstract = {Children's socioeconomic status (SES) is linked to disparate
             access to resources and affects social behaviors such as
             inclusion and resource allocations. Yet it is unclear
             whether children's essentialized view of SES (i.e.,
             believing SES is immutable) or subjective social status
             (SSS) influences behavioral biases toward high- versus
             low-SES peers. We measured 4- to 9-year-old children's SES
             essentialism and SSS to test whether these predict inclusion
             and resource allocations to high- versus low-SES peers
             (<i>N</i> = 127; from a midsize city in the Southeastern
             United States; 49.6% female; parent-reported 54.2% White,
             2.8% Black, 8.3% Latine, 5.6% Asian, 1.4% another race,
             27.8% multiracial, 43.3% not provided). We also compared
             children's SES beliefs to their parent's. Children's SES
             essentialism and SSS decreased across the ages tested, and
             children reported higher SSS than their parents. Parents'
             SES essentialism predicted younger (but not older)
             children's SES essentialism. Moreover, SES essentialism
             mediated the negative relationship between age and
             preference for including high-SES peers, while SSS mediated
             the negative relationship between age and preference for
             allocating more resources to high-SES peers. This suggests
             that beliefs about the nature of SES may influence
             sociorelational behavior like including or excluding others,
             while perceived social status (SSS) may influence resource
             allocations. Furthermore, older children and those with
             lower SES essentialism included low- versus high-SES peers
             more often while older and lower SSS children distributed
             more resources toward low- versus high-SES peers. Thus,
             children's SES essentialism and SSS may also influence their
             behaviors to either perpetuate or rectify inequality.
             (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0001655},
   Key = {fds374966}
}

@article{fds374322,
   Author = {Hantzmon, SV and Davenport, CA and Das Gupta and MN and Adekunle, TA and Gaither, SE and Olsen, MK and Pinheiro, SO and Johnson, KS and Mahoney,
             H and Falls, A and Lloyd, L and Pollak, KI},
   Title = {Race differences in patient trust and distrust from
             audio-recorded cardiology encounters.},
   Journal = {Patient Educ Couns},
   Volume = {119},
   Pages = {108083},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2023.108083},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: Many have reported racial disparities in
             self-reported trust in clinicians but have not directly
             assessed expressions of trust and distrust in
             physician-patient encounters. We created a codebook to
             examine racial differences in patient trust and distrust
             through audio-recorded cardiologist-patient interactions.
             METHODS: We analyzed data from a randomized controlled trial
             of audio-recorded outpatient cardiology encounters (50 White
             and 51 Black patients). We created a codebook for trust and
             distrust that was applied to recordings between White
             cardiologists and White and Black patients. We assessed
             differences in trust, distrust, and guardedness while
             adjusting for patient age, sex, and first appointment with
             the cardiologist. RESULTS: Compared to White patients, Black
             patients had significantly lower expressions of trust ([IRR]
             [95 % CI]: 0.59 [0.41, 0.84]) and a significantly lower mean
             guarded/open score ([β] [95 % CI] -0.38 [-0.71, -0.04]).
             There was no statistically significant association between
             race and odds of at least one distrustful expression (OR [95
             % CI] 1.36 [0.37, 4.94]). CONCLUSION AND PRACTICE
             IMPLICATIONS: We found that coders can reliably identify
             patient expressions of trust and distrust rather than
             relying on problematic self-reported measures. Results
             suggest that White clinicians can improve their
             communication with Black patients to increase expressions of
             trust.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.pec.2023.108083},
   Key = {fds374322}
}

@article{fds355828,
   Author = {Tsai, A and Straka, B and Gaither, S},
   Title = {Mixed-heritage individuals’ encounters with
             raciolinguistic ideologies},
   Journal = {Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural
             Development},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {507-521},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2021.1904964},
   Abstract = {Mixed-heritage individuals (MHIs) are known to face high
             levels of social exclusion. Here, we investigate how
             raciolinguistic ideologies related to one’s heritage
             language abilities add to these exclusionary experiences.
             The results from 293 MHIs reveal frequent experiences of
             marginalisation from members of each of their heritage
             communities because their racial appearance and language
             practices are perceived as deviant and outside imagined
             ‘monoracial’ norms. Specifically, over half of
             respondents described experiences of exclusion for not
             speaking their minority heritage languages with the same
             accent or manner or fluency associated with ‘monoracial’
             native speakers of their heritage languages or dialects.
             Another subset described high pressure to speak ‘proper
             English’ in White dominant work environments. These
             results extend past MHI work by empirically documenting the
             ‘monoracial-only’, monoglossic, and ‘Standard
             English’ ideologies that contribute to the continued
             social exclusion of MHIs.},
   Doi = {10.1080/01434632.2021.1904964},
   Key = {fds355828}
}

@article{fds371657,
   Author = {Adekunle, TA and Knowles, JM and Hantzmon, SV and DasGupta, MN and Pollak, KI and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {A qualitative analysis of trust and distrust within
             patient-clinician interactions.},
   Journal = {PEC Innov},
   Volume = {3},
   Pages = {100187},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pecinn.2023.100187},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVES: Trust represents a key quality of strong
             clinician-patient relationships.1 Many have attempted to
             assess patient-reported trust. However, most trust measures
             suffer from ceiling effects, with no variability, making it
             not possible to examine predictors of trust and distrust.
             Rather than rely on patient reports, we created a codebook
             for instances of trust and distrust from actual
             patient-clinician encounters. METHODS: Three coders
             conducted a qualitative analysis of audio recordings among
             patient-cardiologist outpatient encounters. RESULTS: We
             identified trust and distrust based on vocal and verbal cues
             in the interactions. We found consistent patterns that
             indicated patient trust and distrust. CONCLUSION: Overall,
             this work empirically validates a new more accurate
             measurement of trust for patient-doctor interactions.
             INNOVATION: We are the first to use audio recordings to
             identify verbal markers of trust and distrust in
             patient-clinician interactions. From this work, others can
             code trust and distrust in recorded encounters rather than
             rely on self-report measures.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.pecinn.2023.100187},
   Key = {fds371657}
}

@article{fds368905,
   Author = {Stanaland, A and Gaither, S and Gassman-Pines,
             A},
   Title = {When Is Masculinity "Fragile"? An Expectancy-Discrepancy-Threat
             Model of Masculine Identity.},
   Journal = {Personality and social psychology review : an official
             journal of the Society for Personality and Social
             Psychology, Inc},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {359-377},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/10888683221141176},
   Abstract = {<h4>Academic abstract</h4>Manhood is a precarious social
             status. Under perceived gender identity threat, men are
             disproportionately likely to enact certain
             stereotype-consistent responses such as aggression to
             maintain their gender status. Yet less is known regarding
             individual variation in men's threat responsiveness-that is,
             the psychological conditions under which one's masculine
             identity is more or less "fragile." We propose a novel model
             of masculine identity whereby masculine norm expectancy
             generates discrepancy within the self to the extent that
             rigid norms are internalized as obligational (actual-ought
             discrepancy) versus aspirational (actual-ideal discrepancy),
             which predict extrinsic versus intrinsic motivations to
             reduce these discrepancies, respectively. Under threat,
             then, extrinsic motivations predict externalized responses
             (e.g., aggression), and intrinsic motivations elicit
             internalized responses (e.g., anxiety, shame, self-harm). We
             also consider the conditions under which masculinity may be
             less fragile-for example, in contexts with less rigid
             expectations and among men who reject expectations-as
             pathways to mitigate adverse masculinity threat-related
             outcomes.<h4>Public abstract</h4>In many cultures, men prove
             their manhood by engaging in behaviors that harm themselves
             and others (e.g., violence, sexism, homophobia),
             particularly people from marginalized groups. Yet less is
             known about why some men are more likely than others to
             enact these masculinity-proving behaviors. The goal of our
             model is to specify certain conditions under which
             masculinities become "fragile" and elicit these responses
             when under threat. We start by describing the rigid
             expectations men experience-for example, that they are
             strong and tough. We propose that these expectations cause
             men to experience different forms of discrepancy within
             themselves that produce corresponding motivations to reduce
             these discrepancies. Under threat, motivations driven by
             others' expectations elicit outward attempts to restore
             masculine status (e.g., aggression), whereas motivations
             driven by self-ideals cause internalized responses (e.g.,
             shame, self-harm). We conclude by discussing how to reduce
             these discrepancies, such as mitigating the rigidity of and
             encouraging men's resistance to masculinity
             expectations.},
   Doi = {10.1177/10888683221141176},
   Key = {fds368905}
}

@article{fds372973,
   Author = {Halim, MLD and Atwood, S and Osornio, AC and Pauker, K and Dunham, Y and Olson, KR and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Parent and self-socialization of gender intergroup
             attitudes, perceptions, and behaviors among ethnically and
             geographically diverse young children.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {59},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {1933-1950},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001586},
   Abstract = {Previous work has shown the robust nature of gender bias in
             both children and adults. However, much less attention has
             been paid toward understanding what factors shape these
             biases. The current preregistered study used parent surveys
             and child interviews to test whether parents' conversations
             with their children about and modeling of gender intergroup
             relations and/or children's self-guided interests about
             gender (self-socialization) contribute to the formation of
             gender attitudes, status perceptions, and gender intergroup
             behaviors among young 4- to 6-year-old children. Our
             participant sample also allowed us to explore variation by
             child gender, ethnicity (Asian-, Black-, Latiné-, and
             White-American), and U.S. geographical region (Northeast,
             Pacific Northwest, West, Southeast, and Hawaii). Data
             suggest that children whose parents reported they were
             especially active in seeking information about gender tended
             to allocate more resources to same-gender versus
             other-gender children and expressed less positive
             evaluations of other-gender children in comparison to
             children who were less active. By contrast, we found that
             parents' conversations with their children about gender
             intergroup relations and about gender-play stereotypes
             showed few connections with children's gender attitudes. In
             terms of demographic differences, boys raised in households
             with more unequal versus equal division of labor perceived
             that men had higher status than women, but few differences
             by ethnicity or geographic region emerged. In sum, our study
             suggests that both self- and parent socialization processes
             are at play in shaping early gender attitudes, status
             perceptions, and gender intergroup behavior, although
             self-socialization seemed to play a larger role. (PsycInfo
             Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0001586},
   Key = {fds372973}
}

@article{fds372452,
   Author = {Chen, JM and Meyers, C and Pauker, K and Gaither, SE and Hamilton, DL and Sherman, JW},
   Title = {Intergroup Context Moderates the Impact of White Americans'
             Identification on Racial Categorization of Ambiguous
             Faces.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Pages = {1461672231190264},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01461672231190264},
   Abstract = {We examined how the number of groups in a categorization
             task influences how White Americans categorize ambiguous
             faces. We investigated the strength of <i>identity-driven
             ingroup overexclusion</i>-wherein highly identified
             perceivers overexclude ambiguous members from the
             ingroup-proposing that, compared with dichotomous tasks
             (with only the ingroup and one outgroup), tasks with more
             outgroups attenuate identity-driven ingroup overexclusion (a
             dilution effect). Fourteen studies (<i>n</i> = 4,001)
             measured White Americans' racial identification and their
             categorizations of ambiguous faces and manipulated the
             categorization task to have two groups, three groups, or an
             unspecified number of groups (open-ended). In all three
             conditions, participants overexcluded faces from the White
             category on average. There was limited support for the
             dilution effect: identity-driven ingroup overexclusion was
             absent in the three-group task and only weakly supported in
             the open-ended task. The presence of multiple outgroups may
             dampen the impact of racial identity on race perceptions
             among White Americans.},
   Doi = {10.1177/01461672231190264},
   Key = {fds372452}
}

@article{fds361951,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Chen, C-M and Neal, S and Chien, SH-L},
   Title = {Children's cross-cultural categorizations of racially
             ambiguous faces in Taiwan and the U.S.},
   Journal = {Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {385-396},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000513},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>Racially ambiguous face categorization
             research is growing in prominence, and yet the majority of
             this work has focused on White and Western samples and has
             primarily used biracial Black/White stimuli. Past findings
             suggest that biracial Black/White faces are more often seen
             as Black than White, but without testing these perceptions
             with other groups, generalizability cannot be
             guaranteed.<h4>Methods</h4>We tested 3-7-year-old Asian
             children living in Taiwan-an Eastern cultural context
             (<i>N</i> = 74)-and Asian children living in the U.S.-a
             Western cultural context (<i>N</i> = 65) to explore the role
             that cultural group membership may play in biracial
             perceptions. Children categorized 12 racially ambiguous
             biracial Black/White faces and 12 biracial Asian/White faces
             in a dichotomous forced-choice task and completed a racial
             constancy measurement.<h4>Results</h4>Regarding biracial
             Black/White faces, Taiwanese and Asian American children
             both categorized the faces as White significantly more often
             compared to chance levels, regardless of racial constancy
             beliefs. For biracial Asian/White faces, Taiwanese children
             with racial constancy beliefs categorized the faces
             significantly more often as White, whereas Taiwanese
             children without racial constancy beliefs categorized the
             faces significantly more often as Asian. However, Asian
             American children did not show a bias in categorizing
             biracial Asian/White faces.<h4>Conclusions</h4>Results
             suggest that hyperdescent over hypodescent for more commonly
             studied biracial Black/White faces generalizes in both
             cultural contexts. However, biracial Asian/White stimuli may
             be perceived in more fixed-like patterns in predominately
             Asian contexts, since only Taiwanese children showed
             increased outgroup categorizations once racial constancy
             beliefs were endorsed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023
             APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/cdp0000513},
   Key = {fds361951}
}

@article{fds363071,
   Author = {Levy, A and Nguyen, C and Slepian, ML and Gaither, S and Pauker, K and Dovidio, JF},
   Title = {Categorizing a Face and Facing a Category: The Constructive
             Impacts of Ambiguity and Uncertainty in Racial
             Categorization.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {910-924},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01461672221084537},
   Abstract = {The past generation has seen a dramatic rise in multiracial
             populations and a consequent increase in exposure to
             individuals who challenge monolithic racial categories. We
             examine and compare two potential outcomes of the
             multiracial population growth that may impact people's
             racial categorization experience: (a) exposure to racially
             ambiguous faces that visually challenge the existing
             categories, and (b) a category that conceptually challenges
             existing categories (including "biracial" as an option in
             addition to the monolithic "Black" and "White" categories).
             Across four studies (<i>N</i> = 1,810), we found that
             multiple exposures to faces that are racially ambiguous
             directly lower essentialist views of race. Moreover, we
             found that when people consider a category that blurs the
             line between racial categories (i.e., "biracial"), they
             become less certain in their racial categorization, which is
             associated with less race essentialism, as well.
             Importantly, we found that these two effects happen
             independently from one another and represent two distinct
             cognitive processes.},
   Doi = {10.1177/01461672221084537},
   Key = {fds363071}
}

@article{fds370891,
   Author = {Chen, EE and Ma, S and Bala, M and Groves, JM and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {Do we perceive ethnic ingroup members as wealthier?
             Examining Hong Kong children's inferences of wealth status
             based on resources and ethnic group membership},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {66},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101334},
   Abstract = {The way in which children consider information may depend on
             the groups to which they belong and the social status those
             groups occupy. In three studies, we examined how children
             infer their peers’ wealth status based on the possessions
             present and ethnic group membership. We recruited 242 Hong
             Kong children (Mage = 5;11, SDage = 1;0) from the ethnic
             Chinese majority (Study 1: 115 children) and two ethnic
             minorities (Study 2a: 84 South Asian/Southeast Asian
             children; Study 2b: 43 White/Biracial children). Overall,
             participants were able to distinguish between low- and
             high-wealth items, and make correct wealth-based inferences.
             Compared to both groups of ethnic minority children, ethnic
             majority children tend to judge their ingroup peers as
             wealthier even when they possessed fewer resources than
             outgroup peers. Thus, children inferred peer wealth status
             based on the resources present, but such inference was
             impacted by their own ethnic group membership and
             socioeconomic status.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101334},
   Key = {fds370891}
}

@article{fds367774,
   Author = {Meyers, C and Gaither, SE and Remedios, J and Pauker,
             K},
   Title = {Detecting biracial identity strength: Perceived
             phenotypicality is inaccurate},
   Journal = {Self and Identity},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {533-562},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2022.2146742},
   Abstract = {Past work on Black and Latinx individuals demonstrates that
             observers can accurately predict an individual’s racial
             identity strength based on the observers’ perceptions of
             the individual’s phenotypic prototypicality (how much
             someone looks like a prototypical member of their racial
             group). However, the growing Biracial demographic varies
             considerably in racial identification, suggesting a
             monoracial approach to infer racial identity strength may
             not translate to Biracial individuals. In three studies,
             Biracial Black/White participants were photographed and
             completed a racial identity strength scale. Subsequently, we
             had raters judge the Biracial targets’ phenotypic
             prototypicality and perceived levels racial identity
             strength. Overall, perceivers could not accurately predict
             Biracial individuals’ racial identity strength via their
             phenotypic prototypicality.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15298868.2022.2146742},
   Key = {fds367774}
}

@article{fds374920,
   Author = {Halim, MLD and Glazier, JJ and Martinez, MA and Stanaland, A and Gaither, SE and Dunham, Y and Pauker, K and Olson,
             KR},
   Title = {Gender attitudes and gender discrimination among ethnically
             and geographically diverse young children},
   Journal = {Infant and Child Development},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.2482},
   Abstract = {Despite increasing advocacy for gender equality, gender
             prejudice and discrimination persist. The origins of these
             biases develop in early childhood, but it is less clear
             whether (1) children's gender attitudes predict
             discrimination and (2) gender attitudes and discrimination
             vary by ethnicity and US region. We examine these questions
             with an ethnically (Asian, Black, Latinx and White) and
             geographically (Northeast, Pacific Northwest, West,
             Southeast and Hawaii) diverse sample of 4- to 6-year-old
             children (N = 605) who completed measures of gender
             attitudes and discrimination in a preregistered study.
             Children, across groups, demonstrated more positive
             attitudes towards their gender ingroup. Children who showed
             more pro-ingroup attitudes also showed more pro-ingroup
             behavioural discrimination. Girls showed stronger ingroup
             favouritism than boys, but ethnic and regional groups
             generally did not vary in levels of bias. These findings
             contribute to our understanding of how gender intergroup
             biases develop and highlight the generalizability of these
             processes.},
   Doi = {10.1002/icd.2482},
   Key = {fds374920}
}

@article{fds366202,
   Author = {Suh, Y and Perlin, JD and Turner, AF and West, AL and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {The good life in cultural context: Examining Asian young
             adults’ psychological well-being and narratives of
             negative experiences},
   Journal = {Journal of Research in Personality},
   Volume = {101},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2022.104282},
   Abstract = {Few narrative identity studies have investigated how common
             correlates of well-being in the U.S. (redemption,
             contamination, agency, and communion) relate to the good
             life for non-U.S., racial minority populations. The current
             study assessed whether Asian young adults’ (N = 197)
             degree of acculturation to Euro-American values is
             associated with both the use of these narrative themes and
             how these themes relate to well-being. Asian young adults
             narrated two negative events, one pertaining to their ethnic
             identity and another pertaining to a low point in the life
             story. Narratives were reliably coded for redemption,
             contamination, agency, and communion. While there was no
             association between narrative themes and acculturation,
             agency was associated with lower well-being for less
             acculturated individuals. Results suggest that past studies
             documenting a positive relation between PWB and agency may
             not generalize to Asian samples.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jrp.2022.104282},
   Key = {fds366202}
}

@article{fds367279,
   Author = {Walker, DC and Gaither, SE and De Los Santos and B and Keigan, J and Schaefer, LM and Thompson, JK},
   Title = {Development and validation of a measure of curvy ideals
             internalization.},
   Journal = {Body image},
   Volume = {43},
   Pages = {217-231},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.09.005},
   Abstract = {Several sociocultural female body ideals exist - thin,
             muscular/athletic, and, more recently, curvier ideals, which
             research specifically suggests are more prevalent among
             Black women. Two validated measures assess women's desire
             for curvier bodies, but neither assess certain facets of
             curvy ideals (e.g., thick vs. slim-thick) separately. We
             developed and validated the Curvy Ideals Internalization
             (CII) Scale, to be used alone or alongside existing measures
             of appearance ideal internalization. Focus groups among
             racially/ethnically diverse women informed initial items. A
             sample of 897 White (37.1%), Black (34.2%), and biracial
             Black and White (28.7%) U.S. women completed the initial
             37-item CII to determine factor structure, narrow the item
             pool, and examine validity and reliability. A separate
             sample (N = 366) of U.S. Black, White, and biracial women
             completed the CII to confirm the factor structure. The final
             CII has eleven items, with factors assessing thick/curvy
             ideal internalization and facets of slim-thick ideal
             internalization: thin waist and large breast size. The CII
             has adequate internal consistency, test-retest reliability,
             construct validity, and factorial validity. The CII is
             appropriate for use among Black, White, and biracial women
             to assess internalization of curvier body ideals and needs
             to be validated in more diverse samples.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.09.005},
   Key = {fds367279}
}

@article{fds363243,
   Author = {Leslie, GJ and Masuoka, N and Gaither, SE and Remedios, JD and Chyei
             Vinluan, A},
   Title = {Voter Evaluations of Biracial‐Identified Political
             Candidates},
   Journal = {Social Sciences},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/socsci11040171},
   Abstract = {Today, identity expression and acceptance represent an
             important area of political advocacy and representation.
             Yet, how responsive are voters to new racial identity cues
             promoted by political leaders? Using candidates with
             interracial backgrounds as a case study, we assess whether
             voters are responsive to candidates who assert a
             mixed‐race identity or if voters primarily rely on other
             traits, such as the candidate’s family background, in
             determining their support of that candidate. Using an
             experimental design, this study presents participants with
             various hypothetical candidates who vary both in their
             racial heritages (i.e., candidates with Asian and White
             interracial parents or Black and White interracial parents)
             and identity choices (i.e., as single‐race minority,
             single‐race White, or biracial). We then compare how the
             mixed‐race, single‐race minority, and White participants
             evaluate the candidate. We expect that the mixed‐race
             participants will be most supportive of candidates who
             signal a common in‐group identity by identifying
             specifically as “bi-racial”. On the other hand, the
             single‐race minority and White participants should be more
             likely to adhere to the one‐drop rule or hypodescent in
             their evaluations, meaning they will provide more positive
             evaluations of interracial candidates who identify as a
             single‐race minority. Our study finds that the
             single‐race minority and White participants completely
             overlook racial identity cues and instead focus on the
             description of the candidate’s family heritage along with
             their own assumptions about hypodescent. The mixed‐race
             participants, on the other hand, show strong support for
             biracial‐identified, in‐group political candidates This
             study adds to a burgeoning literature on racial perception
             and on political representation.},
   Doi = {10.3390/socsci11040171},
   Key = {fds363243}
}

@article{fds359472,
   Author = {Rosenfeld, DL and Balcetis, E and Bastian, B and Berkman, ET and Bosson,
             JK and Brannon, TN and Burrow, AL and Cameron, CD and Chen, S and Cook, JE and Crandall, C and Davidai, S and Dhont, K and Eastwick, PW and Gaither,
             SE and Gangestad, SW and Gilovich, T and Gray, K and Haines, EL and Haselton, MG and Haslam, N and Hodson, G and Hogg, MA and Hornsey, MJ and Huo, YJ and Joel, S and Kachanoff, FJ and Kraft-Todd, G and Leary, MR and Ledgerwood, A and Lee, RT and Loughnan, S and MacInnis, CC and Mann, T and Murray, DR and Parkinson, C and Pérez, EO and Pyszczynski, T and Ratner, K and Rothgerber, H and Rounds, JD and Schaller, M and Silver,
             RC and Spellman, BA and Strohminger, N and Swim, JK and Thoemmes, F and Urganci, B and Vandello, JA and Volz, S and Zayas, V and Tomiyama,
             AJ},
   Title = {Psychological Science in the Wake of COVID-19: Social,
             Methodological, and Metascientific Considerations.},
   Journal = {Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the
             Association for Psychological Science},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {311-333},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691621999374},
   Abstract = {The COVID-19 pandemic has extensively changed the state of
             psychological science from what research questions
             psychologists can ask to which methodologies psychologists
             can use to investigate them. In this article, we offer a
             perspective on how to optimize new research in the
             pandemic's wake. Because this pandemic is inherently a
             social phenomenon-an event that hinges on human-to-human
             contact-we focus on socially relevant subfields of
             psychology. We highlight specific psychological phenomena
             that have likely shifted as a result of the pandemic and
             discuss theoretical, methodological, and practical
             considerations of conducting research on these phenomena.
             After this discussion, we evaluate metascientific issues
             that have been amplified by the pandemic. We aim to
             demonstrate how theoretically grounded views on the COVID-19
             pandemic can help make psychological science stronger-not
             weaker-in its wake.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1745691621999374},
   Key = {fds359472}
}

@article{fds365318,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Sims, JP},
   Title = {How Cross-Discipline Understanding and Communication Can
             Improve Research on Multiracial Populations},
   Journal = {Social Sciences},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {90-90},
   Publisher = {MDPI AG},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/SOCSCI11030090},
   Abstract = {One of the strengths of Critical Mixed Race Studies is that
             it represents research methodolo-gies and frameworks from
             multiple disciplines across the social sciences and
             humanities. However, if these disciplines are not in
             dialogue with each other, that benefit may be lost. Here, we
             use psychological and sociological research on Multiracial
             populations as examples to argue how strict disciplinarity
             and methodological trends may limit scientific production.
             We propose that reading and citing work across disciplines,
             expanding methodological training, and rejecting hegemonic
             “white logic” assumptions about what is
             “publishable” can enhance Multiracial research. First,
             the ability to cite effectively across disciplines will
             shorten the time it takes for new theories to be developed
             that focus on empirically underrepresented populations.
             Secondly, increasing understanding of both quantitative and
             qualitative methods will allow more effective reading
             between disciplines while also creating opportunities to
             engage with both causality and the richness of experiences
             that comprise being Multiracial. Finally, these changes
             would then situate scholars to be more effective reviewers,
             thereby enhancing the peer-reviewed publication process to
             one that routinely rejects color evasive racist practices
             that privilege work on majority populations.},
   Doi = {10.3390/SOCSCI11030090},
   Key = {fds365318}
}

@article{fds362938,
   Author = {Paul, M and Gaither, SE and Darity, W},
   Title = {About Face: Seeing Class and Race},
   Journal = {Journal of Economic Issues},
   Volume = {56},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-17},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00213624.2022.2008750},
   Abstract = {People’s social class, and the perceptions of their social
             class are embedded in an institutional context that has
             important ramifications for one’s life opportunities and
             outcomes. Research on first impressions has found that
             people are relatively accurate at judging a variety of
             traits such as perceived sexual orientation and income, but
             there is a paucity of research that investigates whether
             people are also accurate at judging wealth or class. In this
             article, we first investigate whether people understand the
             distinction between income and wealth. Then, using a novel
             dataset, we examine whether people are accurate at
             identifying the income and wealth levels of individuals
             across racial and ethnic groups by facial cues alone. We
             find that participants understand the meaning of income, but
             not wealth. Additionally, we find that perceivers categorize
             class more accurately than by sheer chance, using minimal
             facial cues, but perceivers are particularly inaccurate when
             categorizing high-income and high-wealth Black and Latinx
             subjects.},
   Doi = {10.1080/00213624.2022.2008750},
   Key = {fds362938}
}

@article{fds362937,
   Author = {Ruba, A and Mcmutry, R and Gaither, S and Wilbourn,
             M},
   Title = {How White American Children Develop Racial Biases in Emotion
             Reasoning},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {21-33},
   Booktitle = {PsyArXiv},
   Year = {2022},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ng4fw},
   Abstract = {For decades, affective scientists have examined how adults
             and children reason about others' emotions. Yet, our
             knowledge is limited regarding how emotion reasoning is
             impacted by race-that is, how individuals reason about
             emotions displayed by people of other racial groups. In this
             review, we examine the developmental origins of racial
             biases in emotion reasoning, focusing on how White Americans
             reason about emotions displayed by Black faces/people. We
             highlight how racial biases in emotion reasoning, which
             emerge as early as infancy, likely contribute to
             miscommunications, inaccurate social perceptions, and
             negative interracial interactions across the lifespan. We
             conclude by discussing promising interventions to reduce
             these biases as well as future research directions,
             highlighting how affective scientists can decenter Whiteness
             in their research designs. Together, this review highlights
             how emotion reasoning is a potentially affective component
             of racial bias among White Americans.},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/ng4fw},
   Key = {fds362937}
}

@article{fds351482,
   Author = {Rozek, CS and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Not Quite White or Black: Biracial Students’ Perceptions
             of Threat and Belonging Across School Contexts},
   Journal = {Journal of Early Adolescence},
   Volume = {41},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1308-1337},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431620950476},
   Abstract = {Stereotype threat posits that students who are members of
             negatively stereotyped groups in school should feel more
             threat and less belonging, especially in schools with large
             achievement disparities and low racial/ethnic minority
             representation. This research has focused primarily on the
             experiences of negatively stereotyped monoracial minority
             students, but for a biracial Black/White student who claims
             both a negatively stereotyped (e.g., Black) and a positively
             stereotyped (e.g., White) identity, do these outcomes vary?
             We assessed 1,399 biracial Black/White, monoracial Black,
             and monoracial White middle school students’ perceptions
             of threat and belonging in school, across four
             lower-stereotype-salient schools (i.e., racially diverse
             schools) and seven higher-stereotype-salient schools (i.e.,
             racially homogeneous schools). Biracial students reported a
             similar amount of threat across school contexts, whereas
             monoracial students’ threat was differentially context
             dependent. These findings suggest biracial students may face
             unique identity-related threats in school and point to a
             need to develop supports specific to their
             experiences.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0272431620950476},
   Key = {fds351482}
}

@article{fds355089,
   Author = {Stanaland, A and Gaither, S},
   Title = {"Be a Man": The Role of Social Pressure in Eliciting Men's
             Aggressive Cognition.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {1596-1611},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167220984298},
   Abstract = {Threatening a man's manhood-but not a woman's
             womanhood-elicits aggression. In two studies, we found
             evidence that this aggression is related to the social
             pressure men experience to "be a man." In Study 1a, we
             conducted an exploratory factor analysis to isolate
             participants' (<i>N</i> = 195; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> =
             19.92) differential motivations for conforming to gender
             norms. Study 1b then showed that pressure to be masculine
             moderates the relationship between gender identity threat
             and aggressive cognition for men. In Study 2a, we conducted
             a confirmatory factor analysis to validate the
             aforementioned scales with an age-diverse sample of men
             (<i>N</i> = 391; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 33.16, range =
             18-56 years). Study 2b replicated Study 1b, most notably
             with younger men. In all, these findings reveal one
             pathway-the pressure men experience to be stereotypically
             masculine-that elicits aggressive cognition when under
             threat in a U.S. context.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0146167220984298},
   Key = {fds355089}
}

@article{fds359274,
   Author = {Straka, BC and Stanaland, A and Tomasello, M and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {Who can be in a group? 3- to 5-year-old children construe
             realistic social groups through mutual intentionality},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {60},
   Pages = {101097-101097},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101097},
   Abstract = {Recent research suggests that young children's causal
             justification for minimal group membership can be induced
             via a cognitive framework of mutual intentionality. That is,
             an individual can become a group member when both the
             individual and group agree to membership. Here, we
             investigated if children ages 3–5 understand groups formed
             by mutual intentions and whether they apply mutual
             intentions to realistic groups with varying entitative and
             essentialized qualities. In two studies (N = 197), we asked
             3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children if a novel character could
             join an existing group based on intentionality (mutual,
             individual-, group-only) and group type (task, friends,
             family). We find that 4- and 5-year-olds robustly relied on
             mutual intentions to constitute group membership and
             3-year-olds also demonstrated emerging usage of this
             cognitive framework. Moreover, children employed mutual
             intentionality across different group types, suggesting a
             general framework for children's understanding of different
             social groups.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101097},
   Key = {fds359274}
}

@article{fds356798,
   Author = {Albuja, A and Straka, B and Desjardins, M and Swartzwelder, HS and Gaither, S},
   Title = {Alcohol use and related consequences for monoracial and
             multiracial Native American/American Indian college
             students.},
   Journal = {Exp Clin Psychopharmacol},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {487-500},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pha0000475},
   Abstract = {Native American/American Indian (NA/AI) and Multiracial
             people (those who claim multiple racial identities) report
             notably high alcohol use compared to other racial groups in
             the United States. Nearly half of the NA/AI population is
             also Multiracial, yet NA/AI and Multiracial college students
             report different motivations for drinking alcohol.
             Therefore, it remains unclear if NA/AI individuals who are
             also Multiracial are at different risk for alcohol use and
             negative alcohol-related consequences, and if there are
             distinct patterns of risk factors in these understudied
             populations. Because college-aged students are at risk for
             high levels of alcohol use, this exploratory study used the
             AlcoholEdu for College™ survey to compare the association
             between initial drinking age, college location (urban vs.
             rural), and alcohol use motivations and consequences between
             monoracial NA/AI (N = 2,363) and Multiracial NA/AI
             college-aged students (N = 6,172). Monoracial NA/AI students
             reported higher incidences of alcohol use and
             alcohol-related problems such as blacking out and missing
             class, compared to Multiracial NA/AI students. Risk factors
             like earlier age of drinking onset were more strongly
             associated with negative consequences for monoracial NA/AI
             students compared to Multiracial NA/AI students. Despite
             similar levels of Internal Coping motivations for drinking
             (e.g., to feel more confident or sure of yourself),
             monoracial NA/AI students reported drinking more than
             Multiracial students and experienced more negative
             drinking-related outcomes. These results suggest Multiracial
             NA/AI students may draw on protective factors not accessible
             to monoracial NA/AI students, highlighting the need for
             interventions tailored to students at highest risk.
             (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/pha0000475},
   Key = {fds356798}
}

@article{fds351439,
   Author = {Young, DM and Sanchez, DT and Pauker, K and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {A Meta-Analytic Review of Hypodescent Patterns in
             Categorizing Multiracial and Racially Ambiguous
             Targets.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {705-727},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167220941321},
   Abstract = {Research addressing the increasing multiracial population
             (i.e., identifying with two or more races) is rapidly
             expanding. This meta-analysis (<i>k</i> = 55) examines
             categorization patterns consistent with <i>hypodescent</i>,
             or the tendency to categorize multiracial targets as their
             lower status racial group. Subgroup analyses suggest that
             operationalization of multiracial (e.g., presenting photos
             of racially ambiguous faces, or ancestry information sans
             picture), target gender, and categorization measurement
             (e.g., selecting from binary choices: Black or White; or
             multiple categorization options: Black, White, or
             multiracial) moderated categorization patterns.
             Operationalizing multiracial as ancestry, male targets, and
             measuring categorization with binary or multiple Likert-type
             scale outcomes supported hypodescent. However, categorizing
             multiracial targets as not their lower status racial group
             occurred for female targets or multiple categorization
             options. Evidence was mixed on whether perceiver and target
             race were related to categorization patterns. These results
             point to future directions for understanding categorization
             processes and multiracial perception.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0146167220941321},
   Key = {fds351439}
}

@article{fds365319,
   Author = {Leer, J and Gaither, S and Gassman-Pines, A},
   Title = {Behavioral science interventions: Integrating the decision
             task, context, and individual differences to inform
             policy.},
   Journal = {Translational Issues in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {46-54},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tps0000243},
   Doi = {10.1037/tps0000243},
   Key = {fds365319}
}

@article{fds354591,
   Author = {Ng, S and Liu, Y and Gaither, S and Marsan, S and Zucker,
             N},
   Title = {The clash of culture and cuisine: A qualitative exploration
             of cultural tensions and attitudes toward food and body in
             Chinese young adult women.},
   Journal = {Int J Eat Disord},
   Volume = {54},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {174-183},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.23459},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: Accumulating evidence suggests that the
             prevalence of eating disorders among Chinese women is a
             public health concern. Prior studies have drawn linkages
             between conflicting cultural values, identity confusion, and
             eating disorder symptomatology, which may be relevant for
             understanding the rise of eating disorders amidst China's
             rapid economic and sociocultural transformation. Here, we
             explore how women's experiences with traditional eating
             norms and modernizing norms of femininity may shape their
             food and body attitudes. METHOD: Chinese young adult women
             (N = 34; aged 18-22 years) participated in semi-structured
             interviews focusing on experiences with norms surrounding
             eating and ideal feminine appearance, perceived conflict
             between these norms, and their responses to perceived
             conflict. Interviews were conducted via email (n = 27) or
             via Skype (n = 7). Participants were not asked about past
             or present diagnoses of eating disorders. Analysis of
             responses was guided by the principles of thematic analysis.
             RESULTS: Women reported encounters with cultural eating
             norms and feminine appearance norms, and described factors
             that motivated continued or discontinued adherence to these
             norms. Women reported strategies of conflict resolution,
             which resulted in different emotional and behavioral
             outcomes including eating disorder symptoms. DISCUSSION:
             Women's experiences with norms surrounding eating and
             appearance indicate the centrality of these encounters in
             the formation of individual and interpersonal values. Our
             findings suggest the importance for clinicians to assist
             clients in exploring the meanings behind internalized
             attitudes toward food and body, and to help clients balance
             interpersonal and individual needs.},
   Doi = {10.1002/eat.23459},
   Key = {fds354591}
}

@article{fds348707,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Chen, JM and Rule, N},
   Title = {Perceptions and experiences of (people with) unconventional
             identities},
   Journal = {Self and Identity},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {587-593},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2020.1711802},
   Abstract = {Psychology has long focused on social identities and their
             critical role in defining the self. However, the majority of
             identity-related findings stems from research on traditional
             identities (monoracial, cisgender, heterosexual).
             Considering the relative dearth of research from the full
             range of identities encompassed in society (e.g.,
             multiracial, transgender, bisexual), this special issue (a)
             highlights the experiences and perceptions of people with
             nontraditional identities; (b) argues for research to
             represent modern-day demographics; and (c) discusses
             publication challenges. By comparing special issue
             submissions to membership data from the Society for
             Personality and Social Psychology, we highlight which
             identities are over- and underrepresented, consider groups
             for which it may have been historically more difficult to
             publish, and offer some speculation as to
             why.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15298868.2020.1711802},
   Key = {fds348707}
}

@article{fds357325,
   Author = {Herrmann, SD and Varnum, MEW and Straka, BC and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {Social Class Identity Integration and Success for
             First-Generation College Students: Antecedents, Mechanisms,
             and Generalizability},
   Journal = {Self and Identity},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2021.1924251},
   Abstract = {Social class bicultural identity integration research
             demonstrates that integrated social class identities are
             linked with better health, well-being, and academic
             performance among first-generation students. Here, we
             demonstrate that exposure to college graduates in
             students’ home neighborhoods before college is positively
             related to higher social class bicultural identity
             integration (Study 1), that the effect of identity
             integration on academic performance is mediated by academic
             self-efficacy (Study 2), and that the effects of identity
             integration on acculturative stress, life satisfaction, and
             overall health outcomes observed at a large, public
             university replicated at selective, private universities
             (Study 3). This suggests that the identity integration
             framework is a useful theoretical lens to conceptualize and
             predict health and performance outcomes for first-generation
             students.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15298868.2021.1924251},
   Key = {fds357325}
}

@article{fds362939,
   Author = {Paul, M and Gaither, S and Darity, W},
   Title = {About Face: Seeing Class and Race},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/s3bcq},
   Abstract = {<p>People’s social class, and the perceptions of their
             social class are embedded in an institutional context that
             has important ramifications for one’s life opportunities
             and outcomes. Research on first impressions has found that
             people are relatively accurate at judging a variety of
             traits such as perceived sexual orientation and income, but
             there is a paucity of research that investigates whether
             people are also accurate at judging wealth or class. In this
             article, we first investigate whether people understand the
             distinction between income and wealth (Study 1). Then, using
             a novel dataset, we examine whether people are accurate at
             identifying the income and wealth levels of individuals
             across racial and ethnic groups by facial cues alone (Study
             2). We find that participants understand the meaning of
             income, but not wealth. Additionally, we find that
             perceivers categorize class more accurately than by sheer
             chance, using minimal facial cues, but perceivers are
             particularly inaccurate when categorizing high-income and
             high-wealth Black and Latinx subjects.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/s3bcq},
   Key = {fds362939}
}

@article{fds362940,
   Author = {Gaither, S and Chen, C-M and Neal, S and Chien, SH-L},
   Title = {Children’s cross-cultural categorizations of racially
             ambiguous faces in Taiwan and the U.S.},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ydazv},
   Abstract = {<p>Objectives: Racially ambiguous face categorization
             research is growing in prominence, and yet the majority of
             this work has focused on White and Western samples and has
             primarily used biracial Black/White stimuli. Past findings
             suggest that biracial Black/White faces are more often seen
             as Black than White, but without testing these perceptions
             with other groups, generalizability cannot be guaranteed.
             <h4>Methods:</h4> We tested 3-7-year old Asian children
             living in Taiwan—an Eastern cultural context (N =
             74)—and Asian children living in the U.S.—a Western
             cultural context (N = 65) to explore the role that cultural
             group membership may play in biracial perceptions. Children
             categorized 12 racially ambiguous biracial Black/White faces
             and 12 biracial Asian/White faces in a dichotomous
             forced-choice task and completed a racial constancy
             measurement. <h4>Results:</h4> Regarding biracial
             Black/White faces, Taiwanese and Asian American children
             both categorized the faces as White significantly more often
             compared to chance levels, regardless of racial constancy
             beliefs. For biracial Asian/White faces, Taiwanese children
             with racial constancy beliefs categorized the faces
             significantly more often as White, whereas Taiwanese
             children without racial constancy beliefs categorized the
             faces significantly more often as Asian. However, Asian
             American children did not show a bias in categorizing
             biracial Asian/White faces. <h4>Conclusions:</h4> Results
             suggest that hyperdescent over hypodescent for more commonly
             studied biracial Black/White faces generalizes in both
             cultural contexts. However, biracial Asian/White stimuli may
             be perceived in more fixed-like patterns in predominately
             Asian contexts, since only Taiwanese children showed
             increased outgroup categorizations once racial constancy
             beliefs were endorsed.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/ydazv},
   Key = {fds362940}
}

@article{fds362941,
   Author = {Straka, B and Stanaland, A and Tomasello, M and Gaither,
             S},
   Title = {Who Can Be in a Group? 3- to 5-Year-Old Children Construe
             Realistic Social Groups Through Mutual Intentionality},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ch56b},
   Abstract = {<p>Recent research suggests that young children’s causal
             justification for minimal group membership can be induced
             via a cognitive framework of mutual intentionality. That is,
             an individual can become a group member when both the
             individual and group agree to membership. Here, we
             investigated if children ages 3-5 understand groups formed
             by mutual intentions and whether they apply mutual
             intentions to realistic groups with varying entitative and
             essentialized qualities. In two studies (N = 197), we asked
             3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children if a novel character could
             join an existing group based on intentionality (mutual,
             individual-, group-only) and group type (task, friends,
             family). We find that 4- and 5-year-olds robustly relied on
             mutual intentions to constitute group membership and
             3-year-olds also demonstrated emerging usage of this
             cognitive framework. Moreover, children employed mutual
             intentionality across different group types, suggesting a
             general framework for children’s understanding of
             different social groups.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/ch56b},
   Key = {fds362941}
}

@article{fds362942,
   Author = {Herrmann, S and Varnum, M and Straka, B and Gaither,
             S},
   Title = {Social Class Identity Integration and Success for
             First-Generation College Students: Antecedents, Mechanisms,
             and Generalizability},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/v98sn},
   Abstract = {<p>Past research has investigated challenges
             first-generation college students face, but has overlooked
             the role that acculturation to college may play. Social
             class bicultural identity integration research demonstrates
             that integrated social class identities are linked with
             better health, well-being, and academic performance among
             first-generation students. Here, we build on the identity
             integration framework, demonstrating that exposure to
             college graduates in students’ home neighborhoods before
             college is positively related to higher social class
             bicultural identity integration (Study 1), that the effect
             of identity integration on academic performance is mediated
             by academic self-efficacy (Study 2), and that the effects of
             identity integration on acculturative stress, life
             satisfaction, and overall health outcomes observed at a
             large public university replicated at selective, private
             universities (Study 3). This suggests that the identity
             integration framework is a useful theoretical lens to
             conceptualize and predict health and performance outcomes
             for first-generation students.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/v98sn},
   Key = {fds362942}
}

@article{fds362943,
   Author = {Tsai, A and Straka, B and Gaither, S},
   Title = {Mixed-Heritage Individuals’ Encounters with
             Raciolinguistic Ideologies},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/znkxw},
   Abstract = {<p>Mixed-heritage individuals (MHIs) are known to face high
             levels of social exclusion. Here, we investigate how
             raciolinguistic ideologies related to one’s heritage
             language abilities add to these exclusionary experiences.
             The results from 293 MHIs reveal frequent experiences of
             marginalization from members of each of their heritage
             communities because their racial appearance and language
             practices are perceived as deviant and outside imagined
             ‘monoracial’ norms. Specifically, over half of
             respondents described experiences of exclusion for not
             speaking their minority heritage languages with the same
             accent or manner or fluency associated with ‘monoracial’
             native speakers of their heritage languages or dialects.
             Another subset described high pressure to speak ‘proper
             English’ in White dominant work environments. These
             results extend past MHI work by empirically documenting the
             ‘monoracial-only’, monoglossic, and ‘Standard
             English’ ideologies that contribute to the continued
             social exclusion of MHIs.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/znkxw},
   Key = {fds362943}
}

@article{fds362944,
   Author = {Straka, B and Albuja, A and Desjardins, M and Swartzwelder, S and Gaither, S},
   Title = {Alcohol Use and Related Consequences for Monoracial and
             Multiracial Native American/American Indian College
             students},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/hcve5},
   Abstract = {<p>Native American/American Indian (NA/AI) and Multiracial
             people (those who claim multiple racial groups) report
             notably high alcohol use compared to other racial groups in
             the United States. Nearly half of the NA/AI population is
             also Multiracial, yet NA/AI and Multiracial college students
             report different motivations for drinking alcohol.
             Therefore, it remains unclear if NA/AI individuals who are
             also Multiracial are at different risk for alcohol use and
             negative alcohol-related consequences, and if there are
             distinct patterns of risk factors in these understudied
             populations. Because college-aged students are at risk for
             high levels of alcohol use, this exploratory study used the
             AlcoholEdu for CollegeTM survey to compare the association
             between initial drinking age, college location (urban versus
             rural), and alcohol use motivations and consequences between
             monoracial NA/AI (N = 2,363) and Multiracial NA/AI
             college-aged students (N = 6,172). Monoracial NA/AI students
             reported higher incidences of alcohol use and
             alcohol-related problems such as blacking out and missing
             class, compared to Multiracial NA/AI students. Risk factors
             like earlier age of drinking onset were more strongly
             associated with negative consequences for monoracial NA/AI
             students compared to Multiracial NA/AI students. Despite
             similar levels of Internal Coping motivations for drinking
             (e.g., to feel more confident or sure of yourself),
             monoracial NA/AI students reported drinking more than
             Multiracial students and experienced more negative
             drinking-related outcomes. These results suggest Multiracial
             NA/AI students may draw on protective factors not accessible
             to monoracial NA/AI students, highlighting the need for
             interventions tailored to students at highest
             risk.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/hcve5},
   Key = {fds362944}
}

@article{fds366967,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and Sanchez, DT and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Intra-race intersectionality: Identity denial among
             dual-minority biracial people.},
   Journal = {Translational Issues in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {392-403},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tps0000264},
   Doi = {10.1037/tps0000264},
   Key = {fds366967}
}

@article{fds352653,
   Author = {Sanchez, DT and Gaither, SE and Albuja, AF and Eddy,
             Z},
   Title = {How Policies Can Address Multiracial Stigma},
   Journal = {Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain
             Sciences},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {115-122},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2372732220943906},
   Abstract = {Twenty years ago, Multiracial Americans completed the U.S
             Census with the option to indicate more than one race for
             the first time. As we embark on the second anniversary of
             this shift in Multiracial recognition, this article reviews
             the research related to known sources and systems that
             perpetuate Multiracial-specific stigma. Policy
             recommendations address the needs and the continued
             acknowledgment of this growing racial/ethnic minority
             population.},
   Doi = {10.1177/2372732220943906},
   Key = {fds352653}
}

@article{fds347135,
   Author = {Straka, BC and Gaither, SE and Acheson, SK and Swartzwelder,
             HS},
   Title = {“Mixed” Drinking Motivations: A Comparison of Majority,
             Multiracial, and Minority College Students},
   Journal = {Social Psychological and Personality Science},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {676-687},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550619883294},
   Abstract = {Social exclusion is associated with substance use, but the
             specific link between majority and minority racial group
             membership and substance use is unknown. We examined how
             social exclusion among racial majority (White), Multiracial,
             and racial minority (Native American, Latino, Asian, and
             Black) college students relates to self-reported alcohol use
             and motivations. Using the AlcoholEdu for College™ survey,
             Study 1a reports five factors related to motives for
             initiating or inhibiting alcohol use. Study 1b analyzes
             majority, Multiracial, and minority college students’
             comparative endorsement of these motivations. Study 2
             compares these factors with established belonging scales
             using a separate undergraduate sample. White, Multiracial,
             Native American, and Latino students displayed the highest
             proportion of problematic alcohol use. White students
             endorsed belonging-based drinking motivations, while
             Multiracial and Asian students endorsed motivations similar
             to both majority and minority groups. Native American,
             Latino, and Black students endorsed abstaining motivations
             more than other groups.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1948550619883294},
   Key = {fds347135}
}

@article{fds344741,
   Author = {Meyers, C and Aumer, K and Schoniwitz, A and Janicki, C and Pauker, K and Chang, EC and Gaither, SE and Williams, A},
   Title = {Experiences with microaggressions and discrimination in
             racially diverse and homogeneously white
             contexts.},
   Journal = {Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {250-259},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000293},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>The interaction between one's context and
             identity may be essential in understanding people's racial
             experiences. In this study, we examined 2 contexts (racially
             diverse vs. homogenously White) and measured the experiences
             of discrimination and microaggressions for monoracial people
             of color (POC), multiracial individuals, and White
             individuals. Additionally, we measured experience of
             microaggressions with a new scale that measured instances of
             multiracial-specific microaggressions, and the offensiveness
             of these microaggressions.<h4>Method and results</h4>Through
             a self-reported survey, monoracial POC, multiracial
             individuals, and White individuals across the United States
             reported their experiences with discrimination and
             microaggressions, and offensiveness of multiracial-specific
             microaggressions. Overall, monoracial POC and multiracial
             individuals reported experiencing less discrimination and
             microaggressions in diverse contexts versus homogenous
             contexts. White individuals reported the lowest amounts of
             discrimination and microaggressions, which did not differ
             across contexts.<h4>Conclusions</h4>Living in a racially
             diverse context may have positive benefits for racial
             minorities, and White individuals do not necessarily
             experience greater instances of discrimination or
             microaggressions in diverse contexts. (PsycInfo Database
             Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/cdp0000293},
   Key = {fds344741}
}

@article{fds344833,
   Author = {Skinner, AL and Perry, SP and Gaither, S},
   Title = {Not Quite Monoracial: Biracial Stereotypes
             Explored.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {46},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {377-392},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167219858344},
   Abstract = {Stereotypes often guide our perceptions of members of social
             groups. However, research has yet to document what
             stereotypes may exist for the fastest growing youth
             demographic in the United States-biracial individuals.
             Across seven studies (<i>N</i> = 1,104), we investigate what
             stereotypes are attributed to various biracial groups,
             whether biracial individuals are stereotyped as more similar
             to their lower status monoracial parent group (trait
             hypodescent), and whether contact moderates these
             stereotypes. Results provide evidence of some universal
             biracial stereotypes that are applied to all biracial
             groups: attractive and not fitting in or belonging. We also
             find that all biracial groups are attributed a number of
             unique stereotypes (i.e., which are not associated with
             their monoracial parent groups). However, across all
             studies, we find little evidence of trait hypodescent and no
             evidence that the tendency to engage in trait hypodescent
             varies as a function of contact.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0146167219858344},
   Key = {fds344833}
}

@article{fds344441,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Fan, SP and Kinzler, KD},
   Title = {Thinking about multiple identities boosts children's
             flexible thinking.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e0012871},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12871},
   Abstract = {Studies of children's developing social identification often
             focus on individual forms of identity. Yet, everyone has
             multiple potential identities. Here we investigated whether
             making children aware of their multifaceted
             identities-effectively seeing themselves from multiple
             angles-would promote their flexible thinking. In Experiment
             1, 6- to 7-year-old children (N = 48) were assigned to
             either a Multiple-Identities condition where they were led
             to consider their multiple identities (e.g. friend,
             neighbor) or to a Physical-Traits condition where they
             considered their multiple physical attributes (e.g. legs,
             arms). Children in the Multiple-Identity condition
             subsequently expressed greater flexibility at
             problem-solving and categorization than children in the
             Physical-Traits condition. Experiment 2 (N = 72)
             replicated these findings with a new sample of 6- to
             7-year-old children and demonstrated that a
             Multiple-Identity mindset must be self-relevant. Children
             who were led to think about another child's multiple
             identities did not express as much subsequent creative
             thinking as did children who thought about their own
             multiple identities. Experiment 3 (N = 76) showed that a
             Mmultiple-Identity framework may be particularly effective
             when the identities are presented via generic language
             suggesting that they are enduring traits (in this case,
             identities depicted as noun phrases rather than verbal
             phrases). These findings illustrate that something as simple
             as thinking about one's identity from multiple angles could
             serve as a tool to help reduce rigid thinking, which might
             increase open-mindedness in a society that is becoming
             increasingly diverse.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12871},
   Key = {fds344441}
}

@article{fds351272,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Perlin, JD and Doan, SN},
   Title = {Race, Gender, and the Development of Cross-Race
             Egalitarianism.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychology},
   Volume = {11},
   Pages = {1525},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01525},
   Abstract = {Over the course of development, children acquire adult-like
             thinking about social categories such as race, which in turn
             informs their perceptions, attitudes, and behavior. However,
             children's developing perceptions of race have been
             understudied particularly with respect to their potential
             influence on cross-race egalitarianism. Specifically, the
             acquisition of racial constancy, defined as the perception
             that race is a concrete and stable category, has been
             associated with increased awareness of racial stereotypes
             and group status differences. Yet, little work has
             investigated behavioral outcomes stemming from the
             acquisition of racial constancy beliefs. Here, we
             investigate whether the presence or absence of racial
             constancy beliefs differentially predicts inequality
             aversion with racial ingroup versus outgroup members for
             young children. White children (<i>N</i> = 202; ages 3-8)
             completed three sticker resource-allocation games with
             either a White or a Black partner shown in a photograph,
             after which racial constancy was measured. Results revealed
             that the acquisition of racial constancy interacted with
             partner race to predict inequality aversion outcomes in one
             game; however, age and gender also exerted strong
             effects.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01525},
   Key = {fds351272}
}

@article{fds366035,
   Author = {Stanaland, A and Gaither, S},
   Title = {“Be a Man”: The Role of Social Pressure in Eliciting
             Men’s Aggressive Cognition},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5wxtf},
   Abstract = {<p>[Manuscript published in PSPB on Jan 27, 2021.]
             Threatening a man’s manhood—but not a woman’s
             womanhood—elicits aggression. In two studies, we found
             evidence that this aggression is related to the social
             pressure men experience to “be a man.” In Study 1a, we
             conducted an exploratory factor analysis to isolate
             participants’ (N = 195; Mage = 19.92) differential
             motivations for conforming to gender norms. Study 1b then
             showed that pressure to be masculine moderates the
             relationship between gender identity threat and aggressive
             cognition for men. In Study 2a, we conducted a confirmatory
             factor analysis to validate the aforementioned scales with
             an age-diverse sample of men (N = 391; Mage = 33.16, range
             18-56 years). Study 2b replicated Study 1b, most notably
             with younger men. In all, these findings reveal one
             pathway—the pressure men experience to be stereotypically
             masculine—that elicits aggressive cognition when under
             threat in a U.S. context.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/5wxtf},
   Key = {fds366035}
}

@article{fds345889,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and Gaither, SE and Sanchez, DT and Straka, B and Cipollina,
             R},
   Title = {Psychophysiological Stress Responses to Bicultural and
             Biracial Identity Denial},
   Journal = {Journal of Social Issues},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1165-1191},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josi.12347},
   Abstract = {Bicultural and biracial individuals (those who identify
             either with two cultures or two races) are often denied
             membership in the groups with which they identify, an
             experience referred to as identity denial. The present
             studies used an experimental design to test the effects of
             identity denial on physiological and self-reported stress,
             and naturalistic behavioral responses in a controlled
             laboratory setting for both bicultural (Study 1; N = 126)
             and biracial (Study 2; N = 119) individuals. The results
             suggest that compared to an identity-irrelevant denial,
             bicultural participants who were denied their American
             identity and Minority/White biracial individuals who were
             denied their White identity reported greater stress and were
             more likely to verbally reassert their identity. Bicultural
             participants also demonstrated slower cortisol recovery
             compared to those in the identity-irrelevant denial
             condition. The results are the first to highlight the
             negative physical health consequences of identity denial
             using an experimental design for both bicultural and
             biracial populations, underscoring the necessity to promote
             belongingness and acceptance.},
   Doi = {10.1111/josi.12347},
   Key = {fds345889}
}

@article{fds342825,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and Sanchez, DT and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Identity Questioning: Antecedents and Consequences of
             Prejudice Attributions},
   Journal = {Journal of Social Issues},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {515-537},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josi.12322},
   Abstract = {Many ethnic minorities in the United States hold both an
             ethnic minority and national American identity. Yet, they
             often encounter identity questioning when asked questions
             such as, “Where are you really from?,” which may operate
             as an ambiguous threat to their national identity. Because
             varied motivations (curiosity versus exclusion) create
             ambiguity, targets likely vary in their tendency to view
             identity questioning as prejudicial. Study 1 examined the
             extent to which ethnic minorities attribute identity
             questioning to prejudice, and the associated well-being
             consequences. Study 2 examined the immigration
             policy-oriented antecedents of identity questioning
             prejudice attributions. The results suggest that prejudice
             attributions are psychologically harmful (Study 1) and are
             associated with anti-immigration policies (Study 2). Because
             identity questioning challenges one's ability to maintain a
             dual identity, it is important to better understand identity
             questioning. Specifically, these findings provide initial
             evidence of the role policy contexts may play in shaping
             identity questioning attributions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/josi.12322},
   Key = {fds342825}
}

@article{fds330903,
   Author = {Gomez, EM and Young, DM and Preston, AG and Wilton, LS and Gaither, SE and Kaiser, CR},
   Title = {Loss and loyalty: Change in political and gender identity
             among Clinton supporters after the 2016 U.S. presidential
             election},
   Journal = {Self and Identity},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {103-125},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2017.1391873},
   Abstract = {How do voters’ identities change after a candidate’s
             defeat? A longitudinal, within-subjects study used Hillary
             Clinton’s loss in the 2016 U.S. presidential election to
             explore social identity theory’s (SIT) tenet that threats
             to self-relevant groups motivate further connection to and
             affirmation of the group. Two independent samples
             (university students and adults on Mechanical Turk) were
             assessed before and after the 2016 U.S. presidential
             election. After Hillary Clinton’s defeat, those who
             reported voting for Clinton affirmed their political and
             gender identities in several ways, such as increasing their
             identification with Clinton. These ecologically valid
             results are consistent with SIT, and suggest supporters
             affirm their identities following a threat such as the
             defeat of their candidate during a high-stakes election. We
             discuss the implications of these findings within the
             context of the increasingly polarized U.S.
             electorate.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15298868.2017.1391873},
   Key = {fds330903}
}

@article{fds337470,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and Sanchez, DT and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Identity Denied: Comparing American or White Identity Denial
             and Psychological Health Outcomes Among Bicultural and
             Biracial People.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {416-430},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167218788553},
   Abstract = {Because bicultural and biracial people have two identities
             within one social domain (culture or race), their
             identification is often challenged by others. Although it is
             established that identity denial is associated with poor
             psychological health, the processes through which this
             occurs are less understood. Across two high-powered studies,
             we tested identity autonomy, the perceived compatibility of
             identities, and social belonging as mediators of the
             relationship between identity denial and well-being among
             bicultural and biracial individuals. Bicultural and biracial
             participants who experienced challenges to their American or
             White identities felt less freedom in choosing an identity
             and perceived their identities as less compatible, which was
             ultimately associated with greater reports of depressive
             symptoms and stress. Study 2 replicated these results and
             measured social belonging, which also accounted for
             significant variance in well-being. The results suggest the
             processes were similar across populations, highlighting
             important implications for the generalizability to other
             dual-identity populations.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0146167218788553},
   Key = {fds337470}
}

@article{fds335677,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Toosi, NR and Babbitt, LG and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {Exposure to Biracial Faces Reduces Colorblindness.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {54-66},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167218778012},
   Abstract = {Across six studies, we demonstrate that exposure to biracial
             individuals significantly reduces endorsement of
             colorblindness as a racial ideology among White individuals.
             Real-world exposure to biracial individuals predicts lower
             levels of colorblindness compared with White and Black
             exposure (Study 1). Brief manipulated exposure to images of
             biracial faces reduces colorblindness compared with exposure
             to White faces, Black faces, a set of diverse monoracial
             faces, or abstract images (Studies 2-5). In addition, these
             effects occur only when a biracial label is paired with the
             face rather than resulting from the novelty of the
             mixed-race faces themselves (Study 4). Finally, we show that
             the shift in White participants' colorblindness attitudes is
             driven by social tuning, based on participants' expectations
             that biracial individuals are lower in colorblindness than
             monoracial individuals (Studies 5-6). These studies suggest
             that the multiracial population's increasing size and
             visibility has the potential to positively shift racial
             attitudes.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0146167218778012},
   Key = {fds335677}
}

@article{fds339585,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Chen, JM and Pauker, K and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {At face value: Psychological outcomes differ for real vs.
             computer-generated multiracial faces.},
   Journal = {The Journal of social psychology},
   Volume = {159},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {592-610},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2018.1538929},
   Abstract = {Multiracial research emphasizes hypodescent categorizations
             and relies on computer-generated stimuli. Four experiments
             showed that real biracial faces in a 2-Choice categorization
             task (White, Black) elicited hypodescent more than
             computer-generated faces. Additionally, Experiment 2 showed
             a 2-Choice categorization task with real biracial faces
             increased racial essentialism more than a 3-Choice
             categorization task. Experiment 3 showed that mere exposure
             to real biracial faces did not increase essentialism.
             Finally, Experiments 4a and 4b replicated hypodescent
             outcomes when comparing real biracial faces to
             computer-generated versions of those same faces. In sum,
             these findings initiate a discussion surrounding the
             methodology of multiracial categorizations.},
   Doi = {10.1080/00224545.2018.1538929},
   Key = {fds339585}
}

@article{fds335680,
   Author = {Chen, EE and Corriveau, KH and Lai, VKW and Poon, SL and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {Learning and Socializing Preferences in Hong Kong Chinese
             Children.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {89},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {2109-2117},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13083},
   Abstract = {The impact of social group information on the learning and
             socializing preferences of Hong Kong Chinese children were
             examined. Specifically, the degree to which variability in
             racial out-group exposure affects children's use of race to
             make decisions about unfamiliar individuals (Chinese, White,
             Southeast Asian) was investigated. Participants (N = 212;
             M<sub>age</sub>  = 60.51 months) chose functions for
             novel objects after informants demonstrated their use;
             indicated with which peer group member to socialize; and
             were measured on racial group recognition, preference, and
             identification. Overall, children preferred in-group
             members, though out-group exposure and the relative social
             status of out-groups mattered as well. At a young age,
             children's specific experiences with different races
             influence how they learn and befriend others across racial
             group lines.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.13083},
   Key = {fds335680}
}

@article{fds335681,
   Author = {Loyd, AB and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Racial/ethnic socialization for White youth: What we know
             and future directions},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {59},
   Pages = {54-64},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.05.004},
   Abstract = {Teaching and talking about race and ethnicity with children
             and adults is especially important in racially diverse
             societies. This process has been coined racial/ethnic
             socialization (RES). Despite the importance of RES, we still
             know very little about how this process unfolds in the lives
             of White youth. Thus, from a social, cognitive, and
             developmental perspective, the authors summarize findings
             from empirical research and theory on RES for White youth
             across stages of development—early childhood through young
             adulthood. Since RES is linked with cross-group attitudes
             (e.g., less bias, prejudice, stereotyping) and behaviors
             (e.g., inclusion), we highlight future directions for
             research and discuss applications for existing findings for
             an increasingly diverse society.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.appdev.2018.05.004},
   Key = {fds335681}
}

@article{fds335674,
   Author = {Chen, JM and Pauker, K and Gaither, SE and Hamilton, DL and Sherman,
             JW},
   Title = {Black + White = Not White: A minority bias in
             categorizations of Black-White multiracials},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {78},
   Pages = {43-54},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.05.002},
   Abstract = {The present research sought to provide new insights on the
             principles guiding the categorization of Black-White
             multiracial faces at a first encounter. Previous studies
             have typically measured categorization of multiracial faces
             using close-ended tasks that constrain available
             categorizations. Those studies find evidence that perceivers
             tend to categorize multiracials as Black more often than as
             White. Two studies used less constrained, implicit
             (Experiment 1) and explicit categorization (Experiment 2)
             tasks and found that multiracial faces were most frequently
             categorized into racial minority groups but not necessarily
             as Black. These studies suggested a minority bias in
             multiracial categorizations, whereby multiracials are more
             frequently categorized as non-White than as White.
             Experiment 3 provided additional support for the minority
             bias, showing that participants categorized multiracials as
             “Not White” more often than as any other category.
             Participants were also faster to exclude multiracial faces
             from the White category than from any other racial category.
             Together, these findings are the first to document the
             minority bias as a guiding principle in multiracial
             categorization.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2018.05.002},
   Key = {fds335674}
}

@article{fds335675,
   Author = {Gaither, SE},
   Title = {The multiplicity of belonging: Pushing identity research
             beyond binary thinking},
   Journal = {Self and Identity},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {443-454},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2017.1412343},
   Abstract = {To date, research has primarily used a singular identity
             framework for investigating how social identity shapes
             behavior. Thus, research has also largely ignored the role
             that having multiple, simultaneous identities may play in
             our lives. This paper reviews work that pushes that singular
             identity framework beyond either/or binary constructs of
             identity. Specifically, through exploring racial identity
             flexibility for biracial populations and activating a
             flexible, multiple identity mindset more broadly outside of
             racial domains, these results demonstrate the potential
             impact of multifaceted self-views on flexible behaviors.
             Related work concerning other multiply belonging individuals
             are also discussed in line with a call for needed research
             to pinpoint new models and mechanisms for understanding the
             multiplicity of belonging.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15298868.2017.1412343},
   Key = {fds335675}
}

@article{fds335676,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and Sanchez, DT and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Fluid racial presentation: Perceptions of contextual
             “passing” among biracial people},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {77},
   Pages = {132-142},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.010},
   Abstract = {Existing monoracial identity frameworks fail to capture the
             experiences of biracial people, for whom racial
             identification may depend on the social context. Though
             biracial people can vary their racial identity, the social
             consequences of context-dependent racial self-presentation
             remain underexplored. Five studies examined how contextual
             racial presentation among biracial people is perceived by
             high status groups. White participants read vignettes
             describing a biracial person contextually presenting in an
             academic situation and evaluated the target's character and
             behavior. Asian/White or Black/White biracial students who
             contextually presented as monoracial (compared to biracial
             presentation) were evaluated more negatively because they
             were perceived as less trustworthy (Studies 1–5). The
             effect of White contextual presentation was mediated by
             endorsement of stereotypes that biracial people are confused
             about their racial identity (Studies 4–5). Responses were
             robust to the status of the monoracial identity (Studies
             1–2) and intention to benefit (Study 5), but varied by the
             availability of choice (e.g., conditions of forced choice;
             Study 3). The results suggest contextually choosing an
             identity carries social repercussions because it can
             activate explicit negative stereotypes about biracial
             individuals.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.010},
   Key = {fds335676}
}

@article{fds335678,
   Author = {Pauker, K and Meyers, C and Sanchez, DT and Gaither, SE and Young,
             DM},
   Title = {A review of multiracial malleability: Identity,
             categorization, and shifting racial attitudes},
   Journal = {Social and Personality Psychology Compass},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {e12392-e12392},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12392},
   Abstract = {The majority of social perception research to date has
             focused on perceptually obvious and prototypical
             representations of social categories. However, not all
             people belong to social categories that are easily
             discernable. Within the past decade, there has been an
             upsurge of research demonstrating that multifaceted
             identities (both one's own and perceptions of others'
             identities) influence people to think about social
             categories in a more flexible manner. Here, we specifically
             review research on multiracial identity and perceptions of
             multiracial individuals as 2 domains where researchers have
             documented evidence of the flexible nature of social
             identities and social categorization. Integrating frameworks
             that argue race is a dynamic and interactive process, we
             provide evidence that studying multiracial perceivers and
             targets helps reveal that race changes across situations,
             time, and depending on a number of top-down factors (e.g.,
             expectations, stereotypes, and cultural norms). From the
             perspective of multiracial individuals as perceivers, we
             review research showing that flexible identity in
             multiracial individuals influences the process of social
             perception driven by a reduced belief in the essential
             nature of racial categories. From the perspective of
             multiracial individuals as targets, we review research that
             top-down cues influence the racial categorization process.
             We further discuss emerging work that reveals that exposure
             to multiracial individuals influences beliefs surrounding
             the categorical (or noncategorical) nature of race, itself.
             Needed directions for future work are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1111/spc3.12392},
   Key = {fds335678}
}

@article{fds335679,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Babbitt, LG and Sommers, SR},
   Title = {Resolving racial ambiguity in social interactions},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {76},
   Pages = {259-269},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.03.003},
   Abstract = {People take longer to categorize racially ambiguous
             individuals, but does this perceptual complexity also affect
             social interactions? In Study 1, White participants
             interacted with a racially ambiguous confederate who was
             either labeled as biracial Black/White, monoracial Black, or
             given no racial label. White participants in the biracial
             condition were significantly less cognitively depleted, less
             essentialist in their thoughts about race, and exhibited
             more accurate face memory for their partners than when
             partner race remained unspecified or was labeled as
             monoracial Black. Confederate reports and nonverbal behavior
             in the biracial condition were also more positive. In Study
             2, White participants perceived more similarity with a
             biracial Black/White labeled interaction partner compared to
             a Black-specified or race-unspecified partner, highlighting
             for the first time how racial ambiguity and racial labeling
             affect behavioral outcomes in social interactions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2018.03.003},
   Key = {fds335679}
}

@article{fds332755,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Apfelbaum, EP and Birnbaum, HJ and Babbitt, LG and Sommers, SR},
   Title = {Mere Membership in Racially Diverse Groups Reduces
             Conformity},
   Journal = {Social Psychological and Personality Science},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {402-410},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550617708013},
   Abstract = {Three studies assessed the impact of White individuals’
             mere membership in racially diverse or homogeneous groups on
             conformity. In Study 1, White participants were randomly
             assigned to four-person groups that were racially diverse or
             homogeneous in which three confederates routinely endorsed
             clearly inferior college applicants for admission.
             Participants in diverse groups were significantly less
             likely to conform than those in homogeneous groups. Study 2
             replicated these results using an online conformity
             paradigm, thereby isolating the effects of racial group
             composition from concomitant social cues in face-to-face
             settings. Study 3 presented a third condition—a diverse
             group that included one other White member. Individuals
             conformed less in both types of diverse groups as compared
             with the homogeneous group. Evidence suggests this was
             because Whites in homogeneous (vs. diverse) settings were
             more likely to reconsider their original decision after
             learning how other group members responded.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1948550617708013},
   Key = {fds332755}
}

@article{fds339521,
   Author = {Babbitt, LG and Gaither, SE and Toosi, NR and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {The role of gender in racial meta-stereotypes and
             stereotypes},
   Journal = {Social Cognition},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {589-601},
   Publisher = {Guilford Publications},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2018.36.5.589},
   Abstract = {Stereotypes often guide interracial interactions-both the
             stereotypes we hold about others, and the stereotypes we
             believe others hold about us (i.e., meta-stereotypes). In
             Black-White interactions, the stereotype that Whites are
             prejudiced is one of the most salient, but does this
             stereotype vary by gender? White women tend to express more
             positive racial attitudes than White men, and stereotypes of
             Whites overlap more with stereotypes about men than with
             stereotypes about women. Thus, we hypothesized that both
             prejudice-related meta-stereotypes and stereotypes differ by
             gender. In Study 1, Whites reported that White men are seen
             as more prejudiced than White women. Studies 2a and 2b
             measured Blacks' perspectives, finding that Blacks also
             reported that White men are seen as more prejudiced than
             White women. Together, these findings highlight the
             importance of considering gender to develop a more nuanced
             understanding of race-related stereotypes, meta-stereotypes,
             and interracial interactions.},
   Doi = {10.1521/soco.2018.36.5.589},
   Key = {fds339521}
}

@article{fds330902,
   Author = {Dukes, KN and Gaither, SE},
   Title = {Black Racial Stereotypes and Victim Blaming: Implications
             for Media Coverage and Criminal Proceedings in Cases of
             Police Violence against Racial and Ethnic
             Minorities},
   Journal = {Journal of Social Issues},
   Volume = {73},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {789-807},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josi.12248},
   Abstract = {Posthumous stereotypical media portrayals of Michael Brown
             and other racial and ethnic minority victims of police
             violence have sparked questions about the influence of
             racial stereotypes on public opinions about their deaths and
             criminal proceedings for their killers. However, few studies
             have empirically investigated how the specific type of
             information released about a victim impacts opinions
             surrounding such incidents. Participants (N = 453) read
             about an altercation that resulted in a shooting death where
             the race of the victim and shooter (Black vs. White) was
             randomly assigned. Participants learned either negative,
             Black male stereotypic or positive, Black male
             counterstereotypic information about the victim. Next,
             participants appraised levels of fault and blame, sympathy
             and empathy for the victim and shooter, and indictment
             recommendations for the shooter. Findings suggest that the
             type of information released about a victim can
             significantly sway attitudes toward the victim and the
             shooter. Implications for media portrayals of racial/ethnic
             minority victims of police violence and its impact on
             criminal sentencing are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1111/josi.12248},
   Key = {fds330902}
}

@article{fds349000,
   Author = {Chen, C-M and Gaither, S and Chien, SH-L},
   Title = {Exploring Taiwanese Young Children's Perception and
             Categorization of Racially Ambiguous Faces},
   Journal = {I-PERCEPTION},
   Volume = {8},
   Pages = {45-45},
   Publisher = {SAGE PUBLICATIONS LTD},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {October},
   Key = {fds349000}
}

@article{fds318687,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Remedios, JD and Schultz, JR and Maddox, KB and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {Examining the effects of I-sharing for future white-black
             interactions},
   Journal = {Social Psychology},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {125-135},
   Publisher = {Hogrefe Publishing Group},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000264},
   Abstract = {Research shows that I-sharing, or sharing subjective
             experiences with an outgroup member, positively shapes
             attitudes toward that outgroup member. We investigated
             whether this type of social experience would also promote a
             positive interracial interaction with a novel outgroup
             member. Results showed that White and Black participants who
             I-shared with a racial outgroup member (vs. I-sharing with a
             racial ingroup member) expressed more liking toward that
             outgroup member. However, I-sharing with an outgroup member
             did not reduce anxious behavior in a future social
             interaction with a novel racial outgroup member. Therefore,
             although sharing subjective experiences may increase liking
             toward one individual from a racial outgroup, it remains to
             be seen whether this positive experience can influence
             behaviors in future interactions with other racial outgroup
             members. Future directions are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1027/1864-9335/a000264},
   Key = {fds318687}
}

@article{fds318688,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Pauker, K and Slepian, ML and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {Social belonging motivates categorization of racially
             ambiguous faces},
   Journal = {Social Cognition},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {97-118},
   Publisher = {Guilford Publications},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2016.34.2.97},
   Abstract = {Categorizing racially ambiguous individuals is multifaceted,
             and the current work proposes social-motivational factors
             also exert considerable influence on how racial ambiguity is
             perceived, directing the resolution of ambiguity in a manner
             that is functionally beneficial to the perceiver. Four
             studies tested two motivations related to social belonging:
             belonging needs and racial identification. Greater need to
             belong and racial identification (Study 1), and two types of
             social belonging threats - social exclusion (Studies 2a and
             2b) and racial identity threat (Study 3) - predicted more
             categorizations of racially ambiguous Black/White faces as
             Black, with White participants more likely to categorize
             ambiguous faces as outgroup members (i.e., Black; Studies 1,
             2a, 2b, and 3) and Black participants more likely to
             categorize ambiguous faces as ingroup members (Study 2b).
             Results also demonstrated that self-affirmation mitigated
             this motivated categorization for Whites (Study 3),
             illustrating the malleability of social categorization and
             its dependency on serving self-relevant goals.},
   Doi = {10.1521/soco.2016.34.2.97},
   Key = {fds318688}
}

@article{fds318694,
   Author = {Schultz, JR and Gaither, SE and Urry, HL and Maddox,
             KB},
   Title = {Reframing anxiety to encourage interracial
             interactions.},
   Journal = {Translational Issues in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {392-400},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tps0000048},
   Doi = {10.1037/tps0000048},
   Key = {fds318694}
}

@article{fds318689,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Remedios, JD and Schultz, JR and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {Priming White identity elicits stereotype boost for biracial
             Black-White individuals},
   Journal = {Group Processes and Intergroup Relations},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {778-787},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430215570504},
   Abstract = {Psychological threat experienced by students of negatively
             stereotyped groups impairs test performance. However,
             stereotype boost can also occur if a positively stereotyped
             identity is made salient. Biracial individuals, whose racial
             identities may be associated with both negative and positive
             testing abilities, have not been examined in this context.
             Sixty-four biracial Black-White individuals wrote about
             either their Black or White identity or a neutral topic and
             completed a verbal Graduate Record Examination (GRE)
             examination described as diagnostic of their abilities.
             White-primed participants performed significantly better
             than both Black-primed and control participants. Thus,
             biracial Black-White individuals experience stereotype boost
             only when their White identity is made salient.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1368430215570504},
   Key = {fds318689}
}

@article{fds318690,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Remedios, JD and Sanchez, DT and Sommers,
             SR},
   Title = {Thinking Outside the Box: Multiple Identity Mind-Sets Affect
             Creative Problem Solving},
   Journal = {Social Psychological and Personality Science},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {596-603},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550614568866},
   Abstract = {Rigid thinking is associated with less creativity,
             suggesting that priming a flexible mind-set should boost
             creative thought. In three studies, we investigate whether
             priming multiple social identities predicts more creativity
             in domains unrelated to social identity. Study 1 asked
             monoracial and multiracial participants to write about their
             racial identities before assessing creativity. Priming a
             multiracial’s racial identity led to greater creativity
             compared to a no-prime control. Priming a monoracial’s
             racial identity did not affect creativity. Study 2 showed
             that reminding monoracials that they, too, have multiple
             identities increased creativity. Study 3 replicated this
             effect and demonstrated that priming a multiracial identity
             for monoracials did not affect creativity. These results are
             the first to investigate the association between flexible
             identities and flexible thinking, highlighting the potential
             for identity versatility to predict cognitive differences
             between individuals who have singular versus multifaceted
             views of their social selves.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1948550614568866},
   Key = {fds318690}
}

@article{fds318691,
   Author = {Howell, JL and Gaither, SE and Ratliff, KA},
   Title = {Caught in the Middle: Defensive Responses to IAT Feedback
             Among Whites, Blacks, and Biracial Black/Whites},
   Journal = {Social Psychological and Personality Science},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {373-381},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550614561127},
   Abstract = {This study used archival data to examine how White, Black,
             and biracial Black/White people respond to implicit attitude
             feedback suggesting that they harbor racial bias that does
             not align with their self-reported attitudes. The results
             suggested that people are generally defensive in response to
             feedback indicating that their implicit attitudes differ
             from their explicit attitudes. Among monoracial White and
             Black individuals, this effect was particularly strong when
             they learned that they were implicitly more pro-White than
             they indicated explicitly. By contrast, biracial Black/White
             individuals were defensive about large discrepancies in
             either direction (more pro-Black or more pro-White implicit
             attitudes). These results pinpoint one distinct difference
             between monoracial and biracial populations and pave the way
             for future research to further explore how monoracial
             majority, minority, and biracial populations compare in
             other types of attitudes and responses to personal
             feedback.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1948550614561127},
   Key = {fds318691}
}

@article{fds318692,
   Author = {Gaither, SE},
   Title = {“Mixed” Results: Multiracial Research and Identity
             Explorations},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {114-119},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721414558115},
   Abstract = {Multiracial individuals report that the social pressure of
             having to “choose” one of their racial groups is a
             primary source of psychological conflict. Yet because of
             their ability to maneuver among their multiple identities,
             multiracials also adopt flexible cognitive strategies in
             dealing with their social environments—demonstrating a
             benefit to having multiple racial identities. The current
             article reviews recent research involving multiracial
             participants to examine the behavioral and cognitive
             outcomes linked to being multiracial and pinpoints possible
             moderators that may affect these outcomes. Limitations in
             applying monoracial identity frameworks to multiracial
             populations are also discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0963721414558115},
   Key = {fds318692}
}

@article{fds318693,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Cohen-Goldberg, AM and Gidney, CL and Maddox,
             KB},
   Title = {Sounding Black or White: priming identity and biracial
             speech.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychology},
   Volume = {6},
   Pages = {457},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00457},
   Abstract = {Research has shown that priming one's racial identity can
             alter a biracial individuals' social behavior, but can such
             priming also influence their speech? Language is often used
             as a marker of one's social group membership and studies
             have shown that social context can affect the style of
             language that a person chooses to use, but this work has yet
             to be extended to the biracial population. Audio clips were
             extracted from a previous study involving biracial
             Black/White participants who had either their Black or White
             racial identity primed. Condition-blind coders rated
             Black-primed biracial participants as sounding significantly
             more Black and White-primed biracial participants as
             sounding significantly more White, both when listening to
             whole (Study 1a) and thin-sliced (Study 1b) clips. Further
             linguistic analyses (Studies 2a-c) were inconclusive
             regarding the features that differed between the two groups.
             Future directions regarding the need to investigate the
             intersections between social identity priming and language
             behavior with a biracial lens are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00457},
   Key = {fds318693}
}

@article{fds318695,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Wilton, LS and Young, DM},
   Title = {Perceiving a presidency in black (and white): Four years
             later},
   Journal = {Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {7-21},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/asap.12018},
   Abstract = {When Barack Obama became the "first Black President" of the
             United States in 2008, researchers examined how his election
             impacted Americans' views of racial progress. When he was
             reelected in 2012, the minority status of the president had
             become less novel. In the present study, we investigated
             whether perceptions concerning racial progress varied: (1)
             before and after President Obama's reelection; (2) by
             whether President Obama was labeled as biracial or Black;
             and (3) among White and Black individuals. We replicated
             past findings to demonstrate that after Obama's reelection,
             White participants reported that our country had made racial
             progress and decreased their support for equality programs
             (e.g., affirmative action). Our results also revealed that
             labeling President Obama as either biracial or Black did not
             affect views of racial progress. Additionally, Black
             participants categorized President Obama as Black more than
             White participants, while White participants categorized
             President Obama as White more than Black participants. We
             discuss these results in terms of the impacts of racial
             beliefs that stem from exposure to a minority
             leader.},
   Doi = {10.1111/asap.12018},
   Key = {fds318695}
}

@article{fds318696,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Chen, EE and Corriveau, KH and Harris, PL and Ambady, N and Sommers, SR},
   Title = {Monoracial and biracial children: effects of racial identity
             saliency on social learning and social preferences.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {2299-2316},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12266},
   Abstract = {Children prefer learning from, and affiliating with, their
             racial in-group but those preferences may vary for biracial
             children. Monoracial (White, Black, Asian) and biracial
             (Black/White, Asian/White) children (N = 246, 3-8 years)
             had their racial identity primed. In a learning preferences
             task, participants determined the function of a novel object
             after watching adults (White, Black, and Asian) demonstrate
             its uses. In the social preferences task, participants saw
             pairs of children (White, Black, and Asian) and chose with
             whom they most wanted to socially affiliate. Biracial
             children showed flexibility in racial identification during
             learning and social tasks. However, minority-primed biracial
             children were not more likely than monoracial minorities to
             socially affiliate with primed racial in-group members,
             indicating their in-group preferences are contextually
             based.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12266},
   Key = {fds318696}
}

@article{fds318697,
   Author = {Chen, JM and Moons, WG and Gaither, SE and Hamilton, DL and Sherman,
             JW},
   Title = {Motivation to control prejudice predicts categorization of
             multiracials.},
   Journal = {Personality & social psychology bulletin},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {590-603},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167213520457},
   Abstract = {Multiracial individuals often do not easily fit into
             existing racial categories. Perceivers may adopt a novel
             racial category to categorize multiracial targets, but their
             willingness to do so may depend on their motivations. We
             investigated whether perceivers' levels of internal
             motivation to control prejudice (IMS) and external
             motivation to control prejudice (EMS) predicted their
             likelihood of categorizing Black-White multiracial faces as
             Multiracial. Across four studies, IMS positively predicted
             perceivers' categorizations of multiracial faces as
             Multiracial. The association between IMS and Multiracial
             categorizations was strongest when faces were most racially
             ambiguous. Explicit prejudice, implicit prejudice, and
             interracial contact were ruled out as explanations for the
             relationship between IMS and Multiracial categorizations.
             EMS may be negatively associated with the use of the
             Multiracial category. Therefore, perceivers' motivations to
             control prejudice have important implications for racial
             categorization processes.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0146167213520457},
   Key = {fds318697}
}

@article{fds318698,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Schultz, JR and Pauker, K and Sommers, SR and Maddox,
             KB and Ambady, N},
   Title = {Essentialist thinking predicts decrements in children's
             memory for racially ambiguous faces.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {482-488},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033493},
   Abstract = {Past research shows that adults often display poor memory
             for racially ambiguous and racial outgroup faces, with both
             face types remembered worse than own-race faces. In the
             present study, the authors examined whether children also
             show this pattern of results. They also examined whether
             emerging essentialist thinking about race predicts
             children's memory for faces. Seventy-four White children
             (ages 4-9 years) completed a face-memory task comprising
             White, Black, and racially ambiguous Black-White faces.
             Essentialist thinking about race was also assessed (i.e.,
             thinking of race as immutable and biologically based). White
             children who used essentialist thinking showed the same bias
             as White adults: They remembered White faces significantly
             better than they remembered ambiguous and Black faces.
             However, children who did not use essentialist thinking
             remembered both White and racially ambiguous faces
             significantly better than they remembered Black faces. This
             finding suggests a specific shift in racial thinking wherein
             the boundaries between racial groups become more discrete,
             highlighting the importance of how race is conceptualized in
             judgments of racially ambiguous individuals.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0033493},
   Key = {fds318698}
}

@article{fds318699,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Sommers, SR and Ambady, N},
   Title = {When the half affects the whole: Priming identity for
             biracial individuals in social interactions},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {368-371},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.12.012},
   Abstract = {In two studies we investigate how the fluid identities of
             biracial individuals interact with contextual factors to
             shape behavior in interracial settings. In Study 1, biracial
             Black/White participants (n = 22) were primed with either
             their Black or White identity before having a race-related
             discussion with a Black confederate. Study 2 (n = 34)
             assessed the influence of our prime on racial
             self-identification and examined interactions with a White
             confederate. Self-reports and nonverbal behavior indicated
             that when the primed racial ingroup matched that of an
             interaction partner, biracial participants behaved much like
             participants in same-race interactions in previous studies,
             exhibiting lower levels of anxiety. Priming the opposite
             racial identity, however, led to greater signs of anxiety,
             mimicking past interracial interaction findings. These
             results extend previous findings regarding the influence of
             contextual factors on racial identification for biracial
             individuals, and are the first to demonstrate the
             implications of these effects for behavioral tendencies. ©
             2012 Elsevier Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2012.12.012},
   Key = {fds318699}
}

@article{fds318700,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Sommers, SR},
   Title = {Living with an other-race roommate shapes Whites' behavior
             in subsequent diverse settings},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {272-276},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.020},
   Abstract = {In a multi-phase research design over two academic
             semesters, White college students assigned to either a
             same-race or other-race roommate were tracked across two
             survey phases and a third phase involving an interracial
             interaction with a Black stranger. After four months, Whites
             who lived with an other-race roommate came to have more
             diverse friends and believe that diversity was more
             important than did Whites with a White roommate. After six
             months, self-reports, partner ratings, and nonverbal
             behavior indicated that Whites with an other-race roommate
             were less anxious, more pleasant, and more physically
             engaged during a novel interracial interaction. These
             results demonstrate that residential contact with other-race
             individuals not only affects race-related attitudes, but can
             also reduce interracial anxiety and positively influence
             behavior in subsequent diverse settings. © 2012 Elsevier
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.020},
   Key = {fds318700}
}

@article{fds318701,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Sommers, SR},
   Title = {Honk if you like minorities: Vuvuzela attitudes predict
             outgroup liking},
   Journal = {International Review for the Sociology of
             Sport},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {54-65},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1012690211429219},
   Abstract = {The 2010 FIFA World Cup in South Africa generated extensive
             controversy over spectators' use of the African vuvuzela
             trumpet. We asked 123 White American participants about
             their opinions of vuvuzelas as well as their attitudes
             towards a variety of racial/ethnic minority groups including
             immigrants, African Americans, and Latinos. We found that
             the less participants liked vuvuzelas, the less positively
             they also tended to feel toward minority groups.
             Furthermore, respondents who liked vuvuzelas the least were
             also less generally open to change. These findings suggest
             that the vuvuzela controversy was about more than just a
             plastic trumpet - it was also an episode of differential
             ingroup/outgroup perceptions and a lack of openness to new
             things. © The Author(s) 2011.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1012690211429219},
   Key = {fds318701}
}

@article{fds318702,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Pauker, K and Johnson, SP},
   Title = {Biracial and monoracial infant own-race face perception: an
             eye tracking study.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {775-782},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01170.x},
   Abstract = {We know that early experience plays a crucial role in the
             development of face processing, but we know little about how
             infants learn to distinguish faces from different races,
             especially for non-Caucasian populations. Moreover, it is
             unknown whether differential processing of different race
             faces observed in typically studied monoracial infants
             extends to biracial infants as well. Thus, we investigated
             3-month-old Caucasian, Asian and biracial (Caucasian-Asian)
             infants' ability to distinguish Caucasian and Asian faces.
             Infants completed two within-subject, infant-controlled
             habituation sequences and test trials as an eye tracker
             recorded looking times and scanning patterns. Examination of
             individual differences revealed significant positive
             correlations between own-race novelty preference and
             scanning frequency between eye and mouth regions of own-race
             habituation stimuli for Caucasian and Asian infants,
             suggesting that facility in own-race face discrimination
             stems from active inspection of internal facial features in
             these groups. Biracial infants, however, showed the opposite
             effect: An 'own-race' novelty preference was associated with
             reduced scanning between eye and mouth regions of 'own-race'
             habituation stimuli, suggesting that biracial infants use a
             distinct approach to processing frequently encountered
             faces. Future directions for investigating face processing
             development in biracial populations are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01170.x},
   Key = {fds318702}
}


%% Books   
@book{fds371306,
   Author = {Albuja, AF and West, A and Gaither, S},
   Title = {Two Or More: A Comparative Analysis of Multiracial and
             Multicultural Research},
   Pages = {78 pages},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {December},
   ISBN = {9781009202718},
   Abstract = {Most research has investigated Multiracial and Multicultural
             populations as separate topics, despite demographic and
             experiential overlap between these. This Element bridges
             that divide by reviewing and comparing Multiracial and
             Multicultural research to date—their origins, theoretical
             and methodological development, and key findings in
             socialization, identity negotiation and discrimination—to
             identify points of synthesis and differentiation to guide
             future research. It highlights challenges researchers face
             when studying these populations because such research topics
             necessitate that one moves beyond previous frameworks and
             theories to grapple with identity as flexible, malleable,
             and influenced both by internal factors and external
             perceptions. The areas of overlap and difference are
             meaningful and illustrate the social constructive nature of
             race and culture, which is always in flux and being
             re-defined. This title is also available as open access on
             Cambridge Core.},
   Key = {fds371306}
}


%% Book chapters   
@misc{fds327382,
   Author = {Gaither, SE and Dukes, KN},
   Title = {Young, black, and endangered: Examining the deaths of
             trayvon martin, michael brown, and tamir rice through a
             psychological lens},
   Pages = {83-98},
   Booktitle = {Stereotypes and Stereotyping: Misperceptions, Perspectives
             and Role of Social Media},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781634845106},
   Abstract = {The deaths of unarmed racial minorities across the United
             States have garnered widespread attention and speculation,
             yet the discussions surrounding why these events keep
             occurring remain limited. In this chapter, we apply a
             psychological lens to three notable cases. The shooting
             deaths of Trayvon Martin, Michael Brown, and Tamir Rice
             sparked a national debate surrounding race relations and
             racially biased shootings of unarmed minorities. Using
             psychological theory and findings, we identify and analyze
             factors that may have contributed to these deaths.
             Specifically we consider the roles that physical appearance
             and shooter bias may have played in all three cases. We also
             examine the biased media depictions that negatively
             portrayed Martin and Brown and influenced reactions to their
             deaths. Taken together, we contend that this evidence
             provides a probable narrative for the decisions to shoot
             Martin, Brown, and Rice while also starting a crucial
             discussion surrounding racial stereotyping, biased media
             representations, Stand Your Ground statutes, and needed
             police training paradigms. We conclude with recommendations
             for reform in each of these domains as a way to mitigate
             both future wrongful shootings and to reduce the overall
             bias experienced by racial minorities.},
   Key = {fds327382}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Postdocs * Reload * Login