Psychology and Neuroscience Faculty Database
Psychology and Neuroscience
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > pn > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin    :recent first  alphabetical  by type listing:

%%    
@article{fds376463,
   Author = {Fredrickson, BL and Tugade, MM and Waugh, CE and Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective
             study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist
             attacks on the United States on September 11th,
             2001.},
   Journal = {Journal of Personality & Social Psychology},
   Volume = {84},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {365-376},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2003},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.84.2.365},
   Doi = {10.1037//0022-3514.84.2.365},
   Key = {fds376463}
}

@article{fds318779,
   Author = {Fredrickson, BL and Tugade, MM and Waugh, CE and Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective
             study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist
             attacks on the United States on September 11th,
             2001.},
   Journal = {Journal of personality and social psychology},
   Volume = {84},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {365-376},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.84.2.365},
   Abstract = {Extrapolating from B. L. Fredrickson's (1998, 2001)
             broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions, the authors
             hypothesized that positive emotions are active ingredients
             within trait resilience. U.S. college students (18 men and
             28 women) were tested in early 2001 and again in the weeks
             following the September 11th terrorist attacks. Mediational
             analyses showed that positive emotions experienced in the
             wake of the attacks--gratitude, interest, love, and so
             forth--fully accounted for the relations between (a)
             precrisis resilience and later development of depressive
             symptoms and (b) precrisis resilience and postcrisis growth
             in psychological resources. Findings suggest that positive
             emotions in the aftermath of crises buffer resilient people
             against depression and fuel thriving, consistent with the
             broaden-and-build theory. Discussion touches on implications
             for coping.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0022-3514.84.2.365},
   Key = {fds318779}
}

@article{fds320740,
   Author = {Mikels, JA and Fredrickson, BL and Larkin, GR and Lindberg, CM and Maglio, SJ and Reuter-Lorenz, PA},
   Title = {Emotional category data on images from the International
             Affective Picture System.},
   Journal = {Behavior research methods},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {626-630},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03192732},
   Abstract = {The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) is widely
             used in studies of emotion and has been characterized
             primarily along the dimensions of valence, arousal, and
             dominance. Even though research has shown that the IAPS is
             useful in the study of discrete emotions, the categorical
             structure of the IAPS has not been characterized thoroughly.
             The purpose of the present project was to collect
             descriptive emotional category data on subsets of the LAPS
             in an effort to identify images that elicit onediscrete
             emotion more than others. These data reveal multiple
             emotional categories for the images and indicate that this
             image set has great potential in the investigation of
             discrete emotions. This article makes these data available
             to researchers with such interests. Data for all the
             pictures are archived at www.psychonomic.org/archive/.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03192732},
   Key = {fds320740}
}

@article{fds318778,
   Author = {Mikels, JA and Larkin, GR and Reuter-Lorenz, PA and Cartensen,
             LL},
   Title = {Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: changes in working
             memory for affective versus visual information with
             age.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {542-553},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542},
   Abstract = {Working memory mediates the short-term maintenance of
             information. Virtually all empirical research on working
             memory involves investigations of working memory for verbal
             and visual information. Whereas aging is typically
             associated with a deficit in working memory for these types
             of information, recent findings suggestive of relatively
             well-preserved long-term memory for emotional information in
             older adults raise questions about working memory for
             emotional material. This study examined age differences in
             working memory for emotional versus visual information.
             Findings demonstrate that, despite an age-related deficit
             for the latter, working memory for emotion was unimpaired.
             Further, older adults exhibited superior performance on
             positive relative to negative emotion trials, whereas their
             younger counterparts exhibited the opposite
             pattern.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542},
   Key = {fds318778}
}

@article{fds318777,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Gibbs, SEB and Khanna, K and Nielsen, L and Carstensen, LL and Knutson, B},
   Title = {Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in healthy older
             adults.},
   Journal = {Nature neuroscience},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {787-791},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1894},
   Abstract = {Although global declines in structure have been documented
             in the aging human brain, little is known about the
             functional integrity of the striatum and prefrontal cortex
             in older adults during incentive processing. We used
             event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging to
             determine whether younger and older adults differed in both
             self-reported and neural responsiveness to anticipated
             monetary gains and losses. The present study provides
             evidence for intact striatal and insular activation during
             gain anticipation with age, but shows a relative reduction
             in activation during loss anticipation. These findings
             suggest that there is an asymmetry in the processing of
             gains and losses in older adults that may have implications
             for decision-making.},
   Doi = {10.1038/nn1894},
   Key = {fds318777}
}

@article{fds318776,
   Author = {Samanez Larkin and GR and Gibbs, SEB and Khanna, K and Nielsen, L and Carstensen, LL and Knutson, B},
   Title = {Erratum: Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in
             healthy older adults (Nature Neuroscience (2007) 10,
             (787-791))},
   Journal = {Nature Neuroscience},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1222},
   Publisher = {Springer Nature},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn0907-1222b},
   Doi = {10.1038/nn0907-1222b},
   Key = {fds318776}
}

@article{fds318775,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Hollon, NG and Carstensen, LL and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {Individual differences in insular sensitivity during loss
             anticipation predict avoidance learning.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {320-323},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02087.x},
   Abstract = {The anterior insula has been implicated in both the
             experience and the anticipation of negative outcomes.
             Although individual differences in insular sensitivity have
             been associated with self-report measures of chronic
             anxiety, previous research has not examined whether
             individual differences in insular sensitivity predict
             learning to avoid aversive stimuli. In the present study,
             insular sensitivity was assessed as participants anticipated
             monetary losses while undergoing functional magnetic
             resonance imaging. We found that insular responsiveness to
             anticipated losses predicted participants' ability to learn
             to avoid losses (but not to approach gains) in a behavioral
             test several months later. These findings suggest that in
             addition to correlating with self-reported anxiety,
             heightened insular sensitivity may promote learning to avoid
             loss.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02087.x},
   Key = {fds318775}
}

@article{fds318774,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and D'Esposito, M},
   Title = {Group comparisons: imaging the aging brain.},
   Journal = {Social cognitive and affective neuroscience},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {290-297},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn029},
   Abstract = {With the recent growth of functional magnetic resonance
             imaging (fMRI), scientists across a range of disciplines are
             comparing neural activity between groups of interest, such
             as healthy controls and clinical patients, children and
             young adults and younger and older adults. In this edition
             of Tools of the Trade, we will discuss why great caution
             must be taken when making group comparisons in studies using
             fMRI. Although many methodological contributions have been
             made in recent years, the suggestions for overcoming common
             issues are too often overlooked. This review focuses
             primarily on neuroimaging studies of healthy aging, but many
             of the issues raised apply to other group designs as
             well.},
   Doi = {10.1093/scan/nsn029},
   Key = {fds318774}
}

@article{fds318773,
   Author = {Ersner-Hershfield, H and Garton, MT and Ballard, K and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Knutson, B},
   Title = {Don't stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences
             in future self-continuity account for saving.},
   Journal = {Judgment and decision making},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {280-286},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {June},
   Abstract = {Some people find it more difficult to delay rewards than
             others. In three experiments, we tested a "future
             self-continuity" hypothesis that individual differences in
             the perception of one's present self as continuous with a
             future self would be associated with measures of saving in
             the laboratory and everyday life. Higher future
             self-continuity (assessed by a novel index) predicted
             reduced discounting of future rewards in a laboratory task,
             more matches in adjectival descriptions of present and
             future selves, and greater lifetime accumulation of
             financial assets (even after controlling for age and
             education). In addition to demonstrating the reliability and
             validity of the future self-continuity index, these findings
             are consistent with the notion that increased future
             self-continuity might promote saving for the
             future.},
   Key = {fds318773}
}

@article{fds318771,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Robertson, ER and Mikels, JA and Carstensen,
             LL and Gotlib, IH},
   Title = {Selective attention to emotion in the aging
             brain.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {519-529},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016952},
   Abstract = {A growing body of research suggests that the ability to
             regulate emotion remains stable or improves across the adult
             life span. Socioemotional selectivity theory maintains that
             this pattern of findings reflects the prioritization of
             emotional goals. Given that goal-directed behavior requires
             attentional control, the present study was designed to
             investigate age differences in selective attention to
             emotional lexical stimuli under conditions of emotional
             interference. Both neural and behavioral measures were
             obtained during an experiment in which participants
             completed a flanker task that required them to make
             categorical judgments about emotional and nonemotional
             stimuli. Older adults showed interference in both the
             behavioral and neural measures on control trials but not on
             emotion trials. Although older adults typically show
             relatively high levels of interference and reduced cognitive
             control during nonemotional tasks, they appear to be able to
             successfully reduce interference during emotional
             tasks.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0016952},
   Key = {fds318771}
}

@article{fds318772,
   Author = {Kwon, Y and Scheibe, S and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Tsai, JL and Carstensen, LL},
   Title = {Replicating the positivity effect in picture memory in
             Koreans: evidence for cross-cultural generalizability.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {748-754},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016054},
   Abstract = {Older adults' relatively better memory for positive over
             negative material (positivity effect) has been widely
             observed in Western samples. This study examined whether a
             relative preference for positive over negative material is
             also observed in older Koreans. Younger and older Korean
             participants viewed images from the International Affective
             Picture System (IAPS), were tested for recall and
             recognition of the images, and rated the images for valence.
             Cultural differences in the valence ratings of images
             emerged. Once considered, the relative preference for
             positive over negative material in memory observed in older
             Koreans was indistinguishable from that observed previously
             in older Americans.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0016054},
   Key = {fds318772}
}

@article{fds318770,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Kuhnen, CM and Yoo, DJ and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {Variability in nucleus accumbens activity mediates
             age-related suboptimal financial risk taking.},
   Journal = {The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1426-1434},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4902-09.2010},
   Abstract = {As human life expectancy continues to rise, financial
             decisions of aging investors may have an increasing impact
             on the global economy. In this study, we examined age
             differences in financial decisions across the adult life
             span by combining functional neuroimaging with a dynamic
             financial investment task. During the task, older adults
             made more suboptimal choices than younger adults when
             choosing risky assets. This age-related effect was mediated
             by a neural measure of temporal variability in nucleus
             accumbens activity. These findings reveal a novel neural
             mechanism by which aging may disrupt rational financial
             choice.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.4902-09.2010},
   Key = {fds318770}
}

@article{fds318768,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Mata, R and Radu, PT and Ballard, IC and Carstensen, LL and McClure, SM},
   Title = {Age Differences in Striatal Delay Sensitivity during
             Intertemporal Choice in Healthy Adults.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in neuroscience},
   Volume = {5},
   Pages = {126},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2011.00126},
   Abstract = {Intertemporal choices are a ubiquitous class of decisions
             that involve selecting between outcomes available at
             different times in the future. We investigated the neural
             systems supporting intertemporal decisions in healthy
             younger and older adults. Using functional neuroimaging, we
             find that aging is associated with a shift in the brain
             areas that respond to delayed rewards. Although we replicate
             findings that brain regions associated with the mesolimbic
             dopamine system respond preferentially to immediate rewards,
             we find a separate region in the ventral striatum with very
             modest time dependence in older adults. Activation in this
             striatal region was relatively insensitive to delay in older
             but not younger adults. Since the dopamine system is
             believed to support associative learning about future
             rewards over time, our observed transfer of function may be
             due to greater experience with delayed rewards as people
             age. Identifying differences in the neural systems
             underlying these decisions may contribute to a more
             comprehensive model of age-related change in intertemporal
             choice.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fnins.2011.00126},
   Key = {fds318768}
}

@article{fds318769,
   Author = {Knutson, B and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Kuhnen, CM},
   Title = {Gain and loss learning differentially contribute to life
             financial outcomes.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {e24390},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024390},
   Abstract = {Emerging findings imply that distinct neurobehavioral
             systems process gains and losses. This study investigated
             whether individual differences in gain learning and loss
             learning might contribute to different life financial
             outcomes (i.e., assets versus debt). In a community sample
             of healthy adults (n = 75), rapid learners had smaller
             debt-to-asset ratios overall. More specific analyses,
             however, revealed that those who learned rapidly about gains
             had more assets, while those who learned rapidly about
             losses had less debt. These distinct associations remained
             strong even after controlling for potential cognitive (e.g.,
             intelligence, memory, and risk preferences) and
             socioeconomic (e.g., age, sex, ethnicity, income, education)
             confounds. Self-reported measures of assets and debt were
             additionally validated with credit report data in a subset
             of subjects. These findings support the notion that
             different gain and loss learning systems may exert a
             cumulative influence on distinct life financial
             outcomes.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0024390},
   Key = {fds318769}
}

@article{fds318767,
   Author = {Carstensen, LL and Turan, B and Scheibe, S and Ram, N and Ersner-Hershfield, H and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Brooks, KP and Nesselroade, JR},
   Title = {Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on
             over 10 years of experience sampling.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {21-33},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021285},
   Abstract = {Recent evidence suggests that emotional well-being improves
             from early adulthood to old age. This study used
             experience-sampling to examine the developmental course of
             emotional experience in a representative sample of adults
             spanning early to very late adulthood. Participants (N =
             184, Wave 1; N = 191, Wave 2; N = 178, Wave 3) reported
             their emotional states at five randomly selected times each
             day for a one week period. Using a measurement burst design,
             the one-week sampling procedure was repeated five and then
             ten years later. Cross-sectional and growth curve analyses
             indicate that aging is associated with more positive overall
             emotional well-being, with greater emotional stability and
             with more complexity (as evidenced by greater co-occurrence
             of positive and negative emotions). These findings remained
             robust after accounting for other variables that may be
             related to emotional experience (personality, verbal
             fluency, physical health, and demographic variables).
             Finally, emotional experience predicted mortality;
             controlling for age, sex, and ethnicity, individuals who
             experienced relatively more positive than negative emotions
             in everyday life were more likely to have survived over a 13
             year period. Findings are discussed in the theoretical
             context of socioemotional selectivity theory.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0021285},
   Key = {fds318767}
}

@article{fds318766,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Wagner, AD and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {Expected value information improves financial risk taking
             across the adult life span.},
   Journal = {Social cognitive and affective neuroscience},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {207-217},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq043},
   Abstract = {When making decisions, individuals must often compensate for
             cognitive limitations, particularly in the face of advanced
             age. Recent findings suggest that age-related variability in
             striatal activity may increase financial risk-taking
             mistakes in older adults. In two studies, we sought to
             further characterize neural contributions to optimal
             financial risk taking and to determine whether decision aids
             could improve financial risk taking. In Study 1,
             neuroimaging analyses revealed that individuals whose
             mesolimbic activation correlated with the expected value
             estimates of a rational actor made more optimal financial
             decisions. In Study 2, presentation of expected value
             information improved decision making in both younger and
             older adults, but the addition of a distracting secondary
             task had little impact on decision quality. Remarkably,
             provision of expected value information improved the
             performance of older adults to match that of younger adults
             at baseline. These findings are consistent with the notion
             that mesolimbic circuits play a critical role in optimal
             choice, and imply that providing simplified information
             about expected value may improve financial risk taking
             across the adult life span.},
   Doi = {10.1093/scan/nsq043},
   Key = {fds318766}
}

@article{fds318764,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Introduction to decision making over the life
             span.},
   Journal = {Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences},
   Volume = {1235},
   Pages = {v-vi},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06252.x},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06252.x},
   Key = {fds318764}
}

@article{fds318765,
   Author = {Mata, R and Josef, AK and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Hertwig,
             R},
   Title = {Age differences in risky choice: a meta-analysis.},
   Journal = {Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences},
   Volume = {1235},
   Pages = {18-29},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06200.x},
   Abstract = {Does risk taking change as a function of age? We conducted a
             systematic literature search and found 29 comparisons
             between younger and older adults on behavioral tasks thought
             to measure risk taking (N= 4,093). The reports relied on
             various tasks differing in several respects, such as the
             amount of learning required or the choice framing (gains vs.
             losses). The results suggest that age-related differences
             vary considerably as a function of task characteristics, in
             particular the learning requirements of the task. In
             decisions from experience, age-related differences in risk
             taking were a function of decreased learning performance:
             older adults were more risk seeking compared to younger
             adults when learning led to risk-avoidant behavior, but were
             more risk averse when learning led to risk-seeking behavior.
             In decisions from description, younger adults and older
             adults showed similar risk-taking behavior for the majority
             of the tasks, and there were no clear age-related
             differences as a function of gain/loss framing. We discuss
             limitations and strengths of past research and provide
             suggestions for future work on age-related differences in
             risk taking.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06200.x},
   Key = {fds318765}
}

@article{fds318763,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Levens, SM and Perry, LM and Dougherty, RF and Knutson, B},
   Title = {Frontostriatal white matter integrity mediates adult age
             differences in probabilistic reward learning.},
   Journal = {The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {15},
   Pages = {5333-5337},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5756-11.2012},
   Abstract = {Frontostriatal circuits have been implicated in reward
             learning, and emerging findings suggest that frontal white
             matter structural integrity and probabilistic reward
             learning are reduced in older age. This cross-sectional
             study examined whether age differences in frontostriatal
             white matter integrity could account for age differences in
             reward learning in a community life span sample of human
             adults. By combining diffusion tensor imaging with a
             probabilistic reward learning task, we found that older age
             was associated with decreased reward learning and decreased
             white matter integrity in specific pathways running from the
             thalamus to the medial prefrontal cortex and from the medial
             prefrontal cortex to the ventral striatum. Further, white
             matter integrity in these thalamocorticostriatal paths could
             statistically account for age differences in learning. These
             findings suggest that the integrity of frontostriatal white
             matter pathways critically supports reward learning. The
             findings also raise the possibility that interventions that
             bolster frontostriatal integrity might improve reward
             learning and decision making.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.5756-11.2012},
   Key = {fds318763}
}

@article{fds318761,
   Author = {Kuhnen, CM and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {Serotonergic genotypes, neuroticism, and financial
             choices.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e54632},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0054632},
   Abstract = {Life financial outcomes carry a significant heritable
             component, but the mechanisms by which genes influence
             financial choices remain unclear. Focusing on a polymorphism
             in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene
             (5-HTTLPR), we found that individuals possessing the short
             allele of this gene invested less in equities, were less
             engaged in actively making investment decisions, and had
             fewer credit lines. Short allele carriers also showed higher
             levels of the personality trait neuroticism, despite not
             differing from others with respect to cognitive skills,
             education, or wealth. Mediation analysis suggested that the
             presence of the 5-HTTLPR short allele decreased real life
             measures of financial risk taking through its influence on
             neuroticism. These findings show that 5-HTTLPR short allele
             carriers avoid risky and complex financial choices due to
             negative emotional reactions, and have implications for
             understanding and managing individual differences in
             financial choice.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0054632},
   Key = {fds318761}
}

@article{fds318762,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Li, S-C and Ridderinkhof,
             KR},
   Title = {Complementary approaches to the study of decision making
             across the adult life span.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in neuroscience},
   Volume = {7},
   Pages = {243},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00243},
   Doi = {10.3389/fnins.2013.00243},
   Key = {fds318762}
}

@misc{fds325715,
   Author = {Knutson, B and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Brain, Decision, and Debt},
   Pages = {167-180},
   Booktitle = {A Debtor World: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on
             Debt},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780199873722},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199873722.003.0007},
   Abstract = {This chapter summarizes recent findings in neuroeconomics
             suggesting that emotion (specifically, "anticipatory
             affect") can influence financial decisions. It then
             discusses how individual differences in anticipatory affect
             may promote proneness to consumer debt. Thanks to
             improvements in spatial and temporal resolution, functional
             magnetic resonance imaging experiments have begun to suggest
             that activation of a brain region associated with
             anticipating gains (i.e., the nucleus accumbens or NAcc)
             precedes an increased tendency to seek financial gains,
             whereas activation of another region associated with
             anticipating losses (i.e., the anterior insula) precedes an
             increased tendency to avoid financial losses. By extension,
             individual differences in increased gain anticipation,
             decreased loss anticipation, or some combination of the two
             might promote proneness to debt. Ultimately, neuroeconomic
             advances may help individuals to optimize their investment
             strategies, as well as empower institutions to minimize
             consumer debt.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199873722.003.0007},
   Key = {fds325715}
}

@article{fds327691,
   Author = {Camerer, C and Smith, A and Kuhnen, CM and Wargo, DT and Samanez-Larkin,
             G and Montague, R and Levy, DJ and Smith, D and Meshi, D and Kenning, PH and Clithero, J and Weber, B and Hare, T and Huettel, S and Josephson, C and d'Acremont, M and Knoch, D and Krajbich, I and De Martino and B and Mohr,
             PNC and Barton, J and Halko, M-L and Chick, CF and Gianotti, L and Heekeren, HR},
   Title = {Correspondence Are Cognitive Functions Localizable?},
   Journal = {JOURNAL OF ECONOMIC PERSPECTIVES},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {247-250},
   Publisher = {AMER ECONOMIC ASSOC},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {March},
   Key = {fds327691}
}

@article{fds318760,
   Author = {Garrett, DD and Samanez-Larkin, GR and MacDonald, SWS and Lindenberger, U and McIntosh, AR and Grady, CL},
   Title = {Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier
             in human brain mapping?},
   Journal = {Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {610-624},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015},
   Abstract = {Neuroscientists have long observed that brain activity is
             naturally variable from moment-to-moment, but neuroimaging
             research has largely ignored the potential importance of
             this phenomenon. An emerging research focus on within-person
             brain signal variability is providing novel insights, and
             offering highly predictive, complementary, and even
             orthogonal views of brain function in relation to human
             lifespan development, cognitive performance, and various
             clinical conditions. As a result, brain signal variability
             is evolving as a bona fide signal of interest, and should no
             longer be dismissed as meaningless noise when mapping the
             human brain.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015},
   Key = {fds318760}
}

@article{fds318759,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Financial Decision Making and the Aging Brain.},
   Journal = {APS observer},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {30-33},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {June},
   Key = {fds318759}
}

@article{fds318758,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Buckholtz, JW and Cowan, RL and Woodward, ND and Li, R and Ansari, MS and Arrington, CM and Baldwin, RM and Smith, CE and Treadway, MT and Kessler, RM and Zald, DH},
   Title = {A thalamocorticostriatal dopamine network for
             psychostimulant-enhanced human cognitive
             flexibility.},
   Journal = {Biological psychiatry},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {99-105},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.032},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>Everyday life demands continuous
             flexibility in thought and behavior. We examined whether
             individual differences in dopamine function are related to
             variability in the effects of amphetamine on one aspect of
             flexibility: task switching.<h4>Methods</h4>Forty healthy
             human participants performed a task-switching paradigm
             following placebo and oral amphetamine administration.
             [(18)F]fallypride was used to measure D2/D3 baseline
             receptor availability and amphetamine-stimulated dopamine
             release.<h4>Results</h4>The majority of the participants
             showed amphetamine-induced benefits through reductions in
             switch costs. However, such benefits were variable.
             Individuals with higher baseline thalamic and cortical
             receptor availability and striatal dopamine release showed
             greater reductions in switch costs following amphetamine
             than individuals with lower levels. The relationship between
             dopamine receptors and stimulant-enhanced flexibility was
             partially mediated by striatal dopamine release.<h4>Conclusions</h4>These
             data indicate that the impact of the psychostimulant on
             cognitive flexibility is influenced by the status of
             dopamine within a thalamocorticostriatal network. Beyond
             demonstrating a link between this dopaminergic network and
             the enhancement in task switching, these neural measures
             accounted for unique variance in predicting the
             psychostimulant-induced cognitive enhancement. These results
             suggest that there may be measurable aspects of variability
             in the dopamine system that predispose certain individuals
             to benefit from and hence use psychostimulants for cognitive
             enhancement.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.032},
   Key = {fds318758}
}

@article{fds318757,
   Author = {Hills, TT and Mata, R and Wilke, A and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Mechanisms of age-related decline in memory search across
             the adult life span.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {2396-2404},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032272},
   Abstract = {Three alternative mechanisms for age-related decline in
             memory search have been proposed, which result from either
             reduced processing speed (global slowing hypothesis),
             overpersistence on categories (cluster-switching
             hypothesis), or the inability to maintain focus on local
             cues related to a decline in working memory (cue-maintenance
             hypothesis). We investigated these 3 hypotheses by formally
             modeling the semantic recall patterns of 185 adults between
             27 to 99 years of age in the animal fluency task (Thurstone,
             1938). The results indicate that people switch between
             global frequency-based retrieval cues and local item-based
             retrieval cues to navigate their semantic memory. Contrary
             to the global slowing hypothesis that predicts no
             qualitative differences in dynamic search processes and the
             cluster-switching hypothesis that predicts reduced switching
             between retrieval cues, the results indicate that as people
             age, they tend to switch more often between local and global
             cues per item recalled, supporting the cue-maintenance
             hypothesis. Additional support for the cue-maintenance
             hypothesis is provided by a negative correlation between
             switching and digit span scores and between switching and
             total items recalled, which suggests that cognitive control
             may be involved in cue maintenance and the effective search
             of memory. Overall, the results are consistent with
             age-related decline in memory search being a consequence of
             reduced cognitive control, consistent with models suggesting
             that working memory is related to goal perseveration and the
             ability to inhibit distracting information.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0032272},
   Key = {fds318757}
}

@article{fds318755,
   Author = {Benningfield, MM and Blackford, JU and Ellsworth, ME and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Martin, PR and Cowan, RL and Zald,
             DH},
   Title = {Caudate responses to reward anticipation associated with
             delay discounting behavior in healthy youth.},
   Journal = {Developmental cognitive neuroscience},
   Volume = {7},
   Pages = {43-52},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.10.009},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>Choices requiring delay of gratification
             made during adolescence can have significant impact on life
             trajectory. Willingness to delay gratification can be
             measured using delay discounting tasks that require a choice
             between a smaller immediate reward and a larger delayed
             reward. Individual differences in the subjective value of
             delayed rewards are associated with risk for development of
             psychopathology including substance abuse. The
             neurobiological underpinnings related to these individual
             differences early in life are not fully understood. Using
             functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we tested the
             hypothesis that individual differences in delay discounting
             behavior in healthy youth are related to differences in
             responsiveness to potential reward.<h4>Method</h4>Nineteen
             10-14 year-olds performed a monetary incentive delay task to
             assess neural sensitivity to potential reward and a
             questionnaire to measure discounting of future monetary
             rewards.<h4>Results</h4>Left ventromedial caudate activation
             during anticipation of potential reward was negatively
             correlated with delay discounting behavior. There were no
             regions where brain responses during notification of reward
             outcome were associated with discounting
             behavior.<h4>Conclusions</h4>Brain activation during
             anticipation of potential reward may serve as a marker for
             individual differences in ability or willingness to delay
             gratification in healthy youth.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.dcn.2013.10.009},
   Key = {fds318755}
}

@article{fds318756,
   Author = {Wu, CC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Katovich, K and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of
             incentive anticipation.},
   Journal = {NeuroImage},
   Volume = {84},
   Pages = {279-289},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.055},
   Abstract = {While theorists have speculated that different affective
             traits are linked to reliable brain activity during
             anticipation of gains and losses, few have directly tested
             this prediction. We examined these associations in a
             community sample of healthy human adults (n=52) as they
             played a Monetary Incentive Delay task while undergoing
             functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI). Factor
             analysis of personality measures revealed that subjects
             independently varied in trait Positive Arousal and trait
             Negative Arousal. In a subsample (n=14) retested over
             2.5years later, left nucleus accumbens (NAcc) activity
             during anticipation of large gains (+$5.00) and right
             anterior insula activity during anticipation of large losses
             (-$5.00) showed significant test-retest reliability
             (intraclass correlations>0.50, p's<0.01). In the full sample
             (n=52), trait Positive Arousal correlated with individual
             differences in left NAcc activity during anticipation of
             large gains, while trait Negative Arousal correlated with
             individual differences in right anterior insula activity
             during anticipation of large losses. Associations of
             affective traits with neural activity were not attributable
             to the influence of other potential confounds (including
             sex, age, wealth, and motion). Together, these results
             demonstrate selective links between distinct affective
             traits and reliably-elicited activity in neural circuits
             associated with anticipation of gain versus loss. The
             findings thus reveal neural markers for affective dimensions
             of healthy personality, and potentially for related
             psychiatric symptoms.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.055},
   Key = {fds318756}
}

@misc{fds325714,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Hagen, TA and Weiner, DJ},
   Title = {Financial decision making across adulthood},
   Pages = {121-135},
   Booktitle = {The Psychological Science of Money},
   Publisher = {Springer New York},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9781493909582},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0959-9_6},
   Abstract = {Choices about money have serious consequences both for
             individuals and society, as reckless spending by young
             adults and financial scamming of the elderly all too clearly
             demonstrate. Recent evidence from psychology and
             neuroscience suggests that financial decision making
             capacity may peak at middle age, with unique vulnerabilities
             manifesting early and late in life. In this chapter, we
             review age differences in performance on a series of
             financial decision making tasks, including those involving
             monetary gain and loss, learning and risk, and intertemporal
             choice. Taken together, the evidence suggests that older
             adults do well when making decisions that rely on
             accumulated life experience and perform suboptimally in
             uncertain and novel environments that require fluid
             learning. Brain imaging reveals declines in frontostriatal
             function in the elderly that may explain the observed
             challenges on these dynamic behavioral decision tasks. In an
             effort to translate these findings from the lab to society,
             a small and growing literature has identified real-world
             financial decision correlates of performance on laboratory
             tasks. Such studies hold enormous promise for developing
             tools that can identify individuals at greater risk for poor
             financial decision making.},
   Doi = {10.1007/978-1-4939-0959-9_6},
   Key = {fds325714}
}

@article{fds318754,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Worthy, DA and Mata, R and McClure, SM and Knutson, B},
   Title = {Adult age differences in frontostriatal representation of
             prediction error but not reward outcome.},
   Journal = {Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {672-682},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0297-4},
   Abstract = {Emerging evidence from decision neuroscience suggests that
             although younger and older adults show similar
             frontostriatal representations of reward magnitude, older
             adults often show deficits in feedback-driven reinforcement
             learning. In the present study, healthy adults completed
             reward-based tasks that did or did not depend on
             probabilistic learning, while undergoing functional
             neuroimaging. We observed reductions in the frontostriatal
             representation of prediction errors during probabilistic
             learning in older adults. In contrast, we found evidence for
             stability across adulthood in the representation of reward
             outcome in a task that did not require learning. Together,
             the results identify changes across adulthood in the dynamic
             coding of relational representations of feedback, in spite
             of preserved reward sensitivity in old age. Overall, the
             results suggest that the neural representation of prediction
             error, but not reward outcome, is reduced in old age. These
             findings reveal a potential dissociation between cognition
             and motivation with age and identify a potential mechanism
             for explaining changes in learning-dependent decision making
             in old adulthood.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13415-014-0297-4},
   Key = {fds318754}
}

@article{fds318753,
   Author = {Braver, TS and Krug, MK and Chiew, KS and Kool, W and Westbrook, JA and Clement, NJ and Adcock, RA and Barch, DM and Botvinick, MM and Carver,
             CS and Cools, R and Custers, R and Dickinson, A and Dweck, CS and Fishbach,
             A and Gollwitzer, PM and Hess, TM and Isaacowitz, DM and Mather, M and Murayama, K and Pessoa, L and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Somerville, LH and MOMCAI group},
   Title = {Mechanisms of motivation-cognition interaction: challenges
             and opportunities.},
   Journal = {Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {443-472},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0300-0},
   Abstract = {Recent years have seen a rejuvenation of interest in studies
             of motivation-cognition interactions arising from many
             different areas of psychology and neuroscience. The present
             issue of Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
             provides a sampling of some of the latest research from a
             number of these different areas. In this introductory
             article, we provide an overview of the current state of the
             field, in terms of key research developments and candidate
             neural mechanisms receiving focused investigation as
             potential sources of motivation-cognition interaction.
             However, our primary goal is conceptual: to highlight the
             distinct perspectives taken by different research areas, in
             terms of how motivation is defined, the relevant dimensions
             and dissociations that are emphasized, and the theoretical
             questions being targeted. Together, these distinctions
             present both challenges and opportunities for efforts aiming
             toward a more unified and cross-disciplinary approach. We
             identify a set of pressing research questions calling for
             this sort of cross-disciplinary approach, with the explicit
             goal of encouraging integrative and collaborative
             investigations directed toward them.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13415-014-0300-0},
   Key = {fds318753}
}

@misc{fds325712,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Chapter 3 - Decision Neuroscience and Aging},
   Pages = {41-60},
   Booktitle = {Aging and Decision Making: Empirical and Applied
             Perspectives},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9780124171480},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-417148-0.00003-0},
   Abstract = {Over the past several years, a subfield of the cognitive
             neuroscience of aging has emerged to investigate age
             differences in reward-based decision making across
             adulthood. The approach combines experimental methods,
             models, and theory from psychology, economics, and
             neuroscience to characterize age differences in decision
             making in the laboratory and in the real world. This chapter
             reviews what is presently known about how age differences in
             the structure and function of frontostriatal brain systems
             supporting reward-based decision making are related to age
             differences in sensitivity to monetary gains and losses,
             intertemporal decision making, risky decision making, and
             reward learning. Already this work has identified
             interesting divergent patterns across adulthood; in some
             situations, the elderly outperform young adults and in other
             situations they appear to make more mistakes. Taken
             together, the evidence suggests that older adults do well
             when making decisions that rely on accumulated life
             experience, and perform suboptimally in uncertain
             environments that require the fluid integration of novel
             information.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-417148-0.00003-0},
   Key = {fds325712}
}

@misc{fds325713,
   Author = {Sofia Beas and B and Setlow, B and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Bizon,
             JL},
   Title = {Chapter 2 - Modeling Cost-Benefit Decision Making in Aged
             Rodents},
   Pages = {17-40},
   Booktitle = {Aging and Decision Making: Empirical and Applied
             Perspectives},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9780124171480},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-417148-0.00002-9},
   Abstract = {Aging can impact choices between alternatives that differ
             with respect to relative benefits and "costs" (e.g., time
             delays or risk); however, much remains to be learned about
             the specific cognitive, affective, and neural factors that
             govern choice behavior across the life span. Relative to
             primates, rodents offer both comparatively short life spans
             that facilitate longitudinal evaluation of cognition, and
             enhanced tractability of genetic, cellular, and biochemical
             approaches important for identifying the neurobiological
             mechanisms that mediate decision making. This chapter will
             describe approaches for modeling cost-benefit decision
             making in aged rodents, with a focus on intertemporal and
             risky choice. In addition, examples will be provided of how
             these approaches have yielded convergent findings in animal
             and human subjects, as well as novel data regarding
             neurobehavioral mechanisms of age-related alterations in
             decision making and potential directions for future
             research.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-417148-0.00002-9},
   Key = {fds325713}
}

@article{fds318752,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Knutson, B},
   Title = {Decision making in the ageing brain: changes in affective
             and motivational circuits.},
   Journal = {Nature reviews. Neuroscience},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {278-289},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3917},
   Abstract = {As the global population ages, older decision makers will be
             required to take greater responsibility for their own
             physical, psychological and financial well-being. With this
             in mind, researchers have begun to examine the effects of
             ageing on decision making and associated neural circuits. A
             new 'affect-integration-motivation' (AIM) framework may help
             to clarify how affective and motivational circuits support
             decision making. Recent research has shed light on whether
             and how ageing influences these circuits, providing an
             interdisciplinary account of how ageing can alter decision
             making.},
   Doi = {10.1038/nrn3917},
   Key = {fds318752}
}

@article{fds318751,
   Author = {Dang, LC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Young, JS and Cowan, RL and Kessler,
             RM and Zald, DH},
   Title = {Caudate asymmetry is related to attentional impulsivity and
             an objective measure of ADHD-like attentional problems in
             healthy adults.},
   Journal = {Brain structure & function},
   Volume = {221},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {277-286},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0906-6},
   Abstract = {Case-control studies comparing ADHD with typically
             developing individuals suggest that anatomical asymmetry of
             the caudate nucleus is a marker of attention deficit
             hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, there is no
             consensus on whether the asymmetry favors the right or left
             caudate nucleus in ADHD, or whether the asymmetry is
             increased or decreased in ADHD. The current study aimed to
             clarify this relationship by applying a dimensional approach
             to assessing ADHD symptoms that, instead of relying on
             clinical classification, utilizes the natural behavioral
             continuum of traits related to ADHD. Structural T1-weighted
             MRI was collected from 71 adults between 18 and 35 years
             and analyzed for caudate asymmetry. ADHD-like attentional
             symptoms were assessed with an objective measure of
             attentional problems, the ADHD score from the Test of
             Variables of Attention (TOVA). Impulsivity, a core feature
             in ADHD, was measured using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale,
             a self-report measure that assesses attentional,
             non-planning, and motor features of impulsivity. We found
             that larger right relative to left caudate volumes
             correlated with both higher attentional impulsiveness and
             worse ADHD scores on the TOVA. Higher attentional
             impulsiveness also correlated with worse ADHD scores,
             establishing coherence between the objective measure and the
             self-report measure of attentional problems. These results
             suggest that a differential passage of information through
             frontal-striatal networks may produce instability leading to
             attentional problems. The findings also demonstrate the
             utility of a dimensional approach to understanding
             structural correlates of ADHD symptoms.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00429-014-0906-6},
   Key = {fds318751}
}

@article{fds318749,
   Author = {Kline, RL and Zhang, S and Farr, OM and Hu, S and Zaborszky, L and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Li, C-SR},
   Title = {The Effects of Methylphenidate on Resting-State Functional
             Connectivity of the Basal Nucleus of Meynert, Locus
             Coeruleus, and Ventral Tegmental Area in Healthy
             Adults.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in human neuroscience},
   Volume = {10},
   Pages = {149},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00149},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>Methylphenidate (MPH) influences
             catecholaminergic signaling. Extant work examined the
             effects of MPH on the neural circuits of attention and
             cognitive control, but few studies have investigated the
             effect of MPH on the brain's resting-state functional
             connectivity (rsFC).<h4>Methods</h4>In this observational
             study, we compared rsFC of a group of 24 healthy adults who
             were administered an oral 45 mg dose of MPH with a group of
             24 age and gender matched controls who did not receive MPH.
             We focused on three seed regions: basal nucleus of Meynert
             (BNM), locus coeruleus (LC), and ventral tegmental
             area/substantia nigra, pars compacta (VTA/SNc), each
             providing cholinergic, noradrenergic and dopaminergic inputs
             to the cerebral cortex. Images were pre-processed and
             analyzed as in our recent work (Li et al., 2014; Zhang et
             al., 2015). We used one-sample t-test to characterize
             group-specific rsFC of each seed region and two-sample
             t-test to compare rsFC between groups.<h4>Results</h4>MPH
             reversed negative connectivity between BNM and precentral
             gyri. MPH reduced positive connectivity between LC and
             cerebellum, and induced positive connectivity between LC and
             right hippocampus. MPH decreased positive VTA/SNc
             connectivity to the cerebellum and putamen, and reduced
             negative connectivity to left middle occipital
             gyrus.<h4>Conclusion</h4>MPH had distinct effects on the
             rsFC of BNM, LC, and VTA/SNc in healthy adults. These new
             findings may further our understanding of the role of
             catecholaminergic signaling in Attention Deficit
             Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Parkinson's disease and
             provide insights into the therapeutic mechanisms of MPH in
             the treatment of clinical conditions that implicate
             catecholaminergic dysfunction.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fnhum.2016.00149},
   Key = {fds318749}
}

@article{fds318750,
   Author = {Leong, JK and Pestilli, F and Wu, CC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {White-Matter Tract Connecting Anterior Insula to Nucleus
             Accumbens Correlates with Reduced Preference for Positively
             Skewed Gambles.},
   Journal = {Neuron},
   Volume = {89},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {63-69},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.015},
   Abstract = {Individuals sometimes show inconsistent risk preferences,
             including excessive attraction to gambles featuring small
             chances of winning large amounts (called "positively skewed"
             gambles). While functional neuroimaging research indicates
             that nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and anterior insula (AIns)
             activity inversely predict risky choice, structural
             connections between these regions have not been described in
             humans. By combining diffusion-weighted MRI with
             tractography, we identified the anatomical trajectory of
             white-matter tracts projecting from the AIns to the NAcc and
             statistically validated these tracts using Linear Fascicle
             Evaluation (LiFE) and virtual lesions. Coherence of the
             right AIns-NAcc tract correlated with reduced preferences
             for positively skewed gambles. Further, diminished NAcc
             activity during gamble presentation mediated the association
             between tract structure and choice. These results identify
             an unreported tract connecting the AIns to the NAcc in
             humans and support the notion that structural connections
             can alter behavior by influencing brain activity as
             individuals weigh uncertain gains against uncertain
             losses.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.015},
   Key = {fds318750}
}

@article{fds318747,
   Author = {Dang, LC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Castrellon, JJ and Perkins, SF and Cowan, RL and Zald, DH},
   Title = {Associations between dopamine D2 receptor availability and
             BMI depend on age.},
   Journal = {NeuroImage},
   Volume = {138},
   Pages = {176-183},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.044},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objective</h4>The dopamine D2/3 receptor subtypes
             (DRD2/3) are the most widely studied neurotransmitter
             biomarker in research on obesity, but results to date have
             been inconsistent, have typically involved small samples,
             and have rarely accounted for subjects' ages despite the
             large impact of age on DRD2/3 levels. We aimed to clarify
             the relation between DRD2/3 availability and BMI by
             examining this association in a large sample of subjects
             with BMI spanning the continuum from underweight to
             extremely obese.<h4>Subjects</h4>130 healthy subjects
             between 18 and 81years old underwent PET with
             [18F]fallypride, a high affinity DRD2/3 ligand.<h4>Results</h4>As
             expected, DRD2/3 availability declined with age. Critically,
             age significantly interacted with DRD2/3 availability in
             predicting BMI in the midbrain and striatal regions
             (caudate, putamen, and ventral striatum). Among subjects
             under 30years old, BMI was not associated with DRD2/3
             availability. By contrast, among subjects over 30years old,
             BMI was positively associated with DRD2/3 availability in
             the midbrain, putamen, and ventral striatum.<h4>Conclusion</h4>The
             present results are incompatible with the prominent
             dopaminergic hypofunction hypothesis that proposes that a
             reduction in DRD2/3 availability is associated with
             increased BMI, and highlights the importance of age in
             assessing correlates of DRD2/3 function.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.044},
   Key = {fds318747}
}

@article{fds318748,
   Author = {Josef, AK and Richter, D and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Wagner, GG and Hertwig, R and Mata, R},
   Title = {Stability and change in risk-taking propensity across the
             adult life span.},
   Journal = {Journal of personality and social psychology},
   Volume = {111},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {430-450},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000090},
   Abstract = {Can risk-taking propensity be thought of as a trait that
             captures individual differences across domains, measures,
             and time? Studying stability in risk-taking propensities
             across the life span can help to answer such questions by
             uncovering parallel, or divergent, trajectories across
             domains and measures. We contribute to this effort by using
             data from respondents aged 18 to 85 in the German
             Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP) and by examining (a)
             differential stability, (b) mean-level differences, and (c)
             individual-level changes in self-reported general (N =
             44,076) and domain-specific (N = 11,903) risk-taking
             propensities across adulthood. In addition, we investigate
             (d) the correspondence between cross-sectional trajectories
             of self-report and behavioral measures of social (trust
             game; N = 646) and nonsocial (monetary gamble; N = 433) risk
             taking. The results suggest that risk-taking propensity can
             be understood as a trait with moderate stability. Results
             show reliable mean-level differences across the life span,
             with risk-taking propensities typically decreasing with age,
             although significant variation emerges across domains and
             individuals. Interestingly, the mean-level trajectory for
             behavioral measures of social and nonsocial risk taking was
             similar to those obtained from self-reported risk, despite
             small correlations between task behavior and self-reports.
             Individual-level analyses suggest a link between changes in
             risk-taking propensities both across domains and in relation
             to changes in some of the Big Five personality traits.
             Overall, these results raise important questions concerning
             the role of common processes or events that shape the life
             span development of risk-taking across domains as well as
             other major personality facets. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pspp0000090},
   Key = {fds318748}
}

@article{fds318746,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Gorlick, MA and Vekaria, KM and Hsu, M and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Adult age differences in decision making across domains:
             Increased discounting of social and health-related
             rewards.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {737-746},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000131},
   Abstract = {Although research on aging and decision making continues to
             grow, the majority of studies examine decisions made to
             maximize monetary earnings or points. It is not clear
             whether these results generalize to other types of rewards.
             To investigate this, we examined adult age differences in 92
             healthy participants aged 22 to 83. Participants completed 9
             hypothetical discounting tasks, which included 3 types of
             discounting factors (time, probability, effort) across 3
             reward domains (monetary, social, health). Participants made
             choices between a smaller magnitude reward with a shorter
             time delay/higher probability/lower level of physical effort
             required and a larger magnitude reward with a longer time
             delay/lower probability/higher level of physical effort
             required. Older compared with younger individuals were more
             likely to choose options that involved shorter time delays
             or higher probabilities of experiencing an interaction with
             a close social partner or receiving health benefits from a
             hypothetical drug. These findings suggest that older adults
             may be more motivated than young adults to obtain social and
             health rewards immediately and with certainty. (PsycINFO
             Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000131},
   Key = {fds318746}
}

@article{fds325039,
   Author = {Löckenhoff, CE and Rutt, JL and Samanez-Larkin, GR and O'Donoghue,
             T and Reyna, VF and Ganzel, B},
   Title = {Dread sensitivity in decisions about real and imagined
             electrical shocks does not vary by age.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {890-901},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000136},
   Abstract = {Previous research has found age differences in intertemporal
             choices that involve trade-offs among events or outcomes
             that occur at different points in time, but these findings
             were mostly limited to hypothetical financial and consumer
             choices. We examined whether age effects extend to
             unpleasant physical experiences that elicit states of dread
             which lead participants to speed up the outcomes just to get
             them over with. We asked participants of different ages to
             choose among electrical shocks that varied in timing and
             intensity. We also assessed affective responses as a
             potential mechanism behind age effects and considered other
             potential covariates. In Study 1, the choice task involved
             real outcomes and the sample consisted of younger and older
             adults. In Study 2, the choice task was hypothetical and the
             sample was an adult life span sample. Across both studies,
             there was no evidence of age differences in the preferred
             timing of shocks. Instead, dread-sensitive choices were
             associated with higher conscientiousness. Age effects in
             dread-sensitive choices remained nonsignificant even after
             controlling for a range of age-associated covariates. We
             discuss possible explanations for the lack of age effects
             and consider implications for applied and clinical settings.
             (PsycINFO Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000136},
   Key = {fds325039}
}

@article{fds325038,
   Author = {Dang, LC and Castrellon, JJ and Perkins, SF and Le, NT and Cowan, RL and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Reduced effects of age on dopamine D2 receptor levels in
             physically active adults.},
   Journal = {NeuroImage},
   Volume = {148},
   Pages = {123-129},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.018},
   Abstract = {Physical activity has been shown to ameliorate dopaminergic
             degeneration in non-human animal models. However, the
             effects of regular physical activity on normal age-related
             changes in dopamine function in humans are unknown. Here we
             present cross-sectional data from forty-four healthy human
             subjects between 23 and 80 years old, showing that typical
             age-related dopamine D2 receptor loss, assessed with PET
             [18F]fallypride, was significantly reduced in physically
             active adults compared to less active adults.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.018},
   Key = {fds325038}
}

@article{fds327149,
   Author = {Hosking, JG and Kastman, EK and Dorfman, HM and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Baskin-Sommers, A and Kiehl, KA and Newman, JP and Buckholtz,
             JW},
   Title = {Disrupted Prefrontal Regulation of Striatal Subjective Value
             Signals in Psychopathy.},
   Journal = {Neuron},
   Volume = {95},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {221-231.e4},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.030},
   Abstract = {Psychopathy is a personality disorder with strong links to
             criminal behavior. While research on psychopathy has focused
             largely on socio-affective dysfunction, recent data suggest
             that aberrant decision making may also play an important
             role. Yet, the circuit-level mechanisms underlying
             maladaptive decision making in psychopathy remain unclear.
             Here, we used a multi-modality functional imaging approach
             to identify these mechanisms in a population of adult male
             incarcerated offenders. Psychopathy was associated with
             stronger subjective value-related activity within the
             nucleus accumbens (NAcc) during inter-temporal choice and
             with weaker intrinsic functional connectivity between NAcc
             and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). NAcc-vmPFC
             connectivity strength was negatively correlated with NAcc
             subjective value-related activity; however, this putative
             regulatory pattern was abolished as psychopathy severity
             increased. Finally, weaker cortico-striatal regulation
             predicted more frequent criminal convictions. These data
             suggest that cortico-striatal circuit dysregulation drives
             maladaptive decision making in psychopathy, supporting the
             notion that reward system dysfunction comprises an important
             neurobiological risk factor.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.030},
   Key = {fds327149}
}

@article{fds328897,
   Author = {Dang, LC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Castrellon, JJ and Perkins, SF and Cowan, RL and Newhouse, PA and Zald, DH},
   Title = {Spontaneous Eye Blink Rate (EBR) Is Uncorrelated with
             Dopamine D2 Receptor Availability and Unmodulated by
             Dopamine Agonism in Healthy Adults.},
   Journal = {eNeuro},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {ENEURO.0211-ENEU17.2017},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/eneuro.0211-17.2017},
   Abstract = {Spontaneous eye blink rate (EBR) has been proposed as a
             noninvasive, inexpensive marker of dopamine functioning.
             Support for a relation between EBR and dopamine function
             comes from observations that EBR is altered in populations
             with dopamine dysfunction and EBR changes under a
             dopaminergic manipulation. However, the evidence across the
             literature is inconsistent and incomplete. A direct
             correlation between EBR and dopamine function has so far
             been observed only in nonhuman animals. Given significant
             interest in using EBR as a proxy for dopamine function, this
             study aimed to verify a direct association in healthy, human
             adults. Here we measured EBR in healthy human subjects whose
             dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) availability was assessed with
             positron emission tomography (PET)-[18F]fallypride to
             examine the predictive power of EBR for DRD2 availability.
             Effects of the dopamine agonist bromocriptine on EBR also
             were examined to determine the responsiveness of EBR to
             dopaminergic stimulation and, in light of the hypothesized
             inverted-U profile of dopamine effects, the role of DRD2
             availability in EBR responsivity to bromocriptine. Results
             from 20 subjects (age 33.6 ± 7.6 years, 9F) showed no
             relation between EBR and DRD2 availability. EBR also was not
             responsive to dopaminergic stimulation by bromocriptine, and
             individual differences in DRD2 availability did not modulate
             EBR responsivity to bromocriptine. Given that EBR is
             hypothesized to be particularly sensitive to DRD2 function,
             these findings suggest caution in using EBR as a proxy for
             dopamine function in healthy humans.},
   Doi = {10.1523/eneuro.0211-17.2017},
   Key = {fds328897}
}

@article{fds326610,
   Author = {Karrer, TM and Josef, AK and Mata, R and Morris, ED and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Reduced dopamine receptors and transporters but not
             synthesis capacity in normal aging adults: a
             meta-analysis.},
   Journal = {Neurobiology of aging},
   Volume = {57},
   Pages = {36-46},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.006},
   Abstract = {Many theories of cognitive aging are based on evidence that
             dopamine (DA) declines with age. Here, we performed a
             systematic meta-analysis of cross-sectional positron
             emission tomography and single-photon emission-computed
             tomography studies on the average effects of age on distinct
             DA targets (receptors, transporters, or relevant enzymes) in
             healthy adults (N = 95 studies including 2611
             participants). Results revealed significant moderate to
             large, negative effects of age on DA transporters and
             receptors. Age had a significantly larger effect on D1- than
             D2-like receptors. In contrast, there was no significant
             effect of age on DA synthesis capacity. The average age
             reductions across the DA system were 3.7%-14.0% per decade.
             A meta-regression found only DA target as a significant
             moderator of the age effect. This study precisely quantifies
             prior claims of reduced DA functionality with age. It also
             identifies presynaptic mechanisms (spared synthesis capacity
             and reduced DA transporters) that may partially account for
             previously unexplained phenomena whereby older adults appear
             to use dopaminergic resources effectively. Recommendations
             for future studies including minimum required samples sizes
             are provided.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.006},
   Key = {fds326610}
}

@article{fds329469,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Leong, JK and Wu, CC and Knutson, B and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Individual differences in skewed financial risk-taking
             across the adult life span.},
   Journal = {Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1232-1241},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017-0545-5},
   Abstract = {Older adults are disproportionately targeted by fraud
             schemes that advertise unlikely but large returns
             (positively skewed risks). We examined adult age differences
             in choice and neural activity as individuals considered
             risky gambles. Gambles were symmetric (50% chance of modest
             win or loss), positively skewed (25% chance of large gain),
             or negatively skewed (25% chance of large loss). The
             willingness to accept positively skewed relative to
             symmetric gambles increased with age, and this effect
             replicated in an independent behavioral study. Whole-brain
             functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses comparing
             positively (vs. negatively) skewed trials revealed that
             relative to younger adults, older adults showed increased
             anticipatory activity for negatively skewed gambles but
             reduced activity for positively skewed gambles in the
             anterior cingulate and lateral prefrontal regions.
             Individuals who were more biased toward positively skewed
             gambles showed increased activity in a network of regions
             including the nucleus accumbens. These results reveal age
             biases toward positively skewed gambles and age differences
             in corticostriatal regions during skewed risk-taking, and
             have implications for identifying financial decision biases
             across adulthood.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13415-017-0545-5},
   Key = {fds329469}
}

@article{fds357570,
   Author = {Gutchess, A and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Social Function and Motivation in the Aging
             Brain},
   Pages = {165-184},
   Booktitle = {The aging brain: Functional adaptation across
             adulthood.},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association},
   Year = {2018},
   ISBN = {1433830531},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cv5jn},
   Abstract = {<p>In this chapter, we review some themes from the emerging
             literature on the social neuroscience of aging. Much of the
             research thus far focuses on abilities at the intersection
             of social function and emotion, such as empathy or thinking
             about the self or other people. Intriguingly, findings from
             social and motivational tasks largely depart from the
             lessons about brain aging derived from cognitive tasks.
             There are hints that strategy or goal shifts across the life
             span may underlie some of the age differences, which
             illustrate the importance of considering task context and
             motivation across the life span.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/cv5jn},
   Key = {fds357570}
}

@article{fds335712,
   Author = {Kircanski, K and Notthoff, N and DeLiema, M and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Shadel, D and Mottola, G and Carstensen, LL and Gotlib,
             IH},
   Title = {Emotional arousal may increase susceptibility to fraud in
             older and younger adults.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {325-337},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000228},
   Abstract = {Financial fraud is a societal problem for adults of all
             ages, but financial losses are especially damaging to older
             adults who typically live on fixed incomes and have less
             time to recoup losses. Persuasion tactics used by fraud
             perpetrators often elicit high levels of emotional arousal;
             thus, studying emotional arousal may help to identify the
             conditions under which individuals are particularly
             susceptible to fraud. We examined whether inducing
             high-arousal positive (HAP) and high-arousal negative (HAN)
             emotions increased susceptibility to fraud. Older (ages 65
             to 85) and younger (ages 30 to 40) adults were randomly
             assigned to 1 of 3 emotional arousal conditions in a
             laboratory task: HAP, HAN, or low arousal (LA). Fraud
             susceptibility was assessed through participants' responses
             to misleading advertisements. Both HAP and HAN emotions were
             successfully induced in older and younger participants. For
             participants who exhibited the intended induced emotional
             arousal, both the HAP and HAN conditions, relative to the LA
             condition, significantly increased participants' reported
             intention to purchase falsely advertised items. These
             effects did not differ significantly between older and
             younger adults and were mitigated in participants who did
             not exhibit the intended emotional arousal. However,
             irrespective of the emotional arousal condition to which
             older adults were assigned (HAP, HAN, or LA), they reported
             greater purchase intention than did younger adults. These
             results inform the literature on fraud susceptibility and
             aging. Educating consumers to postpone financial decisions
             until they are in calm emotional states may protect against
             this common persuasion tactic. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000228},
   Key = {fds335712}
}

@article{fds335711,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Brooks, N and Karrer, TM and Castrellon, JJ and Perkins,
             SF and Dang, LC and Hsu, M and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Subjective value representations during effort, probability
             and time discounting across adulthood.},
   Journal = {Social cognitive and affective neuroscience},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {449-459},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy021},
   Abstract = {Every day, humans make countless decisions that require the
             integration of information about potential benefits (i.e.
             rewards) with other decision features (i.e. effort required,
             probability of an outcome or time delays). Here, we examine
             the overlap and dissociation of behavioral preferences and
             neural representations of subjective value in the context of
             three different decision features (physical effort,
             probability and time delays) in a healthy adult life span
             sample. While undergoing functional neuroimaging,
             participants (N = 75) made incentive compatible choices
             between a smaller monetary reward with lower physical
             effort, higher probability, or a shorter time delay versus a
             larger monetary reward with higher physical effort, lower
             probability, or a longer time delay. Behavioral preferences
             were estimated from observed choices, and subjective values
             were computed using individual hyperbolic discount
             functions. We found that discount rates were uncorrelated
             across tasks. Despite this apparent behavioral dissociation
             between preferences, we found overlapping subjective
             value-related activity in the medial prefrontal cortex
             across all three tasks. We found no consistent evidence for
             age differences in either preferences or the neural
             representations of subjective value across adulthood. These
             results suggest that while the tolerance of decision
             features is behaviorally dissociable, subjective value
             signals share a common representation across
             adulthood.},
   Doi = {10.1093/scan/nsy021},
   Key = {fds335711}
}

@article{fds335709,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Green, MA and Shu, S and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Individual differences in loss aversion and preferences for
             skewed risks across adulthood.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {654-659},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000261},
   Abstract = {In a previous study, we found adult age differences in the
             tendency to accept more positively skewed gambles (with a
             small chance of a large win) than other equivalent risks, or
             an age-related positive-skew bias. In the present study, we
             examined whether loss aversion explained this bias. A total
             of 508 healthy participants (ages 21-82) completed measures
             of loss aversion and skew preference. Age was not related to
             loss aversion. Although loss aversion was a significant
             predictor of gamble acceptance, it did not influence the
             age-related positive-skew bias. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000261},
   Key = {fds335709}
}

@article{fds335708,
   Author = {von Helversen, B and Mata, R and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Wilke,
             A},
   Title = {Foraging, exploration, or search? On the (lack of)
             convergent validity between three behavioral
             paradigms},
   Journal = {Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {152-162},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000121},
   Abstract = {Recently it has been suggested that individual humans and
             other animals possess different levels of a general tendency
             to explore or exploit that may influence behavior in
             different contexts. In the present work, we investigated
             whether individual differences in this general tendency to
             explore (exploit) can be captured across three behavioral
             paradigms that involve exploration- exploitation trade-offs:
             A foraging task involving sequential search for fish in
             several ponds, a multiarmed bandit task involving repeatedly
             choosing from a set of options, and a sequential choice task
             involving choosing a candidate from a pool of applicants.
             Two hundred and sixty-one participants completed two
             versions of each of the three tasks. Structural equation
             modeling revealed that there was no single, general factor
             underlying exploration behavior in all tasks, even though
             individual differences in exploration were stable across the
             two versions of the same task. The results suggest that
             task-specific factors influence individual levels of
             exploration. This finding causes difficulties in the
             enterprise of measuring general exploration tendencies using
             single behavioral paradigms and suggests that more work is
             needed to understand how general exploration tendencies and
             task-specific characteristics translate into exploratory
             behavior in different contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1037/ebs0000121},
   Key = {fds335708}
}

@article{fds331245,
   Author = {Dang, LC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Smith, CT and Castrellon, JJ and Perkins, SF and Cowan, RL and Claassen, DO and Zald,
             DH},
   Title = {FTO affects food cravings and interacts with age to
             influence age-related decline in food cravings.},
   Journal = {Physiology & behavior},
   Volume = {192},
   Pages = {188-193},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.013},
   Abstract = {The fat mass and obesity associated gene (FTO) was the first
             gene identified by genome-wide association studies to
             correlate with higher body mass index (BMI) and increased
             odds of obesity. FTO remains the locus with the largest and
             most replicated effect on body weight, but the mechanism
             whereby FTO affects body weight and the development of
             obesity is not fully understood. Here we tested whether FTO
             is associated with differences in food cravings and a key
             aspect of dopamine function that has been hypothesized to
             influence food reward mechanisms. Moreover, as food cravings
             and dopamine function are known to decline with age, we
             explored effects of age on relations between FTO and food
             cravings and dopamine function. Seven-eight healthy subjects
             between 22 and 83years old completed the Food Cravings
             Questionnaire and underwent genotyping for FTO rs9939609,
             the first FTO single nucleotide polymorphism associated with
             obesity. Compared to TT homozygotes, individuals carrying
             the obesity-susceptible A allele had higher total food
             cravings, which correlated with higher BMI. Additionally,
             food cravings declined with age, but this age effect
             differed across variants of FTO rs9939609: while TT
             homozygotes showed the typical age-related decline in food
             cravings, there was no such decline among A carriers. All
             subjects were scanned with [18F]fallypride PET to assess a
             recent proposal that at the neurochemical level FTO alters
             dopamine D2-like receptor (DRD2) function to influence food
             reward related mechanisms. However, we observed no evidence
             of FTO effects on DRD2 availability.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.013},
   Key = {fds331245}
}

@article{fds335710,
   Author = {Dang, LC and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Castrellon, JJ and Perkins, SF and Cowan, RL and Zald, DH},
   Title = {Individual differences in dopamine D2 receptor
             availability correlate with reward valuation.},
   Journal = {Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {739-747},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0601-9},
   Abstract = {Reward valuation, which underlies all value-based
             decision-making, has been associated with dopamine function
             in many studies of nonhuman animals, but there is relatively
             less direct evidence for an association in humans. Here, we
             measured dopamine D<sub>2</sub> receptor (DRD2) availability
             in vivo in humans to examine relations between individual
             differences in dopamine receptor availability and neural
             activity associated with a measure of reward valuation,
             expected value (i.e., the product of reward magnitude and
             the probability of obtaining the reward). Fourteen healthy
             adult subjects underwent PET with [<sup>18</sup>F]fallypride,
             a radiotracer with strong affinity for DRD2, and fMRI (on a
             separate day) while performing a reward valuation task.
             [<sup>18</sup>F]fallypride binding potential, reflecting
             DRD2 availability, in the midbrain correlated positively
             with neural activity associated with expected value,
             specifically in the left ventral striatum/caudate. The
             present results provide in vivo evidence from humans showing
             midbrain dopamine characteristics are associated with reward
             valuation.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13415-018-0601-9},
   Key = {fds335710}
}

@article{fds335707,
   Author = {Leong, JK and MacNiven, KH and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Knutson,
             B},
   Title = {Distinct neural circuits support incentivized
             inhibition.},
   Journal = {NeuroImage},
   Volume = {178},
   Pages = {435-444},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.055},
   Abstract = {The ability to inhibit responses under high stakes, or
             "incentivized inhibition," is critical for adaptive impulse
             control. While previous research indicates that right
             ventrolateral prefrontal cortical (VLPFC) activity plays a
             key role in response inhibition, less research has addressed
             how incentives might influence this circuit. By combining a
             novel behavioral task, functional magnetic resonance imaging
             (FMRI), and diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI), we targeted
             and characterized specific neural circuits that support
             incentivized inhibition. Behaviorally, large incentives
             enhanced responses to obtain money, but also reduced
             response inhibition. Functionally, activity in both right
             VLPFC and right anterior insula (AIns) predicted successful
             inhibition for high incentives. Structurally,
             characterization of a novel white-matter tract connecting
             the right AIns and VLPFC revealed an association of tract
             coherence with incentivized inhibition performance. Finally,
             individual differences in right VLPFC activity statistically
             mediated the association of right AIns-VLPFC tract coherence
             with incentivized inhibition performance. These multimodal
             findings bridge brain structure, brain function, and
             behavior to clarify how individuals can inhibit impulses,
             even in the face of high stakes.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.055},
   Key = {fds335707}
}

@article{fds339363,
   Author = {Karrer, T and McLaughlin, C and Guaglianone, C and Samanez-Larkin,
             G},
   Title = {Reduced serotonin receptors and transporters in normal aging
             adults: a meta-analysis of PET and SPECT imaging
             studies},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/429266},
   Abstract = {Abstract Alterations in serotonin (5-HT) function have been
             hypothesized to underlie a range of physiological,
             emotional, and cognitive changes in older age. Here, we
             conducted a quantitative synthesis and comparison of the
             effects of age on 5-HT receptors and transporters from
             cross-sectional PET and SPECT imaging studies.
             Random-effects meta-analyses of 31 studies including 1087
             healthy adults yielded large negative effects of age in
             5-HT-2A receptors (largest in global cortex), moderate
             negative effects of age in 5-HT transporters (largest in
             thalamus), and small negative effects of age in 5-HT-1A
             receptors (largest in parietal cortex). Presynaptic 5-HT-1A
             autoreceptors in raphe/midbrain, however, were preserved
             across adulthood. Adult age differences were significantly
             larger in 5-HT-2A receptors compared to 5-HT-1A receptors. A
             meta-regression showed that 5-HT target, radionuclide, and
             publication year significantly moderated the age effects.
             The findings overall identify reduced serotonergic signal
             transmission in healthy aging. The evidence for the relative
             preservation of 5-HT-1A compared to 5-HT-2A receptors may
             partially explain psychological age differences, such as why
             older adults use more emotion-focused rather than
             problem-focused coping strategies.},
   Doi = {10.1101/429266},
   Key = {fds339363}
}

@article{fds340435,
   Author = {Smith, CT and San Juan, MD and Dang, LC and Katz, DT and Perkins, SF and Burgess, LL and Cowan, RL and Manning, HC and Nickels, ML and Claassen,
             DO and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Zald, DH},
   Title = {Ventral striatal dopamine transporter availability is
             associated with lower trait motor impulsivity in healthy
             adults.},
   Journal = {Translational psychiatry},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {269},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0328-y},
   Abstract = {Impulsivity is a transdiagnostic feature of a range of
             externalizing psychiatric disorders. Preclinical work links
             reduced ventral striatal dopamine transporter (DAT)
             availability with heightened impulsivity and novelty
             seeking. However, there is a lack of human data
             investigating the relationship between DAT availability,
             particularly in subregions of the striatum, and the
             personality traits of impulsivity and novelty seeking. Here
             we collected PET measures of DAT availability
             (BP<sub>ND</sub>) using the tracer <sup>18</sup>F-FE-PE2I in
             47 healthy adult subjects and examined relations between
             BP<sub>ND</sub> in striatum, including its subregions:
             caudate, putamen, and ventral striatum (VS), and trait
             impulsivity (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: BIS-11) and
             novelty seeking (Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire:
             TPQ-NS), controlling for age and sex. DAT BP<sub>ND</sub> in
             each striatal subregion showed nominal negative associations
             with total BIS-11 but not TPQ-NS. At the subscale level, VS
             DAT BP<sub>ND</sub> was significantly associated with BIS-11
             motor impulsivity (e.g., taking actions without thinking)
             after correction for multiple comparisons. VS DAT
             BP<sub>ND</sub> explained 13.2% of the variance in motor
             impulsivity. Our data demonstrate that DAT availability in
             VS is negatively related to impulsivity and suggest a
             particular influence of DAT regulation of dopamine signaling
             in VS on acting without deliberation (BIS motor
             impulsivity). While needing replication, these data converge
             with models of ventral striatal functions that emphasize its
             role as a key interface linking motivation to
             action.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41398-018-0328-y},
   Key = {fds340435}
}

@article{fds357569,
   Author = {Burr, D and Castrellon, J and Zald, D and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Emotion dynamics across adulthood in everyday life: Older
             adults are more emotionally stable and better at regulating
             desires},
   Year = {2019},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/a3ku2},
   Abstract = {<p>Older adults report experiencing improved emotional
             health, such as more intense positive affect and less
             intense negative affect. However, there are mixed findings
             on whether older adults are better at regulating emotion—a
             hallmark feature of emotional health—and most research is
             based on laboratory studies that may not capture how people
             regulate their emotions in everyday life. We used experience
             sampling to examine how multiple measures of emotional
             health, including mean affect, dynamic fluctuations between
             affective states and the ability to resist desires—a
             common form of emotion regulation—differ in daily life
             across adulthood. Participants (N = 122, ages 20-80)
             reported how they were feeling and responding to desire
             temptations for 10 days. Older adults experienced more
             intense positive affect, less intense negative affect and
             were more emotionally stable, even after controlling for
             individual differences in global life satisfaction. Older
             adults were more successful at regulating desires, even
             though they experienced more intense desires than younger
             adults. In addition, adults in general experiencing more
             intense affect were less successful at resisting desires.
             These results demonstrate how emotional experience is
             related to more successful desire regulation in everyday
             life and provide unique evidence that emotional health and
             regulation improve with age.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/a3ku2},
   Key = {fds357569}
}

@article{fds339897,
   Author = {Castrellon, JJ and Seaman, KL and Crawford, JL and Young, JS and Smith,
             CT and Dang, LC and Hsu, M and Cowan, RL and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Individual Differences in Dopamine Are Associated with
             Reward Discounting in Clinical Groups But Not in Healthy
             Adults.},
   Journal = {The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {321-332},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1984-18.2018},
   Abstract = {Some people are more willing to make immediate, risky, or
             costly reward-focused choices than others, which has been
             hypothesized to be associated with individual differences in
             dopamine (DA) function. In two studies using PET imaging,
             one empirical (Study 1: <i>N</i> = 144 males and females
             across 3 samples) and one meta-analytic (Study 2: <i>N</i> =
             307 across 12 samples), we sought to characterize
             associations between individual differences in DA and time,
             probability, and physical effort discounting in human
             adults. Study 1 demonstrated that individual differences in
             DA D2-like receptors were not associated with time or
             probability discounting of monetary rewards in healthy
             humans, and associations with physical effort discounting
             were inconsistent across adults of different ages.
             Meta-analytic results for temporal discounting corroborated
             our empirical finding for minimal effect of DA measures on
             discounting in healthy individuals but suggested that
             associations between individual differences in DA and reward
             discounting depend on clinical features. Addictions were
             characterized by negative correlations between DA and
             discounting, but other clinical conditions, such as
             Parkinson's disease, obesity, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity
             disorder, were characterized by positive correlations
             between DA and discounting. Together, the results suggest
             that trait differences in discounting in healthy adults do
             not appear to be strongly associated with individual
             differences in D2-like receptors. The difference in
             meta-analytic correlation effects between healthy controls
             and individuals with psychopathology suggests that
             individual difference findings related to DA and reward
             discounting in clinical samples may not be reliably
             generalized to healthy controls, and vice
             versa.<b>SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT</b> Decisions to forgo large
             rewards for smaller ones due to increasing time delays,
             uncertainty, or physical effort have been linked to
             differences in dopamine (DA) function, which is disrupted in
             some forms of psychopathology. It remains unclear whether
             alterations in DA function associated with psychopathology
             also extend to explaining associations between DA function
             and decision making in healthy individuals. We show that
             individual differences in DA D2 receptor availability are
             not consistently related to monetary discounting of time,
             probability, or physical effort in healthy individuals
             across a broad age range. By contrast, we suggest that
             psychopathology accounts for observed inconsistencies in the
             relationship between measures of DA function and reward
             discounting behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.1984-18.2018},
   Key = {fds339897}
}

@article{fds330812,
   Author = {Löckenhoff, CE and Rutt, JL and Samanez-Larkin, GR and O'Donoghue,
             T and Reyna, VF},
   Title = {Preferences for Temporal Sequences of Real Outcomes Differ
             Across Domains but do not Vary by Age.},
   Journal = {The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological
             sciences and social sciences},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {430-439},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx094},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>People's preferences for temporal
             sequences of events have implications for life-long health
             and well-being. Prior research suggests that other aspects
             of intertemporal choice vary by age, but evidence for age
             differences in sequence-preferences is limited and
             inconclusive. In response, the present research examined age
             differences in sequence-preferences for real outcomes
             administered in a controlled laboratory setting.<h4>Methods</h4>A
             pilot study examined sequence-preferences for aversive
             electrodermal shocks in 30 younger and 30 older adults. The
             main study examined sequence-preferences for electrodermal
             shocks, physical effort, and monetary gambles in an adult
             life-span sample (N = 120). It also examined emotional and
             physiological responses to sequences as well as underlying
             mechanisms including time perception and
             emotion-regulation.<h4>Results</h4>There were no significant
             age differences in sequence-preferences in either of the
             studies, and there were no age differences in responses to
             sequences in the main study. Instead, there was a domain
             effect with participants preferring decreasing sequences for
             shocks and mixed sequences for effort and
             money.<h4>Discussion</h4>After considering potential
             methodological limitations, theoretical contributions and
             implications for real-life decisions are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1093/geronb/gbx094},
   Key = {fds330812}
}

@article{fds339420,
   Author = {Smith, CT and Dang, LC and Burgess, LL and Perkins, SF and San Juan, MD and Smith, DK and Cowan, RL and Le, NT and Kessler, RM and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR and Zald, DH},
   Title = {Lack of consistent sex differences in D-amphetamine-induced
             dopamine release measured with [18F]fallypride
             PET.},
   Journal = {Psychopharmacology},
   Volume = {236},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {581-590},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-5083-5},
   Abstract = {<h4>Rationale</h4>Sex differences in the dopaminergic
             response to psychostimulants could have implications for
             drug abuse risk and other psychopathology involving the
             dopamine system, but human data are limited and
             mixed.<h4>Objectives</h4>Here, we sought to investigate sex
             differences in dopamine release after oral D-amphetamine
             administration.<h4>Methods</h4>We used [<sup>18</sup>F]fallypride
             positron emission tomography (PET) to measure the change in
             dopamine D2/3 receptor availability (%ΔBP<sub>ND</sub>, an
             index of dopamine release) between placebo and D-amphetamine
             sessions in two independent datasets containing a total of
             39 females (on either hormonal birth control n = 18,
             postmenopausal n = 10, or studied in the first 10 days
             of their menstrual cycle n = 11) and 37
             males.<h4>Results</h4>Using both a priori anatomical regions
             of interest based on previous findings and voxelwise
             analyses, we failed to consistently detect broad sex
             differences in D-amphetamine-induced dopamine release.
             Nevertheless, there was limited evidence for greater right
             ventral striatal dopamine release in young adult males
             relative to similarly aged females, but this was not
             consistently observed across samples. Plasma estradiol did
             not correlate with dopamine release and this measure did not
             differ in females on and off hormonal birth
             control.<h4>Conclusions</h4>While our finding in young
             adults from one dataset of greater %ΔBP<sub>ND</sub> in
             males is partially consistent with a previously published
             study on sex differences in D-amphetamine-induced dopamine
             release, our data do not support the presence of consistent
             widespread sex differences in this measure of dopamine
             release.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00213-018-5083-5},
   Key = {fds339420}
}

@article{fds333504,
   Author = {Holland, CAC and Ebner, NC and Lin, T and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Emotion identification across adulthood using the Dynamic
             FACES database of emotional expressions in younger, middle
             aged, and older adults.},
   Journal = {Cognition & emotion},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {245-257},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1445981},
   Abstract = {Facial stimuli are widely used in behavioural and brain
             science research to investigate emotional facial processing.
             However, some studies have demonstrated that dynamic
             expressions elicit stronger emotional responses compared to
             static images. To address the need for more ecologically
             valid and powerful facial emotional stimuli, we created
             Dynamic FACES, a database of morphed videos (n = 1026)
             from younger, middle-aged, and older adults displaying
             naturalistic emotional facial expressions (neutrality,
             sadness, disgust, fear, anger, happiness). To assess adult
             age differences in emotion identification of dynamic stimuli
             and to provide normative ratings for this modified set of
             stimuli, healthy adults (n = 1822, age range 18-86
             years) categorised for each video the emotional expression
             displayed, rated the expression distinctiveness, estimated
             the age of the face model, and rated the naturalness of the
             expression. We found few age differences in emotion
             identification when using dynamic stimuli. Only for angry
             faces did older adults show lower levels of identification
             accuracy than younger adults. Further, older adults
             outperformed middle-aged adults' in identification of
             sadness. The use of dynamic facial emotional stimuli has
             previously been limited, but Dynamic FACES provides a large
             database of high-resolution naturalistic, dynamic
             expressions across adulthood. Information on using Dynamic
             FACES for research purposes can be found at
             http://faces.mpib-berlin.mpg.de .},
   Doi = {10.1080/02699931.2018.1445981},
   Key = {fds333504}
}

@article{fds330813,
   Author = {Smith, CT and Crawford, JL and Dang, LC and Seaman, KL and San Juan, MD and Vijay, A and Katz, DT and Matuskey, D and Cowan, RL and Morris, ED and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Partial-volume correction increases estimated dopamine
             D2-like receptor binding potential and reduces adult age
             differences.},
   Journal = {Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official
             journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow
             and Metabolism},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {822-833},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0271678x17737693},
   Abstract = {The relatively modest spatial resolution of positron
             emission tomography (PET) increases the likelihood of
             partial volume effects such that binding potential
             (BP<sub>ND</sub>) may be underestimated. Given structural
             grey matter losses across adulthood, partial volume effects
             may be even more problematic in older age leading to
             overestimation of adult age differences. Here we examined
             the effects of partial volume correction (PVC) in two
             studies from different sites using different high-affinity
             D2-like radioligands (18 F-Fallypride, 11C-FLB457) and
             different PET camera resolutions (∼5 mm, 2.5 mm).
             Results across both data sets revealed that PVC increased
             estimated BP<sub>ND</sub> and reduced, though did not
             eliminate, age effects on BP<sub>ND</sub>. As expected, the
             effects of PVC were smaller in higher compared to lower
             resolution data. Analyses using uncorrected data that
             controlled for grey matter volume in each region of interest
             approximated PVC corrected data for some but not all
             regions. Overall, the findings suggest that PVC increases
             estimated BP<sub>ND</sub> in general and reduces adult age
             differences especially when using lower resolution cameras.
             The findings suggest that the past 30 years of research on
             dopamine receptor availability, for which very few studies
             use PVC, may overestimate effects of aging on dopamine
             receptor availability.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0271678x17737693},
   Key = {fds330813}
}

@article{fds342369,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Smith, CT and Juarez, EJ and Dang, LC and Castrellon, JJ and Burgess, LL and San Juan, MD and Kundzicz, PM and Cowan, RL and Zald,
             DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Differential regional decline in dopamine receptor
             availability across adulthood: Linear and nonlinear effects
             of age.},
   Journal = {Human brain mapping},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {3125-3138},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24585},
   Abstract = {Theories of adult brain development, based on
             neuropsychological test results and structural neuroimaging,
             suggest differential rates of age-related change in function
             across cortical and subcortical sub-regions. However, it
             remains unclear if these trends also extend to the aging
             dopamine system. Here we examined cross-sectional adult age
             differences in estimates of D2-like receptor binding
             potential across several cortical and subcortical brain
             regions using PET imaging and the radiotracer [<sup>18</sup>
             F]Fallypride in two samples of healthy human adults
             (combined N = 132). After accounting for regional
             differences in overall radioligand binding, estimated
             percent difference in receptor binding potential by decade
             (linear effects) were highest in most temporal and frontal
             cortical regions (~6-16% per decade), moderate in
             parahippocampal gyrus, pregenual frontal cortex, fusiform
             gyrus, caudate, putamen, thalamus, and amygdala (~3-5%), and
             weakest in subcallosal frontal cortex, ventral striatum,
             pallidum, and hippocampus (~0-2%). Some regions showed
             linear effects of age while many showed curvilinear effects
             such that binding potential declined from young adulthood to
             middle age and then was relatively stable until old age.
             Overall, these data indicate that the rate and pattern of
             decline in D2 receptor availability is regionally
             heterogeneous. However, the differences across regions were
             challenging to organize within existing theories of brain
             development and did not show the same pattern of regional
             change that has been observed in gray matter volume, white
             matter integrity, or cognitive performance. This variation
             suggests that existing theories of adult brain development
             may need to be modified to better account for the spatial
             dynamics of dopaminergic system aging.},
   Doi = {10.1002/hbm.24585},
   Key = {fds342369}
}

@article{fds343481,
   Author = {Karrer, TM and McLaughlin, CL and Guaglianone, CP and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Reduced serotonin receptors and transporters in normal aging
             adults: a meta-analysis of PET and SPECT imaging
             studies.},
   Journal = {Neurobiology of aging},
   Volume = {80},
   Pages = {1-10},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.021},
   Abstract = {Alterations in serotonin (5-HT) function have been
             hypothesized to underlie a range of physiological,
             emotional, and cognitive changes in older age. Here, we
             conducted a quantitative synthesis and comparison of the
             effects of age on 5-HT receptors and transporters from
             cross-sectional positron emission tomography and
             single-photon emission computed tomography imaging studies.
             Random-effects meta-analyses of 31 studies including 1087
             healthy adults yielded large negative effects of age in
             5-HT-2A receptors (largest in global cortex), moderate
             negative effects of age in 5-HT transporters (largest in
             thalamus), and small negative effects of age in 5-HT-1A
             receptors (largest in parietal cortex). Presynaptic 5-HT-1A
             autoreceptors in raphe/midbrain, however, were preserved
             across adulthood. Adult age differences were significantly
             larger in 5-HT-2A receptors compared with 5-HT-1A receptors.
             A meta-regression showed that 5-HT target, radionuclide, and
             publication year significantly moderated the age effects.
             The findings overall identify reduced serotonergic signal
             transmission in healthy aging. The evidence for the relative
             preservation of 5-HT-1A compared with 5-HT-2A receptors may
             partially explain psychological age differences, such as why
             older adults use more emotion-focused rather than
             problem-focused coping strategies.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.021},
   Key = {fds343481}
}

@article{fds346775,
   Author = {Juarez, EJ and Castrellon, JJ and Green, MA and Crawford, JL and Seaman,
             KL and Smith, CT and Dang, LC and Matuskey, D and Morris, ED and Cowan, RL and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Reproducibility of the correlative triad among aging,
             dopamine receptor availability, and cognition.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {921-932},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000403},
   Abstract = {The evidence that dopamine function mediates the association
             between aging and cognition is one of the most cited
             findings in the cognitive neuroscience of aging. However,
             few and relatively small studies have directly examined
             these associations. Here we examined correlations among
             adult age, dopamine D2-like receptor (D2R) availability, and
             cognition in two cross-sectional studies of healthy human
             adults. Participants completed a short cognitive test
             battery and, on a separate day, a PET scan with either the
             high-affinity D2R tracer [18F]Fallypride (Study 1) or
             [11C]FLB457 (Study 2). Digit span, a measure of short-term
             memory maintenance and working memory, was the only
             cognitive test for which dopamine D2R availability partially
             mediated the age effect on cognition. In Study 1, age was
             negatively correlated with digit span. Striatal D2R
             availability was positively correlated with digit span
             controlling for age. The age effect on digit span was
             smaller when controlling for striatal D2R availability.
             Although other cognitive measures used here have
             individually been associated with age and D2R availability
             in prior studies, we found no consistent evidence for
             significant associations between low D2R availability and
             low cognitive performance on these measures. These results
             at best only partially supported the correlative triad of
             age, dopamine D2R availability, and cognition. While a
             wealth of other research in human and nonhuman animals
             demonstrates that dopamine makes critical contributions to
             cognition, the present studies suggest caution in
             interpreting PET findings as evidence that dopamine D2R loss
             is a primary cause of broad age-related declines in fluid
             cognition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000403},
   Key = {fds346775}
}

@article{fds348032,
   Author = {Castrellon, JJ and Young, JS and Dang, LC and Cowan, RL and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Mesolimbic dopamine D2 receptors and neural representations
             of subjective value.},
   Journal = {Scientific reports},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {20229},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56858-1},
   Abstract = {The process by which the value of delayed rewards is
             discounted varies from person to person. It has been
             suggested that these individual differences in subjective
             valuation of delayed rewards are supported by mesolimbic
             dopamine D2-like receptors (D2Rs) in the ventral striatum.
             However, no study to date has documented an association
             between direct measures of dopamine receptors and neural
             representations of subjective value in humans. Here, we
             examined whether individual differences in D2R availability
             were related to neural subjective value signals during
             decision making. Human participants completed a monetary
             delay discounting task during an fMRI scan and on a separate
             visit completed a PET scan with the high affinity D2R tracer
             [18 F]fallypride. Region-of-interest analyses revealed
             that D2R availability in the ventral striatum was positively
             correlated with subjective value-related activity in the
             ventromedial prefrontal cortex and midbrain but not with
             choice behavior. Whole-brain analyses revealed a positive
             correlation between ventral striatum D2R availability and
             subjective value-related activity in the left inferior
             frontal gyrus and superior insula. These findings identify a
             link between a direct measure of mesolimbic dopamine
             function and subjective value representation in humans and
             suggest a mechanism by which individuals vary in neural
             representation of discounted subjective value.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41598-019-56858-1},
   Key = {fds348032}
}

@article{fds349002,
   Author = {Juarez, EJ and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Exercise, Dopamine, and Cognition in Older
             Age.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {986-988},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.10.006},
   Abstract = {Jonasson et al. investigated whether individual differences
             in human dopamine receptors (D2R) were related to cognitive
             performance before and after a 6-month aerobic exercise
             intervention (compared with active control). While D2R
             decreased (perhaps counterintuitively) with exercise, there
             was no relationship between D2R and working memory at
             baseline or following exercise.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2019.10.006},
   Key = {fds349002}
}

@article{fds356136,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Juarez, E and Troutman, A and Salerno, J and Samanez-Larkin, S and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Decision making across adulthood during physical
             distancing},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dr798},
   Abstract = {<p>Covid-19-related social-distancing measures have
             dramatically limited physical social contact between
             individuals and increased monetary and health concerns for
             individuals of all ages. We wondered how these new societal
             conditions would impact the choices individuals make about
             monetary, health, and social rewards, and if these
             unprecedented conditions would have a differential impact on
             older individuals. We conducted two racially diverse online
             studies to examine temporal discounting of monetary, health,
             and social rewards; stated preferences for monetary, health,
             and social rewards; and social distancing behaviors. We used
             the initial study (N = 233) to test our hypotheses and we
             ran the second, pre-registered study (N = 243) to determine
             if these relationships replicated. Both studies recruited
             equal numbers of White/Caucasian, Black/African American,
             and Hispanic/Latinx participants. In both studies, we found
             that older adults were more likely to prefer smaller, sooner
             social and health-related rewards in decision-making tasks,
             despite the fact that there were no age differences in the
             stated importance of social and health rewards and no age
             differences in self-reported social distancing behaviors.
             These data further support the assertion that older adults
             have increased motivation for social and health rewards
             compared to younger individuals and that motivation is
             important to consider when examining decision making across
             the adult life span.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/dr798},
   Key = {fds356136}
}

@article{fds356137,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Abiodun, S and Fenn, Z and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Mata,
             R},
   Title = {Temporal Discounting Across Adulthood: A Systematic Review
             and Meta-analysis},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7ysxa},
   Abstract = {<p>A number of developmental theories have been proposed
             that make differential predictions about the links between
             age and temporal discounting, or the devaluation of future
             rewards. Most empirical studies examining adult age
             differences in temporal discounting have relied on economic
             intertemporal choice tasks, which pit choosing a smaller,
             sooner monetary reward against choosing a larger, later one.
             Although initial studies using these tasks suggested older
             adults discount less than younger adults, follow-up studies
             provided heterogeneous, and thus inconclusive, results.
             Using an open science approach, we test the replicability of
             adult age differences in temporal discounting by conducting
             a preregistered systematic literature search and
             meta-analysis of adult age differences in intertemporal
             choice tasks. Across 37 cross-sectional studies (Total N =
             104,737), a planned meta-analysis found no sizeable relation
             between age and temporal discounting (r = -0.068, 95% CI
             [-0.170, 0.035]). We also found little evidence of
             publication bias or p-hacking. Exploratory analyses of
             moderators found no effect of research design (e.g.,
             extreme-group vs. continuous age), incentives (hypothetical
             vs. real rewards), duration of delay (e.g., days, weeks,
             months, or years), or quantification of discounting behavior
             (e.g., proportion of immediate choices vs. parameters from
             computational modeling). Additional analyses of 12
             participant-level data sets found little support for a
             nonlinear relation between age and temporal discounting
             across adulthood. Overall, the results suggest that younger,
             middle-aged, and older adults show similar preferences for
             smaller, sooner over larger, later rewards. We provide
             recommendations for future empirical work on temporal
             discounting across the adult life span.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/7ysxa},
   Key = {fds356137}
}

@article{fds356138,
   Author = {Samanez-Larkin, GR and Mottola, G and Heflin, D and Yu, L and Boyle,
             P},
   Title = {Overconfidence in financial knowledge associated with
             financial risk tolerance in older adults},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/p5gec},
   Abstract = {<p>Taking excessive financial risk in older age can have
             harmful, far-reaching consequences as opportunities to
             recover lost wealth are limited. Better understanding the
             mechanisms of financial risk taking in older age is
             critically important for both identifying vulnerabilities in
             certain older adults and for developing interventions to
             empower aging investors to make wise financial choices into
             the most advanced ages. The goals of the present study were
             to identify age differences in financial literacy,
             confidence in financial knowledge, and risk taking and how
             literacy and confidence were related to financial risk
             taking across older adults with and without cognitive
             impairment (ages 58–101). Using cross-sectional data from
             the Rush Memory and Aging Project, analyses revealed that
             risk aversion was higher and self-reported willingness to
             take financial risks was lower at older ages. Financial
             literacy was similar across the sixties and seventies but
             lower at the oldest ages. However, confidence in financial
             knowledge was not associated with age when controlling for
             financial literacy. In exploratory analyses, a measure of
             overconfidence in financial knowledge was positively
             associated with self-reported financial risk tolerance but
             not a behavioral measure of risk aversion. The
             overconfidence effect on risk tolerance did not vary across
             individuals with no cognitive impairment or Mild Cognitive
             Impairment (MCI). Overconfidence accounted for about 6% of
             the variance in financial risk tolerance. The present
             results suggest that overconfidence may contribute to risky
             financial behavior. Calibration of confidence levels to
             actual literacy is a potential target for future
             interventions aimed at protecting senior
             investors.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/p5gec},
   Key = {fds356138}
}

@article{fds356139,
   Author = {Castrellon, J and Meade, J and Greenwald, L and Hurst, K and Samanez-Larkin, G},
   Title = {Dopaminergic modulation of reward discounting in healthy
             rats: a systematic review and meta-analysis},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.03.024364},
   Abstract = {Although numerous studies have suggested that
             pharmacological alteration of the dopamine (DA) system
             modulates reward discounting, these studies have produced
             inconsistent findings. Here, we conducted a systematic
             review and pre-registered meta-analysis to evaluate DA
             drug-mediated effects on reward discounting of time,
             probability, and effort costs in studies of healthy rats.
             This produced a total of 1,343 articles to screen for
             inclusion/exclusion. From the literature, we identified 117
             effects from approximately 1,549 individual rats. Using
             random-effects with maximum-likelihood estimation, we
             meta-analyzed placebo-controlled drug effects for (1) DA
             D1-like receptor agonists and (2) antagonists, (3) D2-like
             agonists and (4) antagonists, and (5) DA
             transporter-modulating drugs. Meta-analytic effects showed
             that DAT-modulating drugs decreased reward discounting.
             While D1-like and D2-like antagonists both increased
             discounting, agonist drugs for those receptors had no
             significant effect on discounting behavior. A number of
             these effects appear contingent on study design features
             like cost type, rat strain, and microinfusion location.
             These findings suggest a nuanced relationship between DA and
             discounting behavior and urge caution when drawing
             generalizations about the effects of pharmacologically
             manipulating dopamine on reward-based decision
             making.},
   Doi = {10.1101/2020.04.03.024364},
   Key = {fds356139}
}

@article{fds357568,
   Author = {Abiodun, S and Salerno, J and McAllister, G and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Seaman, KL},
   Title = {Adult age differences in evoked emotional responses to
             dynamic facial expressions},
   Year = {2020},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/4u3kc},
   Abstract = {<p>Objective: Facial expressions are powerful social signals
             that motivate feelings and action in the observer. Research
             on face processing has overwhelmingly used static facial
             images, which are limited in their ecological validity.
             Previous research on the age-related positivity effect and
             age differences in social motivation suggest that older
             adults might experience different evoked emotional responses
             to facial expressions than younger adults. Here we were
             explored age-related differences in evoked responses to
             dynamic facial expressions across adulthood.
             <h4>Method:</h4> We used dynamic facial expressions which
             varied by expression type (happy, sad, angry) and expression
             level (low, medium, full) to gather participant ratings on
             their evoked emotional response to these stimuli along the
             dimensions of valence (positive vs. negative) and arousal.
             <h4>Results:</h4> As predicted, older adults rated the
             emotions evoked by positive facial expressions (happy) more
             positively than younger adults. Further, older adults rated
             the emotion evoked by negative facial expressions (angry and
             sad) more negatively than younger adults. Contrary to our
             predictions, older adults did not differ significantly in
             arousal to negative expressions compared to younger adults.
             Across all ages, individuals rated positive expressions as
             more arousing than negative expressions.
             <h4>Discussion:</h4> The findings provide some evidence that
             older adults may be more sensitive to variations in dynamic
             facial expressions than younger adults, particularly in
             terms of their estimates of valence. These dynamic facial
             stimuli that vary in magnitude are promising for future
             studies of more naturalistic affect elicitation, studies of
             social incentive processing, and use in incentive-driven
             choice tasks.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/4u3kc},
   Key = {fds357568}
}

@article{fds345724,
   Author = {Löckenhoff, CE and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Age Differences in Intertemporal Choice: The Role of Task
             Type, Outcome Characteristics, and Covariates.},
   Journal = {The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological
             sciences and social sciences},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {85-95},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz097},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>Prior research has revealed age
             differences in the preferred timing of monetary outcomes,
             but results are inconsistent across studies. The present
             study examined the role of task type, outcome
             characteristics, and a range of theoretically implicated
             covariates that may contribute to variations in age
             effects.<h4>Method</h4>Two types of intertemporal choice
             paradigms (temporal discounting and sequence construction)
             were administered to a diverse life-span sample (n = 287,
             aged 18-87). The design experimentally manipulated outcome
             delay (months vs years), amount (hundreds vs thousands), and
             valence (gain vs loss) while statistically controlling for a
             range of potential covariates including demographics,
             affect, personality, time perspective, subjective health,
             and numeracy.<h4>Results</h4>In the temporal discounting
             task, no significant age differences were observed and this
             pattern did not differ by outcome delay, amount, or valence.
             In the sequence-construction task, age was associated with a
             preference for sequences of decreasing impact in the gain
             condition but not in the loss condition, whereas outcome
             delay and amount did not moderate age effects. Age patterns
             in discounting and sequences preferences remained unchanged
             after controlling for covariates.<h4>Discussion</h4>These
             findings converge with prior studies reporting weak or null
             effects of age in temporal discounting tasks and suggest
             that inconsistent results are not due to variations in
             outcome valence, delay, or amount across studies. Findings
             also add to the scarce evidence for age differences
             sequence-preferences. After discussing methodological
             limitations, we consider implications for future research
             and practice.},
   Doi = {10.1093/geronb/gbz097},
   Key = {fds345724}
}

@article{fds338426,
   Author = {Löckenhoff, CE and Rutt, JL and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Gallagher, C and O'Donoghue, T and Reyna, VF},
   Title = {Age Effects in Sequence-Construction for a Continuous
             Cognitive Task: Similar Sequence-Trends but Fewer
             Switch-Points.},
   Journal = {The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological
             sciences and social sciences},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {762-771},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby090},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>Many real-life settings require decision
             makers to sort a predetermined set of outcomes or activities
             into a preferred sequence and people vary in whether they
             prefer to tackle the most challenging aspects first, leave
             them for the last, or intersperse them with less challenging
             outcomes. Prior research on age differences in
             sequence-preferences has focused on discrete and
             hypothetical events. The present study expands this work by
             examining sequence-preferences for a realistic, continuous,
             sustained, and cognitively challenging task.<h4>Methods</h4>Participants
             (N = 121, aged 21-86) were asked to complete 10 min of a
             difficult cognitive task (2-back), 10 min of an easy
             cognitive task (1-back), and 10 min of rest over the course
             of a 30-min interval. They could complete the tasks in any
             order and switch tasks as often as they wished and they were
             rewarded for correct performance. Additional measures
             included affective and physiological responses, task
             accuracy, time-perspective, and demographics.<h4>Results</h4>The
             majority of participants constructed sequences with
             decreasing task difficulty. Preferences for the general
             trend of the sequence were not significantly related to age,
             but the number of switches among the tasks decreased with
             age, and task-switching tended to incur greater accuracy
             decrements among older as compared to younger
             adults.<h4>Discussion</h4>We address potential
             methodological concerns, discuss theoretical implications,
             and consider potential real-life applications.},
   Doi = {10.1093/geronb/gby090},
   Key = {fds338426}
}

@article{fds349001,
   Author = {Burr, DA and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Advances in Emotion-Regulation Choice from Experience
             Sampling.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {344-346},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.008},
   Abstract = {Recent experience-sampling studies by Blanke et al. and
             Grommisch et al. provide insights into how individuals
             regulate their emotions in daily life. The rich datasets
             accessible from experience sampling allow researchers to
             detect nuances in the relationship between
             emotion-regulation choice and psychological health that may
             not be observed in traditional laboratory
             studies.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.008},
   Key = {fds349001}
}

@article{fds349874,
   Author = {Botvinik-Nezer, R and Holzmeister, F and Camerer, CF and Dreber, A and Huber, J and Johannesson, M and Kirchler, M and Iwanir, R and Mumford,
             JA and Adcock, RA and Avesani, P and Baczkowski, BM and Bajracharya, A and Bakst, L and Ball, S and Barilari, M and Bault, N and Beaton, D and Beitner, J and Benoit, RG and Berkers, RMWJ and Bhanji, JP and Biswal,
             BB and Bobadilla-Suarez, S and Bortolini, T and Bottenhorn, KL and Bowring, A and Braem, S and Brooks, HR and Brudner, EG and Calderon, CB and Camilleri, JA and Castrellon, JJ and Cecchetti, L and Cieslik, EC and Cole, ZJ and Collignon, O and Cox, RW and Cunningham, WA and Czoschke,
             S and Dadi, K and Davis, CP and Luca, AD and Delgado, MR and Demetriou, L and Dennison, JB and Di, X and Dickie, EW and Dobryakova, E and Donnat, CL and Dukart, J and Duncan, NW and Durnez, J and Eed, A and Eickhoff, SB and Erhart, A and Fontanesi, L and Fricke, GM and Fu, S and Galván, A and Gau,
             R and Genon, S and Glatard, T and Glerean, E and Goeman, JJ and Golowin,
             SAE and González-García, C and Gorgolewski, KJ and Grady, CL and Green, MA and Guassi Moreira and JF and Guest, O and Hakimi, S and Hamilton, JP and Hancock, R and Handjaras, G and Harry, BB and Hawco, C and Herholz, P and Herman, G and Heunis, S and Hoffstaedter, F and Hogeveen,
             J and Holmes, S and Hu, C-P and Huettel, SA and Hughes, ME and Iacovella,
             V and Iordan, AD and Isager, PM and Isik, AI and Jahn, A and Johnson, MR and Johnstone, T and Joseph, MJE and Juliano, AC and Kable, JW and Kassinopoulos, M and Koba, C and Kong, X-Z and Koscik, TR and Kucukboyaci, NE and Kuhl, BA and Kupek, S and Laird, AR and Lamm, C and Langner, R and Lauharatanahirun, N and Lee, H and Lee, S and Leemans, A and Leo, A and Lesage, E and Li, F and Li, MYC and Lim, PC and Lintz, EN and Liphardt, SW and Losecaat Vermeer and AB and Love, BC and Mack, ML and Malpica, N and Marins, T and Maumet, C and McDonald, K and McGuire, JT and Melero, H and Méndez Leal and AS and Meyer, B and Meyer, KN and Mihai, G and Mitsis, GD and Moll, J and Nielson, DM and Nilsonne, G and Notter, MP and Olivetti, E and Onicas, AI and Papale, P and Patil, KR and Peelle, JE and Pérez, A and Pischedda, D and Poline, J-B and Prystauka, Y and Ray, S and Reuter-Lorenz, PA and Reynolds, RC and Ricciardi, E and Rieck, JR and Rodriguez-Thompson, AM and Romyn, A and Salo, T and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR and Sanz-Morales, E and Schlichting, ML and Schultz, DH and Shen, Q and Sheridan, MA and Silvers, JA and Skagerlund, K and Smith, A and Smith,
             DV and Sokol-Hessner, P and Steinkamp, SR and Tashjian, SM and Thirion,
             B and Thorp, JN and Tinghög, G and Tisdall, L and Tompson, SH and Toro-Serey, C and Torre Tresols and JJ and Tozzi, L and Truong, V and Turella, L and van 't Veer, AE and Verguts, T and Vettel, JM and Vijayarajah, S and Vo, K and Wall, MB and Weeda, WD and Weis, S and White,
             DJ and Wisniewski, D and Xifra-Porxas, A and Yearling, EA and Yoon, S and Yuan, R and Yuen, KSL and Zhang, L and Zhang, X and Zosky, JE and Nichols,
             TE and Poldrack, RA and Schonberg, T},
   Title = {Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset
             by many teams.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {582},
   Number = {7810},
   Pages = {84-88},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9},
   Abstract = {Data analysis workflows in many scientific domains have
             become increasingly complex and flexible. Here we assess the
             effect of this flexibility on the results of functional
             magnetic resonance imaging by asking 70 independent teams
             to analyse the same dataset, testing the same 9 ex-ante
             hypotheses1. The flexibility of analytical approaches is
             exemplified by the fact that no two teams chose identical
             workflows to analyse the data. This flexibility resulted in
             sizeable variation in the results of hypothesis tests, even
             for teams whose statistical maps were highly correlated at
             intermediate stages of the analysis pipeline. Variation in
             reported results was related to several aspects of analysis
             methodology. Notably, a meta-analytical approach that
             aggregated information across teams yielded a significant
             consensus in activated regions. Furthermore, prediction
             markets of researchers in the field revealed an
             overestimation of the likelihood of significant findings,
             even by researchers with direct knowledge of the dataset2-5.
             Our findings show that analytical flexibility can have
             substantial effects on scientific conclusions, and identify
             factors that may be related to variability in the analysis
             of functional magnetic resonance imaging. The results
             emphasize the importance of validating and sharing complex
             analysis workflows, and demonstrate the need for performing
             and reporting multiple analyses of the same data. Potential
             approaches that could be used to mitigate issues related to
             analytical variability are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9},
   Key = {fds349874}
}

@article{fds356134,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Stanley, M and Hakimi, S and Cabeza, R and Adcock, A and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Imagining a Personalized Scenario Selectively Increases
             Perceived Risk of Viral Transmission for Older
             Adults},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6m5p4},
   Abstract = {<p>The COVID-19 pandemic has created a serious and prolonged
             public-health emergency. Older adults have been at
             significantly greater risk of hospitalization, ICU
             admission, and death due to COVID-19; as of February 2021,
             over 81% of COVID-19-related deaths in the U.S. occurred for
             people over the age of 65. Converging evidence from around
             the world suggests that age is the most significant risk
             factor for severe COVID-19 illness and for the experience of
             adverse health outcomes. Therefore, effectively
             communicating health-related risk information requires
             tailoring interventions to older adults’ needs. Using a
             novel informational intervention with a nationally-representative
             sample of 546 U.S. residents, we found that older adults
             reported increased perceived risk of COVID-19 transmission
             after imagining a personalized scenario with social
             consequences. Although older adults tended to forget
             numerical information over time, the personalized
             simulations elicited increases in perceived risk that
             persisted over a 1-3 week delay. Overall, our results bear
             broad implications for communicating information about
             health risks to older adults, and suggest new strategies to
             combat annual influenza outbreaks.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/6m5p4},
   Key = {fds356134}
}

@article{fds356135,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Hakimi, S and Stanley, M and Adcock, A and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Pairing Facts with Imagined Consequences Improves
             Pandemic-Related Risk Perception},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/53a9f},
   Abstract = {<p>The COVID-19 pandemic reached staggering new peaks during
             a global resurgence more than a year after the crisis began.
             Although public health guidelines initially helped to slow
             the spread of disease, widespread pandemic fatigue and
             prolonged harm to financial stability and mental wellbeing
             contributed to this resurgence. In the late stage of the
             pandemic, it became clear that new interventions were needed
             to support long-term behavior change. Here, we examined
             subjective perceived risk about COVID-19, and the
             relationship between perceived risk and engagement in risky
             behaviors. In Study 1 (N = 303), we found that subjective
             perceived risk was likely inaccurate, but predicted
             compliance with public health guidelines. In Study 2 (N =
             735), we developed a multi-faceted intervention designed to
             realign perceived risk with actual risk. Participants
             completed an episodic simulation task; we expected that
             imagining a COVID-related scenario would increase the
             salience of risk information and enhance behavior change.
             Immediately following the episodic simulation, participants
             completed a risk estimation task with individualized
             feedback about local risk levels. We found that information
             prediction error, a measure of surprise, drove beneficial
             change in perceived risk and willingness to engage in risky
             activities. Imagining a COVID-related scenario beforehand
             enhanced the effect of prediction error on learning.
             Importantly, our intervention produced lasting effects that
             persisted after a 1-3 week delay. Overall, we describe a
             fast and feasible online intervention that effectively
             changed beliefs and intentions about risky
             behaviors.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/53a9f},
   Key = {fds356135}
}

@article{fds357567,
   Author = {Green, M and Seaman, K and Crawford, J and Kuhnen, C and Samanez-Larkin,
             G},
   Title = {Multivariate associations between dopamine receptor
             availability and risky investment decision making across
             adulthood},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/2021.06.15.448537},
   Abstract = {Pharmacological manipulations have revealed that enhancing
             dopamine increases financial risk taking across adulthood.
             However, it is unclear whether baseline individual
             differences in dopamine function, assessed using PET
             imaging, are related to performance on risky financial
             decision making tasks. Here, thirty-five healthy adults
             completed an incentive-compatible learning-based risky
             investment decision task and a PET scan at rest using
             [11C]FLB457 to assess dopamine D2-like receptor
             availability. In the task, participants made choices between
             a safe asset (bond) and a risky asset (stock) with either an
             expected value less than the bond (“bad stock”) or
             expected value greater than the bond (“good stock”).
             Five measures of behavioral performance (choice
             inflexibility, risk seeking, suboptimal investment) and
             beliefs (absolute error, optimism) were extracted from the
             task data and average non-displaceable dopamine D2-like
             binding potential was extracted from four brain regions of
             interest (midbrain, amygdala, anterior cingulate, insula)
             from the PET imaging data. Given the presence of multiple
             independent and dependent variables, we used canonical
             correlation analysis (CCA) to evaluate multivariate
             associations between learning-based decision making and
             dopamine function controlling for age. Decomposition of the
             first dimension (r = .76) revealed that the strongest
             associations were between measures of choice inflexibility,
             incorrect choice, optimism, amygdala binding potential, and
             age. Follow-up univariate analyses revealed that amygdala
             binding potential and age were both independently associated
             with choice inflexibility. The findings reveal latent
             associations between baseline neural and behavioral measures
             suggesting that individual differences in dopamine function
             may be associated with learning-based financial risk taking
             in healthy adults.},
   Doi = {10.1101/2021.06.15.448537},
   Key = {fds357567}
}

@article{fds362165,
   Author = {Castrellon, J and Young, J and Dang, L and Smith, C and Cowan, R and Zald,
             D and Samanez-Larkin, G},
   Title = {Dopamine biases sensitivity to personal goals and social
             influence in self-control over everyday desires},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/2021.09.10.459829},
   Abstract = {People regularly give in to daily temptations in spite of
             conflict with personal goals. To test hypotheses about
             neuropharmacological influences on self-control, we used
             positron emission tomography to measure dopamine D2-like
             receptors (D2R) and experience sampling surveys to
             naturalistically track daily desires outside the laboratory
             in everyday life in a sample of 103 adults. Higher D2R
             availability in the ventral striatum was associated with
             increased sensitivity to personal goal conflict but not
             desire strength in deciding whether to attempt to resist a
             desire. The influence of D2Rs on sensitivity to personal
             goal conflict depended on whether desires were experienced
             in a social context. D2R availability in the midbrain (but
             not the ventral striatum) influenced whether desires were
             enacted. These findings provide unique evidence that the
             dopamine system influences decision making and regulatory
             behavior and provides new insights into how these mechanisms
             interact with personal goals and social contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1101/2021.09.10.459829},
   Key = {fds362165}
}

@article{fds353509,
   Author = {Castrellon, JJ and Meade, J and Greenwald, L and Hurst, K and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Dopaminergic modulation of reward discounting in healthy
             rats: a systematic review and meta-analysis.},
   Journal = {Psychopharmacology},
   Volume = {238},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {711-723},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-020-05723-5},
   Abstract = {<h4>Rationale</h4>Although numerous studies have suggested
             that pharmacological alteration of the dopamine (DA) system
             modulates reward discounting, these studies have produced
             inconsistent findings.<h4>Objectives</h4>Here, we conducted
             a systematic review and pre-registered meta-analysis to
             evaluate DA drug-mediated effects on reward discounting of
             time, probability, and effort costs in studies of healthy
             rats. This produced a total of 1343 articles to screen for
             inclusion/exclusion. From the literature, we identified 117
             effects from approximately 1549 individual
             rats.<h4>Methods</h4>Using random effects with
             maximum-likelihood estimation, we meta-analyzed
             placebo-controlled drug effects for (1) DA D1-like receptor
             agonists and (2) antagonists, (3) D2-like agonists and (4)
             antagonists, and (5) DA transporter-modulating
             drugs.<h4>Results</h4>Meta-analytic effects showed that
             DAT-modulating drugs decreased reward discounting. While
             D1-like and D2-like antagonists both increased discounting,
             agonist drugs for those receptors had no significant effect
             on discounting behavior. A number of these effects appear
             contingent on study design features like cost type, rat
             strain, and microinfusion location.<h4>Conclusions</h4>These
             findings suggest a nuanced relationship between DA and
             discounting behavior and urge caution when drawing
             generalizations about the effects of pharmacologically
             manipulating dopamine on reward-based decision-making.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00213-020-05723-5},
   Key = {fds353509}
}

@article{fds348794,
   Author = {Burr, DA and Castrellon, JJ and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Emotion dynamics across adulthood in everyday life: Older
             adults are more emotionally stable and better at regulating
             desires.},
   Journal = {Emotion (Washington, D.C.)},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {453-464},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000734},
   Abstract = {Older adults report experiencing improved emotional health,
             such as more intense positive affect and less intense
             negative affect. However, there are mixed findings on
             whether older adults are better at regulating emotion-a
             hallmark feature of emotional health-and most research is
             based on laboratory studies that may not capture how people
             regulate their emotions in everyday life. We used experience
             sampling to examine how multiple measures of emotional
             health, including mean affect, dynamic fluctuations between
             affective states and the ability to resist desires-a common
             form of emotion regulation-differ in daily life across
             adulthood. Participants (<i>N</i> = 122, ages 20-80)
             reported how they were feeling and responding to desire
             temptations for 10 days. Older adults experienced more
             intense positive affect, less intense negative affect, and
             were more emotionally stable, even after controlling for
             individual differences in global life satisfaction. Older
             adults were more successful at regulating desires, even
             though they experienced more intense desires than younger
             adults. In addition, adults in general experiencing more
             intense affect were less successful at resisting desires.
             These results demonstrate how emotional experience is
             related to more successful desire regulation in everyday
             life and provide unique evidence that emotional health and
             regulation improve with age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c)
             2021 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/emo0000734},
   Key = {fds348794}
}

@article{fds358693,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Stanley, ML and Hakimi, S and Cabeza, R and Adcock, RA and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Imagining a Personalized Scenario Selectively Increases
             Perceived Risk of Viral Transmission for Older
             Adults.},
   Journal = {Nat Aging},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {677-683},
   Publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media LLC},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s43587-021-00095-7},
   Abstract = {The COVID-19 pandemic has created a serious and prolonged
             public-health emergency. Older adults have been at
             substantially greater risk of hospitalization, ICU
             admission, and death due to COVID-19; as of February 2021,
             over 81% of COVID-19-related deaths in the U.S. occurred for
             people over the age of 651,2. Converging evidence from
             around the world suggests that age is the greatest risk
             factor for severe COVID-19 illness and for the experience of
             adverse health outcomes3,4. Therefore, effectively
             communicating health-related risk information requires
             tailoring interventions to older adults' needs5. Using a
             novel informational intervention with a nationally-representative
             sample of 546 U.S. residents, we found that older adults
             reported increased perceived risk of COVID-19 transmission
             after imagining a personalized scenario with social
             consequences. Although older adults tended to forget
             numerical information over time, the personalized
             simulations elicited increases in perceived risk that
             persisted over a 1-3 week delay. Overall, our results bear
             broad implications for communicating information about
             health risks to older adults, and they suggest new
             strategies to combat annual influenza outbreaks.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s43587-021-00095-7},
   Key = {fds358693}
}

@article{fds358299,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Hakimi, S and Stanley, ML and Adcock, RA and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Pairing facts with imagined consequences improves
             pandemic-related risk perception.},
   Journal = {Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A},
   Volume = {118},
   Number = {32},
   Pages = {e2100970118},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2100970118},
   Abstract = {The COVID-19 pandemic reached staggering new peaks during a
             global resurgence more than a year after the crisis began.
             Although public health guidelines initially helped to slow
             the spread of disease, widespread pandemic fatigue and
             prolonged harm to financial stability and mental well-being
             contributed to this resurgence. In the late stage of the
             pandemic, it became clear that new interventions were needed
             to support long-term behavior change. Here, we examined
             subjective perceived risk about COVID-19 and the
             relationship between perceived risk and engagement in risky
             behaviors. In study 1 (n = 303), we found that subjective
             perceived risk was likely inaccurate but predicted
             compliance with public health guidelines. In study 2 (n =
             735), we developed a multifaceted intervention designed to
             realign perceived risk with actual risk. Participants
             completed an episodic simulation task; we expected that
             imagining a COVID-related scenario would increase the
             salience of risk information and enhance behavior change.
             Immediately following the episodic simulation, participants
             completed a risk estimation task with individualized
             feedback about local viral prevalence. We found that
             information prediction error, a measure of surprise, drove
             beneficial change in perceived risk and willingness to
             engage in risky activities. Imagining a COVID-related
             scenario beforehand enhanced the effect of prediction error
             on learning. Importantly, our intervention produced lasting
             effects that persisted after a 1- to 3-wk delay. Overall, we
             describe a fast and feasible online intervention that
             effectively changed beliefs and intentions about risky
             behaviors.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.2100970118},
   Key = {fds358299}
}

@article{fds358111,
   Author = {Bazley, W and Korniotis, GM and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR},
   Title = {Can the Past Hinder Investor Learning?},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {November},
   Key = {fds358111}
}

@article{fds362164,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Taylor, M and Brandel-Tanis, FA and Davidson, A and Chande, A and Rishishwar, L and Andris, C and Adcock, A and Weitz, J and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Beckett, S},
   Title = {Communicating COVID-19 Exposure Risk with an Interactive
             Website Counteracts Risk Misestimation},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {e0290708},
   Booktitle = {PsyArXiv},
   Year = {2022},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/v8tdf},
   Abstract = {During the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals depended on risk
             information to make decisions about everyday behaviors and
             public policy. Here, we assessed whether an interactive
             website influenced individuals' risk tolerance to support
             public health goals. We collected data from 11,169 unique
             users who engaged with the online COVID-19 Event Risk Tool
             (https://covid19risk.biosci.gatech.edu/) between 9/22/21 and
             1/22/22. The website featured interactive elements,
             including a dynamic risk map, survey questions, and a risk
             quiz with accuracy feedback. After learning about the risk
             of COVID-19 exposure, participants reported being less
             willing to participate in events that could spread COVID-19,
             especially for high-risk large events. We also uncovered a
             bias in risk estimation: Participants tended to overestimate
             the risk of small events but underestimate the risk of large
             events. Importantly, even participants who voluntarily
             sought information about COVID risks tended to misestimate
             exposure risk, demonstrating the need for intervention.
             Participants from liberal-leaning counties were more likely
             to use the website tools and more responsive to feedback
             about risk misestimation, indicating that political
             partisanship influences how individuals seek and engage with
             COVID-19 information. Lastly, we explored temporal dynamics
             and found that user engagement and risk estimation
             fluctuated over the course of the Omicron variant outbreak.
             Overall, we report an effective large-scale method for
             communicating viral exposure risk; our findings are relevant
             to broader research on risk communication, epidemiological
             modeling, and risky decision-making.},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/v8tdf},
   Key = {fds362164}
}

@article{fds363999,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Taylor, M and Davidson, A and Weitz, J and Beckett, S and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Scenario-Based Messages on Social Media Motivate COVID-19
             Information Seeking},
   Booktitle = {PsyArXiv},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2022},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/en35p},
   Abstract = {Communicating information about health risks empowers
             individuals to make informed decisions. To identify
             effective communication strategies, we manipulated the
             specificity, self-relevance, and emotional framing of
             messages designed to motivate information seeking about
             COVID-19 exposure risk. In Study 1 (N = 221,829), we
             conducted a large-scale social media field study. Using
             Facebook advertisements, we targeted users by age and
             political attitudes. Episodic specificity drove engagement:
             Advertisements that contextualized risk in specific
             scenarios produced the highest click-through rates, across
             all demographic groups. In Study 2, we replicated and
             extended our findings in an online experiment (N = 4,233).
             Message specificity (but not self-relevance or emotional
             valence) drove interest in learning about COVID-19 risks.
             Across both studies, we found that older adults and liberals
             were more interested in learning about COVID-19 risks.
             However, message specificity increased engagement across
             demographic groups. Overall, evoking specific scenarios
             motivated information seeking about COVID-19, facilitating
             risk communication to a broad audience.},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/en35p},
   Key = {fds363999}
}

@article{fds365959,
   Author = {Taylor, M and Marsh, E and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Heuristic Decision Making Across Adulthood},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {508-518},
   Booktitle = {PsyArXiv},
   Year = {2022},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dp3x5},
   Abstract = {In general, research on aging and decision-making has grown
             in recent years. Yet, little work has investigated how
             reliance on classic heuristics may differ across adulthood.
             For example, younger adults rely on the availability of
             information from memory when judging the relative frequency
             of plane crashes versus car accidents, but it is unclear if
             older adults are similarly reliant on this heuristic. In the
             present study, participants aged 20-90 years old made
             judgments that could be answered by relying on five
             different heuristics: anchoring, availability, recognition,
             representativeness, and sunk-cost bias. We found no evidence
             of age-related differences in the use of the classic
             heuristics-younger and older adults employed anchoring,
             availability, recognition, and representativeness to equal
             degrees in order to make decisions. However, replicating
             past work, we found age-related differences in the sunk-cost
             bias-older adults were more likely to avoid this fallacy
             compared to younger adults. We explain these different
             patterns by drawing on the distinctive roles that stored
             knowledge and personal experience likely play across
             heuristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/dp3x5},
   Key = {fds365959}
}

@article{fds362096,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Abiodun, SJ and Fenn, Z and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Mata,
             R},
   Title = {Temporal discounting across adulthood: A systematic review
             and meta-analysis.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {111-124},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000634},
   Abstract = {A number of developmental theories have been proposed that
             make differential predictions about the links between age
             and temporal discounting, or the devaluation of future
             rewards. Most empirical studies examining adult age
             differences in temporal discounting have relied on economic
             intertemporal choice tasks, which pit choosing a smaller,
             sooner monetary reward against choosing a larger, later one.
             Although initial studies using these tasks suggested older
             adults discount less than younger adults, follow-up studies
             provided heterogeneous, and thus inconclusive, results.
             Using an open science approach, we test the replicability of
             adult age differences in temporal discounting by conducting
             a preregistered systematic literature search and
             meta-analysis of adult age differences in intertemporal
             choice tasks. Across 37 cross-sectional studies (Total N =
             104,737), a planned meta-analysis found no sizeable relation
             between age and temporal discounting, r = -0.068, 95% CI
             [-0.170, 0.035]. We also found little evidence of
             publication bias or p-hacking. Exploratory analyses of
             moderators found no effect of research design (e.g.,
             extreme-group vs. continuous age), incentives (hypothetical
             vs. real rewards), duration of delay (e.g., days, weeks,
             months, or years), or quantification of discounting behavior
             (e.g., proportion of immediate choices vs. parameters from
             computational modeling). Additional analyses of 12
             participant-level data sets found little support for a
             nonlinear relation between age and temporal discounting
             across adulthood. Overall, the results suggest that younger,
             middle-aged, and older adults show similar preferences for
             smaller, sooner over larger, later rewards. We provide
             recommendations for future empirical work on temporal
             discounting across the adult life span. (PsycInfo Database
             Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000634},
   Key = {fds362096}
}

@article{fds358298,
   Author = {Seaman, KL and Juarez, EJ and Troutman, A and Salerno, JM and Samanez-Larkin, SP and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Decision Making across Adulthood during Physical
             Distancing.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol
             Cogn},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {53-65},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2021.1962793},
   Abstract = {Covid-19-related social-distancing measures have
             dramatically limited physical social contact between
             individuals and increased monetary and health concerns for
             individuals of all ages. We wondered how these new societal
             conditions would impact the choices individuals make about
             monetary, health, and social rewards, and if these
             unprecedented conditions would have a differential impact on
             older individuals. We conducted two online studies to
             examine temporal discounting of monetary, health, and social
             rewards; stated preferences for monetary, health, and social
             rewards; and physical distancing behaviors. Both studies
             recruited equal numbers of White/Caucasian, Black/African
             American, and Hispanic/Latinx participants. We found that
             older adults were more likely to prefer smaller, sooner
             social and health-related rewards in decision-making tasks.
             These data further support the assertion that older adults
             have increased motivation for social and health rewards
             compared to younger individuals and that these age
             differences in motivation are important to consider when
             examining decision-making across the adult life
             span.},
   Doi = {10.1080/13825585.2021.1962793},
   Key = {fds358298}
}

@article{fds369179,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Taylor, MK and Weitz, JS and Beckett, SJ and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Reasons for Receiving or Not Receiving Bivalent COVID-19
             Booster Vaccinations Among Adults - United States, November
             1-December 10, 2022.},
   Journal = {MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report},
   Volume = {72},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {73-75},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm7203a5},
   Abstract = {Bivalent COVID-19 booster vaccines, developed to protect
             against both ancestral and Omicron BA.4/BA.5 variants, are
             recommended to increase protection against SARS-CoV-2
             infection and severe disease* (1,2). However, relatively few
             eligible U.S. adults have received a bivalent booster dose
             (3), and reasons for low coverage are unclear. An opt-in
             Internet survey of 1,200 COVID-19-vaccinated U.S. adults was
             conducted to assess reasons for receiving or not receiving a
             bivalent booster dose. Participants could select multiple
             reasons from a list of suggested reasons to report why they
             had or had not received a bivalent booster dose. The most
             common reasons cited for not receiving the bivalent booster
             dose were lack of awareness of eligibility for vaccination
             (23.2%) or of vaccine availability (19.3%), and perceived
             immunity against infection (18.9%). After viewing
             information about eligibility and availability, 67.8% of
             participants who had not received the bivalent booster dose
             indicated that they planned to do so; in a follow-up survey
             1 month later, 28.6% of these participants reported having
             received the dose. Among those who had planned to receive
             the booster dose but had not yet done so, 82.6% still
             intended to do so. Participants who had still not received
             the booster dose most commonly reported being too busy to
             get vaccinated (35.6%). To help increase bivalent booster
             dose coverage, health care and public health professionals
             should use evidence-based strategies to convey information
             about booster vaccination recommendations and waning
             immunity (4), while also working to increase convenient
             access.},
   Doi = {10.15585/mmwr.mm7203a5},
   Key = {fds369179}
}

@article{fds370892,
   Author = {Green, MA and Crawford, JL and Kuhnen, CM and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Seaman, KL},
   Title = {Multivariate associations between dopamine receptor
             availability and risky investment decision-making across
             adulthood.},
   Journal = {Cerebral cortex communications},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {tgad008},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press (OUP)},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgad008},
   Abstract = {Enhancing dopamine increases financial risk taking across
             adulthood but it is unclear whether baseline individual
             differences in dopamine function are related to risky
             financial decisions. Here, thirty-five healthy adults
             completed an incentive-compatible risky investment decision
             task and a PET scan at rest using [11C]FLB457 to assess
             dopamine D2-like receptor availability. Participants made
             choices between a safe asset (bond) and a risky asset
             (stock) with either an expected value less than the bond
             ("bad stock") or expected value greater than the bond ("good
             stock"). Five measures of behavior (choice inflexibility,
             risk seeking, suboptimal investment) and beliefs (absolute
             error, optimism) were computed and D2-like binding potential
             was extracted from four brain regions of interest (midbrain,
             amygdala, anterior cingulate, insula). We used canonical
             correlation analysis to evaluate multivariate associations
             between decision-making and dopamine function controlling
             for age. Decomposition of the first dimension
             (<i>r</i> = 0.76) revealed that the strongest
             associations were between measures of choice inflexibility,
             incorrect choice, optimism, amygdala binding potential, and
             age. Follow-up univariate analyses revealed that amygdala
             binding potential and age were both independently associated
             with choice inflexibility. The findings suggest that
             individual differences in dopamine function may be
             associated with financial risk taking in healthy
             adults.},
   Doi = {10.1093/texcom/tgad008},
   Key = {fds370892}
}

@article{fds374402,
   Author = {Castrellon, JJ and Zald, DH and Samanez-Larkin, GR and Seaman,
             KL},
   Title = {Adult age-related differences in susceptibility to social
             conformity pressures in self-control over daily
             desires.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000790},
   Abstract = {Developmental literature suggests that susceptibility to
             social conformity pressure peaks in adolescence and
             disappears with maturity into early adulthood. Predictions
             about these behaviors are less clear for middle-aged and
             older adults. On the one hand, while age-related increases
             in prioritization of socioemotional goals might predict
             greater susceptibility to social conformity pressures, aging
             is also associated with enhanced emotion regulation that
             could support resistance to conformity pressures. In this
             exploratory research study, we used mobile experience
             sampling surveys to naturalistically track how 157 healthy
             adults between the ages of 18 and 80 practice self-control
             over spontaneous desires in daily life. Many of these
             desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting
             that desire. Results showed that middle-aged and older
             adults were better at controlling their desires than younger
             adults when desires were experienced in the presence of
             others enacting that desire. Consistent with the literature
             on improved emotion regulation with age, these results
             provide evidence that the ability to resist social
             conformity pressure is enhanced across the adult life span.
             (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000790},
   Key = {fds374402}
}

@article{fds372974,
   Author = {Abiodun, SJ and Salerno, JM and McAllister, GA and Samanez-Larkin,
             GR and Seaman, KL},
   Title = {Adult Age Differences in Evoked Emotional Responses to
             Dynamic Facial Expressions.},
   Journal = {The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological
             sciences and social sciences},
   Volume = {79},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {gbad141},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbad141},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>Facial expressions are powerful social
             signals that motivate feelings and actions in the observer.
             Research on face processing has overwhelmingly used static
             facial images, which have limited ecological validity.
             Previous research on the age-related positivity effect and
             age differences in social motivation suggest that older
             adults might experience different evoked emotional responses
             to facial expressions than younger adults. Here, we
             introduce a new method to explore age-related differences in
             evoked responses to dynamic facial expressions across
             adulthood.<h4>Methods</h4>We used dynamic facial expressions
             which varied by expression type (happy, sad, and angry) and
             expression magnitude (low, medium, and full) to gather
             participant ratings on their evoked emotional response to
             these stimuli along the dimensions of valence (positive vs
             negative) and arousal.<h4>Results</h4>As predicted, older
             adults rated the emotions evoked by positive facial
             expressions (happy) more positively than younger adults.
             Furthermore, older adults rated the emotion evoked by
             negative facial expressions (angry and sad) more negatively
             than younger adults. Contrary to our predictions, older
             adults did not differ significantly in arousal to negative
             expressions compared with younger adults. Across all ages,
             individuals rated positive expressions as more arousing than
             negative expressions.<h4>Discussion</h4>The findings provide
             some evidence that older adults may be more sensitive to
             variations in dynamic facial expressions than younger
             adults, particularly in terms of their estimates of valence.
             These dynamic facial stimuli that vary in magnitude are
             promising for future studies of more naturalistic affect
             elicitation, studies of social incentive processing, and use
             in incentive-driven choice tasks.},
   Doi = {10.1093/geronb/gbad141},
   Key = {fds372974}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Postdocs * Reload * Login