Marketing Marketing
Fuqua School of Business
Duke University

 HOME > Fuqua > Marketing    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 
Webpage

Marketing : Publications since January 2023

List all publications in the database.    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:
%% Ariely, Dan   
@article{fds376744,
   Author = {Peer, E and Mazar, N and Feldman, Y and Ariely, D},
   Title = {How pledges reduce dishonesty: The role of involvement and
             identification},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {113},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104614},
   Abstract = {Authorities and managers often rely on individuals and
             businesses' self-reports and employ various forms of honesty
             declarations to ensure that those individuals and businesses
             do not over-claim payments, benefits, or other resources.
             While previous work has found that honesty pledges have the
             potential to decrease dishonesty, effects have been mixed.
             We argue that understanding and predicting when honesty
             pledges are effective has been obstructed due to variations
             in experimental designs and operationalizations of honesty
             pledges in previous research. Specifically, we focus on the
             role of whether and how an ex-ante honesty pledge asks
             individuals to identify (by ID, name, initials) and how much
             involvement the pledge requires from the individual (low:
             just reading vs. high: re-typing the text of the pledge). In
             four pre-registered online studies (N > 5000), we
             systematically examine these two dimensions of a pledge to
             find that involvement is often more effective than
             identification. In addition, low involvement pledges,
             without any identification, are mostly ineffective. Finally,
             we find that the effect of a high (vs. low) involvement
             pledge is relatively more persistent across tasks. Yet,
             repeating a low involvement pledge across tasks increases
             its effectiveness and compensates for the lower persistency
             across tasks. Taken together, these results contribute both
             to theory by comparing some of the mechanisms possibly
             underlying honesty pledges as well as to practice by
             providing guidance to managers and policymakers on how to
             effectively design pledges to prevent or reduce dishonesty
             in self-reports.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104614},
   Key = {fds376744}
}

@article{fds372454,
   Author = {Mitkidis, P and Perkovic, S and Nichols, A and Elbæk, CT and Gerlach,
             P and Ariely, D},
   Title = {Morality in minimally deceptive environments.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Applied},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {48-61},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xap0000476},
   Abstract = {Psychologists, economists, and philosophers have long argued
             that in environments where deception is normative, moral
             behavior is harmed. In this article, we show that
             individuals making decisions within minimally deceptive
             environments do not behave more dishonestly than in
             nondeceptive environments. We demonstrate the latter using
             an example of experimental deception within established
             institutions, such as laboratories and institutional review
             boards. We experimentally manipulated whether participants
             received information about their deception. Across three
             well-powered studies, we empirically demonstrate that
             minimally deceptive environments do not affect downstream
             dishonest behavior. Only when participants were in a
             minimally deceptive environment and aware of being observed,
             their dishonest behavior decreased. Our results show that
             the relationship between deception and dishonesty might be
             more complicated than previous interpretations have
             suggested and expand the understanding of how deception
             might affect (im)moral behavior. We discuss possible
             limitations and future directions as well as the applied
             nature of these findings. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024
             APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xap0000476},
   Key = {fds372454}
}

@article{fds374611,
   Author = {Landry, AP and Fincher, K and Barr, N and Brosowsky, NP and Protzko, J and Ariely, D and Seli, P},
   Title = {Harnessing dehumanization theory, modern media, and an
             intervention tournament to reduce support for retributive
             war crimes},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {111},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2023.104567},
   Abstract = {We demonstrate how psychological scientists can curate
             rich-yet-accessible media to intervene on
             conflict-escalating attitudes during the earliest stages of
             violent conflicts. Although wartime atrocities all-too-often
             ignite destructive cycles of tit-for-tat war crimes,
             powerful third parties can de-escalate the bloodshed.
             Therefore, following Russia's illegal invasion of Ukraine,
             we aimed to reduce Americans' support for committing
             retributive war crimes against Russian soldiers. To
             intervene during the earliest stages of the invasion, we
             drew on theories of dehumanization and “parasocial”
             intergroup contact to curate publicly available media
             expected to humanize Russian soldiers. We then identified
             the most effective materials by simultaneously evaluating
             all of them with an intervention tournament. This allowed us
             to quickly implement a psychological intervention that
             reliably reduced support for war crimes during the first
             days of a momentous land war. Our work provides a practical,
             result-driven model for developing psychological
             interventions with the potential to de-escalate incipient
             conflicts.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2023.104567},
   Key = {fds374611}
}

@article{fds376098,
   Author = {Diamond, JE and Kaltenbach, LA and Granger, BB and Fonarow, GC and Al-Khalidi, HR and Albert, NM and Butler, J and Allen, LA and Lanfear,
             DE and Thibodeau, JT and Granger, CB and Hernandez, AF and Ariely, D and DeVore, AD},
   Title = {Access to Mobile Health Interventions Among Patients
             Hospitalized With Heart Failure: Insights Into the Digital
             Divide From the CONNECT-HF mHealth Substudy.},
   Journal = {Circ Heart Fail},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {e011140},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIRCHEARTFAILURE.123.011140},
   Doi = {10.1161/CIRCHEARTFAILURE.123.011140},
   Key = {fds376098}
}

@article{fds373935,
   Author = {Nichols, AD and Axt, J and Gosnell, E and Ariely,
             D},
   Title = {A field study of the impacts of workplace diversity on the
             recruitment of minority group members.},
   Journal = {Nature human behaviour},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {2212-2227},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01731-5},
   Abstract = {Increasing workplace diversity is a common goal. Given
             research showing that minority applicants anticipate better
             treatment in diverse workplaces, we ran a field experiment
             (N = 1,585 applicants, N = 31,928 website visitors)
             exploring how subtle organizational diversity cues affected
             applicant behaviour. Potential applicants viewed a company
             with varying levels of racial/ethnic or gender diversity.
             There was little evidence that racial/ethnic or gender
             diversity impacted the demographic composition or quality of
             the applicant pool. However, fewer applications were
             submitted to organizations with one form of diversity (that
             is, racial/ethnic or gender diversity), and more
             applications were submitted to organizations with only white
             men employees or employees diverse in race/ethnicity and
             gender. Finally, exploratory analyses found that female
             applicants were rated as more qualified than male
             applicants. Presenting a more diverse workforce does not
             guarantee more minority applicants, and organizations
             seeking to recruit minority applicants may need stronger
             displays of commitments to diversity.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41562-023-01731-5},
   Key = {fds373935}
}

@article{fds362209,
   Author = {Bartmann, N and Rayburn-Reeves, R and Lindemans, J and Ariely,
             D},
   Title = {Does Real Age Feedback Really Motivate Us to Change our
             Lifestyle? Results from an Online Experiment.},
   Journal = {Health communication},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1744-1753},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2022.2030078},
   Abstract = {We set out to research the causal impact of Real Age
             feedback, a popular tool on health and lifestyle platforms,
             on health behaviors. We ran an online experiment where
             participants were randomly assigned a Real Age that differed
             in both direction (older or younger) and magnitude (much or
             slightly) from their passport age, or to a control condition
             where they received no Real Age feedback. We measured the
             impact of Real Age feedback on motivation to begin a
             healthier lifestyle, interest in taking a Real Age test, and
             percentage click-rate on an optional health link. We found
             that younger Real Age feedback was associated with higher
             interest. In addition, participants who received a slightly
             older Real Age were significantly less motivated to begin a
             healthier lifestyle compared to not only those who received
             a much younger or much older Real Age, but also to those in
             the control condition, suggesting a backfire effect. This
             effect remained even after accounting for participant
             health, demographics, and other psychological correlates to
             motivation. Real Age tests may backfire and demotivate
             people, and the positive effects they may have on
             psychological states may not outweigh the negative effects.
             Though promising, we caution using Real Age tests in their
             current form as stand-alone interventions to get people
             motivated.},
   Doi = {10.1080/10410236.2022.2030078},
   Key = {fds362209}
}

@article{fds373690,
   Author = {Elman, I and Ariely, D and Tsoy-Podosenin, M and Verbitskaya, E and Wahlgren, V and Wang, AL and Zvartau, E and Borsook, D and Krupitsky,
             E},
   Title = {Contextual processing and its alterations in patients with
             addictive disorders},
   Journal = {Addiction Neuroscience},
   Volume = {7},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addicn.2023.100100},
   Abstract = {Contextual processing is implicated in the pathophysiology
             of addictive disorders, but the nature of putative
             deficiencies remains unclear. We assessed some aspects of
             contextual processing across multimodal experimental
             procedures with detoxified subjects who were dependent on
             opioids (n = 18), alcohol- (n = 20), both opioids and
             alcohol (n = 22) and healthy controls (n = 24) using a)
             facial- and b) emotionally laden images; c) gambling task
             and d) sucrose solutions. Healthy subjects displayed
             consistent response pattern throughout all categories of the
             presented stimuli. As a group, dependent subjects rated
             (i.e., valuated) attractive and average faces respectively
             more and less attractive in comparison to controls.
             Dependent subjects' motivational effort, measured in the
             units of computer keypress to determine the attractive
             faces' viewing time, accorded the valuational context but
             was diminished relatively to the average faces’ valuation.
             Dependent subjects’ motivational effort for pleasant and
             aversive images respectively mirrored the attractive and
             average faces; their neutral images’ motivational effort
             was incongruent with the valuational context framed by the
             intermixed images. Also, dependent subjects’ emotional
             responses to counterfactual comparisons of gambling outcomes
             were unmatched by the riskiness context. Moreover, dependent
             subjects failed to show greater liking of sweet solutions
             that normally accompanies low sweetness perceptual context
             indicative of higher sucrose concentration needed for
             maximal hedonic experience. Consistent differences among the
             dependent groups (opioid vs. alcohol vs. comorbid) on the
             above procedures were not observed. The present findings
             suggest that opioid and/or alcohol dependence may be
             associated with amplified hedonic and motivational valuation
             of pleasant stimuli and with a disrupted link between
             behavioral/emotional responsivity and contextual variations.
             Further research is warranted to unravel the distinctive
             features of contextual processing in opioid- vis-à-vis
             alcohol addiction and how these features may interrelate in
             comorbid conditions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.addicn.2023.100100},
   Key = {fds373690}
}


%% Bettman, James R.   
@article{fds369095,
   Author = {Brick, DJ and Wight, KG and Bettman, JR and Chartrand, TL and Fitzsimons, GJ},
   Title = {Celebrate Good Times: How Celebrations Increase Perceived
             Social Support},
   Journal = {Journal of Public Policy and Marketing},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {115-132},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/07439156221145696},
   Abstract = {Despite the ubiquity of celebrations in everyday life,
             little is known about how celebrations may contribute to
             consumer well-being. In the current work, the authors
             propose that celebrations promote perceived social support,
             which prior work has conceptualized as the belief that
             others will be there for you for future negative life
             events. The authors further theorize that celebrations
             require three key characteristics that, in combination, are
             necessary for increasing perceived social support.
             Specifically, celebrations must (1) mark an individual's
             separate positive event and (2) involve consumption (3) with
             others (i.e., social). They test this theory across eight
             studies and demonstrate a process mechanism for this effect:
             these characteristics lead to increases in enacted support
             and perceived responsiveness, which in turn lead to
             increases in more general perceived social support. They
             then extend these findings by investigating virtually held
             celebrations, the individual's role at the celebration, and
             a downstream prosocial outcome. By doing so, this work
             highlights the broader benefits of celebrations beyond the
             focal individual and the immediate experience. Finally,
             specific policy implications and suggestions for enhancing
             consumer well-being are provided.},
   Doi = {10.1177/07439156221145696},
   Key = {fds369095}
}

@article{fds372840,
   Author = {Wiener, HJD and Bettman, JR and Luce, MF},
   Title = {Product-facilitated conversations: When does starting a
             conversation by mentioning a product lead to better
             conversational outcomes?},
   Journal = {Journal of Consumer Psychology},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1387},
   Abstract = {This paper examines product-facilitated conversations. In
             three studies, we show that the products consumers publicly
             display influence how other consumers start conversations
             with them and how enjoyable and self-disclosing these
             conversations are. Study 1 is an experiment in the field
             that shows that product-facilitated conversations are deeper
             and more enjoyable than non-product-facilitated ones. Study
             2 examines the characteristics of products that, when
             mentioned, lead to good conversations and identifies
             uniqueness and commonality as key characteristics. Study 3
             is an additional experiment in the field that tests these
             characteristics and shows that products with those
             characteristics are better conversation starters than the
             weather. Overall, these studies show novel social benefits
             to talking about products and generate new ideas about how
             talking about products can help consumers meet new people,
             smooth awkward social situations, and build
             relationships.},
   Doi = {10.1002/jcpy.1387},
   Key = {fds372840}
}


%% Chartrand, Tanya L.   
@article{fds369097,
   Author = {Brick, DJ and Wight, KG and Bettman, JR and Chartrand, TL and Fitzsimons, GJ},
   Title = {Celebrate Good Times: How Celebrations Increase Perceived
             Social Support},
   Journal = {Journal of Public Policy and Marketing},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {115-132},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/07439156221145696},
   Abstract = {Despite the ubiquity of celebrations in everyday life,
             little is known about how celebrations may contribute to
             consumer well-being. In the current work, the authors
             propose that celebrations promote perceived social support,
             which prior work has conceptualized as the belief that
             others will be there for you for future negative life
             events. The authors further theorize that celebrations
             require three key characteristics that, in combination, are
             necessary for increasing perceived social support.
             Specifically, celebrations must (1) mark an individual's
             separate positive event and (2) involve consumption (3) with
             others (i.e., social). They test this theory across eight
             studies and demonstrate a process mechanism for this effect:
             these characteristics lead to increases in enacted support
             and perceived responsiveness, which in turn lead to
             increases in more general perceived social support. They
             then extend these findings by investigating virtually held
             celebrations, the individual's role at the celebration, and
             a downstream prosocial outcome. By doing so, this work
             highlights the broader benefits of celebrations beyond the
             focal individual and the immediate experience. Finally,
             specific policy implications and suggestions for enhancing
             consumer well-being are provided.},
   Doi = {10.1177/07439156221145696},
   Key = {fds369097}
}


%% Fitzsimons, Gavan J.   
@article{fds374928,
   Author = {Abakoumkin, G and Tseliou, E and McCabe, KO and Lemay, EP and Stroebe,
             W and Agostini, M and Bélanger, JJ and Gützkow, B and Kreienkamp, J and Kutlaca, M and VanDellen, MR and Abdul Khaiyom and JH and Ahmedi, V and Akkas, H and Almenara, CA and Atta, M and Bagci, SC and Basel, S and Berisha Kida and E and Bernardo, ABI and Buttrick, NR and Chobthamkit,
             P and Choi, HS and Cristea, M and Csaba, S and Damnjanovic, K and Danyliuk,
             I and Di Santo and D and Douglas, KM and Enea, V and Faller, DG and Fitzsimons, G and Gheorghiu, A and Gómez, Á and Grzymala-Moszczynska, J and Hamaidia, A and Han, Q and Helmy, M and Hudiyana, J and Jeronimus, BF and Jiang, DY and Jovanović, V and Kamenov, Ž and Kende, A and Keng, SL and Kieu, TTT and Koc, Y and Kovyazina, K and Kozytska, I and Krause, J and Kruglanski, AW and Kurapov, A and Lantos, NA and Lesmana, CBJ and Louis, WR and Lueders, A and Malik, NI and Martinez, A and Mehulić, J and Milla, MN and Mohammed, I and Molinario, E and Moyano, M and Muhammad, H and Mula, S and Muluk, H and Myroniuk, S and Najafi, R and Nisa, CF and Nyúl, B and O’Keefe, PA and Olivas Osuna and JJ and Osin, EN and Park, J and Pica, G and Pierro, A and Rees, J and Reitsema, AM and Resta, E and Rullo, M and Ryan, MK and Samekin, A and Santtila, P and Sasin, E and Schumpe, BM and Selim, HA and Stanton, MV and Sultana, S and Sutton, RM and Utsugi, A and van Breen,
             JA and Van Lissa and CJ and Van Veen and K and Vázquez, A and Wollast, R and Yeung, VWL and Zand, S and Žeželj, IL and Zheng, B and Zick,
             A},
   Title = {Conceptual replication and extension of health behavior
             theories' predictions in the context of COVID-19: Evidence
             across countries and over time},
   Journal = {Social and Personality Psychology Compass},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12909},
   Abstract = {Virus mitigation behavior has been and still is a powerful
             means to fight the COVID-19 pandemic irrespective of the
             availability of pharmaceutical means (e.g., vaccines). We
             drew on health behavior theories to predict
             health-protective (coping-specific) responses and hope
             (coping non-specific response) from health-related
             cognitions (vulnerability, severity, self-assessed
             knowledge, efficacy). In an extension of this model, we
             proposed orientation to internal (problem-focused coping)
             and external (country capability) coping resources as
             antecedents of health protection and hope; health-related
             cognitions were assumed as mediators of this link. We tested
             these predictions in a large multi-national multi-wave study
             with a cross-sectional panel at T1 (Baseline, March-April
             2020; N = 57,631 in 113 countries) and a panel subsample
             at two later time points, T2 (November 2020; N = 3097) and
             T3 (April 2021; N = 2628). Multilevel models showed that
             health-related cognitions predicted health-protective
             responses and hope. Problem-focused coping was mainly linked
             to health-protective behaviors (T1-T3), whereas country
             capability was mainly linked to hope (T1-T3). These
             relationships were partially mediated by health-related
             cognitions. We conceptually replicated predictions of health
             behavior theories within a real health threat, further
             suggesting how different coping resources are associated
             with qualitatively distinct outcomes. Both patterns were
             consistent across countries and time.},
   Doi = {10.1111/spc3.12909},
   Key = {fds374928}
}

@article{fds376115,
   Author = {Wu, F and Samper, A and Morales, AC and Fitzsimons,
             GJ},
   Title = {When do photos on products hurt or help consumption? How
             magical thinking shapes consumer reactions to
             photo-integrated products},
   Journal = {Journal of Consumer Psychology},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1415},
   Abstract = {Consumers and companies frequently integrate products with
             lifelike photographs of people, animals, and other entities.
             However, consumer responses to such products are relatively
             unknown. Drawing on magical thinking and moral psychology,
             we propose that, due to a photograph's lifelike resemblance
             to its referent, consumers believe that photo-integrated
             products embody the depicted entity's underlying essence. As
             such, in cases where consumption compromises the product's
             integrity (e.g., food, disposable goods), people are less
             likely to consume photo-integrated products because doing so
             is perceived as destroying the depicted entity's essence,
             which elicits moral discomfort. In contrast, when the
             photographic image remains intact through consumption, as is
             the case with durable goods (e.g., magnets), people increase
             consumption of photo-integrated products relative to
             products without photo integration, consistent with their
             popularity in the marketplace. We highlight two strategies
             to promote more positive outcomes for managers and consumers
             alike: (1) choose images of entities whose essence
             destruction is perceived as less immoral, and (2) increase
             the durability of the product so the depicted entity's
             essence is preserved through consumption.},
   Doi = {10.1002/jcpy.1415},
   Key = {fds376115}
}

@article{fds370294,
   Author = {Westgate, EC and Buttrick, NR and Lin, Y and El Helou and G and Agostini,
             M and Bélanger, JJ and Gützkow, B and Kreienkamp, J and Abakoumkin, G and Abdul Khaiyom and JH and Ahmedi, V and Akkas, H and Almenara, CA and Atta,
             M and Bagci, SC and Basel, S and Berisha Kida and E and Bernardo, ABI and Chobthamkit, P and Choi, H-S and Cristea, M and Csaba, S and Damnjanovic, K and Danyliuk, I and Dash, A and Di Santo and D and Douglas,
             KM and Enea, V and Faller, DG and Fitzsimons, G and Gheorghiu, A and Gómez, Á and Hamaidia, A and Han, Q and Helmy, M and Hudiyana, J and Jeronimus, BF and Jiang, D-Y and Jovanović, V and Kamenov, Ž and Kende, A and Keng, S-L and Kieu, TTT and Koc, Y and Kovyazina, K and Kozytska, I and Krause, J and Kruglanski, AW and Kurapov, A and Kutlaca,
             M and Lantos, NA and Lemay, EP and Lesmana, CBJ and Louis, WR and Lueders,
             A and Maj, M and Malik, NI and Martinez, A and McCabe, KO and Mehulić, J and Milla, MN and Mohammed, I and Molinario, E and Moyano, M and Muhammad,
             H and Mula, S and Muluk, H and Myroniuk, S and Najafi, R and Nisa, CF and Nyúl, B and O'Keefe, PA and Olivas Osuna and JJ and Osin, EN and Park, J and Pica, G and Pierro, A and Rees, J and Reitsema, AM and Resta, E and Rullo,
             M and Ryan, MK and Samekin, A and Santtila, P and Sasin, E and Schumpe, BM and Selim, HA and Stanton, MV and Stroebe, W and Sutton, RM and Tseliou, E and Utsugi, A and van Breen, JA and Van Lissa and CJ and Van Veen and K and vanDellen, MR and Vázquez, A and Wollast, R and Et
             Al},
   Title = {Pandemic boredom: Little evidence that lockdown-related
             boredom affects risky public health behaviors across 116
             countries.},
   Journal = {Emotion (Washington, D.C.)},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {2370-2384},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0001118},
   Abstract = {Some public officials have expressed concern that policies
             mandating collective public health behaviors (e.g.,
             national/regional "lockdown") may result in behavioral
             fatigue that ultimately renders such policies ineffective.
             Boredom, specifically, has been singled out as one potential
             risk factor for noncompliance. We examined whether there was
             empirical evidence to support this concern during the
             COVID-19 pandemic in a large cross-national sample of 63,336
             community respondents from 116 countries. Although boredom
             was higher in countries with more COVID-19 cases and in
             countries that instituted more stringent lockdowns, such
             boredom did not predict longitudinal within-person decreases
             in social distancing behavior (or vice versa; <i>n</i> =
             8,031) in early spring and summer of 2020. Overall, we found
             little evidence that changes in boredom predict individual
             public health behaviors (handwashing, staying home,
             self-quarantining, and avoiding crowds) over time, or that
             such behaviors had any reliable longitudinal effects on
             boredom itself. In summary, contrary to concerns, we found
             little evidence that boredom posed a public health risk
             during lockdown and quarantine. (PsycInfo Database Record
             (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/emo0001118},
   Key = {fds370294}
}

@article{fds371674,
   Author = {Douglas, KM and Sutton, RM and Van Lissa and CJ and Stroebe, W and Kreienkamp, J and Agostini, M and Bélanger, JJ and Gützkow, B and Abakoumkin, G and Khaiyom, JHA and Ahmedi, V and Akkas, H and Almenara,
             CA and Atta, M and Bagci, SC and Basel, S and Berisha Kida and E and Bernardo,
             ABI and Buttrick, NR and Chobthamkit, P and Choi, HS and Cristea, M and Csaba, S and Damnjanovic, K and Danyliuk, I and Dash, A and Di Santo and D and Enea, V and Faller, DG and Fitzsimons, G and Gheorghiu, A and Gómez,
             Á and Hamaidia, A and Han, Q and Helmy, M and Hudiyana, J and Jeronimus,
             BF and Yu Jiang and D and Jovanović, V and Kamenov, Ž and Kende, A and Keng,
             SL and Kieu, TTT and Koc, Y and Kovyazina, K and Kozytska, I and Krause, J and Kruglanski, AW and Kurapov, A and Kutlaca, M and Lantos, NA and Lemay,
             EP and Lesmana, CBJ and Louis, WR and Lueders, A and Malik, NI and Martinez, A and McCabe, KO and Mehulić, J and Milla, MN and Mohammed,
             I and Molinario, E and Moyano, M and Muhammad, H and Mula, S and Muluk, H and Myroniuk, S and Najafi, R and Nisa, CF and Nyúl, B and O'Keefe, PA and Olivas Osuna and JJ and Osin, EN and Park, J and Pica, G and Pierro, A and Rees, J and Reitsema, AM and Resta, E and Rullo, M and Ryan, MK and Samekin, A and Santtila, P and Sasin, E and Schumpe, BM and Selim, HA and Stanton, MV and Sultana, S and Tseliou, E and Utsugi, A and van Breen,
             JA and Van Veen and K and vanDellen, MR and Vázquez, A and Wollast, R and Yeung, VWL and Zand, S and Žeželj, IL and Zheng, B and Zick,
             A},
   Title = {Identifying important individual- and country-level
             predictors of conspiracy theorizing: A machine learning
             analysis},
   Journal = {European Journal of Social Psychology},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1191-1203},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2968},
   Abstract = {Psychological research on the predictors of conspiracy
             theorizing—explaining important social and political
             events or circumstances as secret plots by malevolent
             groups—has flourished in recent years. However, research
             has typically examined only a small number of predictors in
             one, or a small number of, national contexts. Such
             approaches make it difficult to examine the relative
             importance of predictors, and risk overlooking some
             potentially relevant variables altogether. To overcome this
             limitation, the present study used machine learning to
             rank-order the importance of 115 individual- and
             country-level variables in predicting conspiracy theorizing.
             Data were collected from 56,072 respondents across 28
             countries during the early weeks of the COVID-19 pandemic.
             Echoing previous findings, important predictors at the
             individual level included societal discontent, paranoia, and
             personal struggle. Contrary to prior research, important
             country-level predictors included indicators of political
             stability and effective government COVID response, which
             suggests that conspiracy theorizing may thrive in relatively
             well-functioning democracies.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ejsp.2968},
   Key = {fds371674}
}

@article{fds370648,
   Author = {Dias, RS and Spiller, SA and Fitzsimons, GJ},
   Title = {Understanding effect sizes in consumer psychology},
   Journal = {Marketing Letters},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {367-374},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11002-023-09680-9},
   Abstract = {Over the past decade, behavioral scientists have learned
             that many findings in the field may not replicate, leading
             to calls for change in how behavioral research is conducted.
             Krefeld-Schwalb and Scheibehenne (2023) examine changes in
             the methodological practices in consumer research between
             2008 and 2020. They find that sample sizes have increased
             and that effect sizes have decreased. In this article, we
             take these findings as a starting point and reflect on how
             we can further improve methodological practices in the
             field. We argue that in order to build a more replicable,
             rigorous field, we must place effect sizes at the center of
             scientific reasoning. Specifically, we make four claims
             about effect sizes that we hope will help consumer
             researchers plan, conduct, and interpret their research: (1)
             effect sizes in consumer psychology are small, and that is a
             natural consequence of the field’s maturity; (2) effect
             sizes need to be contextualized; (3) our samples are still
             too small to detect the small effects of modern empirical
             consumer research; and (4) larger samples do not inherently
             generate smaller effects. It is our hope that the current
             article increases the field’s understanding about effect
             sizes and motivates researchers to place effect sizes at the
             center of their scientific reasoning. By thinking carefully
             about effect sizes, we believe we can collectively improve
             methodological practices and confidence in the findings of
             consumer psychology.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11002-023-09680-9},
   Key = {fds370648}
}

@article{fds362118,
   Author = {Enea, V and Eisenbeck, N and Carreno, DF and Douglas, KM and Sutton, RM and Agostini, M and Bélanger, JJ and Gützkow, B and Kreienkamp, J and Abakoumkin, G and Abdul Khaiyom and JH and Ahmedi, V and Akkas, H and Almenara, CA and Atta, M and Bagci, SC and Basel, S and Berisha Kida and E and Bernardo, ABI and Buttrick, NR and Chobthamkit, P and Choi, H-S and Cristea, M and Csaba, S and Damnjanovic, K and Danyliuk, I and Dash, A and Di Santo and D and Faller, DG and Fitzsimons, G and Gheorghiu, A and Gómez, Á and Grzymala-Moszczynska, J and Hamaidia, A and Han, Q and Helmy, M and Hudiyana, J and Jeronimus, BF and Jiang, D-Y and Jovanović, V and Kamenov, Ž and Kende, A and Keng, S-L and Kieu, TTT and Koc, Y and Kovyazina, K and Kozytska, I and Krause, J and Kruglanski,
             AW and Kurapov, A and Kutlaca, M and Lantos, NA and Lemay, EP and Lesmana,
             CBJ and Louis, WR and Lueders, A and Malik, NI and Martinez, A and McCabe,
             KO and Mehulić, J and Milla, MN and Mohammed, I and Molinario, E and Moyano, M and Muhammad, H and Mula, S and Muluk, H and Myroniuk, S and Najafi, R and Nisa, CF and Nyúl, B and O'Keefe, PA and Osuna, JJO and Osin, EN and Park, J and Pica, G and Pierro, A and Rees, J and Reitsema,
             AM and Resta, E and Rullo, M and Ryan, MK and Samekin, A and Santtila, P and Sasin, E and Schumpe, BM and Selim, HA and Stanton, MV and Sultana, S and Tseliou, E and Utsugi, A and van Breen, JA and Van Lissa and CJ and Van
             Veen, K and vanDellen, MR and Vázquez, A and Wollast, R and Yeung,
             VW-L and Zand, S and Žeželj, IL and Zheng, B and Zick, A and Zúñiga, C and Leander, NP},
   Title = {Intentions to be Vaccinated Against COVID-19: The Role of
             Prosociality and Conspiracy Beliefs across 20
             Countries.},
   Journal = {Health communication},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1530-1539},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2021.2018179},
   Abstract = {Understanding the determinants of COVID-19 vaccine uptake is
             important to inform policy decisions and plan vaccination
             campaigns. The aims of this research were to: (1) explore
             the individual- and country-level determinants of intentions
             to be vaccinated against SARS-CoV-2, and (2) examine
             worldwide variation in vaccination intentions. This
             cross-sectional online survey was conducted during the first
             wave of the pandemic, involving 6697 respondents across 20
             countries. Results showed that 72.9% of participants
             reported positive intentions to be vaccinated against
             COVID-19, whereas 16.8% were undecided, and 10.3% reported
             they would not be vaccinated. At the individual level,
             prosociality was a significant positive predictor of
             vaccination intentions, whereas generic beliefs in
             conspiracy theories and religiosity were negative
             predictors. Country-level determinants, including cultural
             dimensions of individualism/collectivism and power distance,
             were not significant predictors of vaccination intentions.
             Altogether, this study identifies individual-level
             predictors that are common across multiple countries,
             provides further evidence on the importance of combating
             conspiracy theories, involving religious institutions in
             vaccination campaigns, and stimulating prosocial motives to
             encourage vaccine uptake.},
   Doi = {10.1080/10410236.2021.2018179},
   Key = {fds362118}
}

@article{fds364191,
   Author = {Brick, DJ and Wight, KG and Fitzsimons, GJ},
   Title = {Secret consumer behaviors in close relationships},
   Journal = {Journal of Consumer Psychology},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {403-411},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1315},
   Abstract = {Although close relationships are often characterized by
             openness and disclosure, in the present research, we propose
             that there are times when individuals choose not to tell
             close others about their consumer behavior, keeping it a
             secret. For example, one partner may eat a candy bar on the
             way home from work, hide a package that was delivered to the
             house, or hire a cleaning service and not tell the other
             partner. We theorize that this type of consumer behavior is
             both common and mundane. That is, the consumption itself is
             minor—and has likely been done with the partner's
             knowledge in the past—but is being intentionally kept from
             the partner. We further investigate whether such behavior
             has downstream effects on the relationship, despite its
             mundaneness. Five studies support our conceptualization of
             secret consumer behaviors in close relationships and
             illustrate one consequence: guilt from secret consumption
             leads to greater relationship investment. This research
             explores a common, yet understudied, area of consumer
             behavior and highlights areas for future research. Thus, we
             contribute to the literature by being the first work to
             examine emotional, behavioral, and relational aspects of
             secret consumer behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1002/jcpy.1315},
   Key = {fds364191}
}

@article{fds369098,
   Author = {Brick, DJ and Wight, KG and Bettman, JR and Chartrand, TL and Fitzsimons, GJ},
   Title = {Celebrate Good Times: How Celebrations Increase Perceived
             Social Support},
   Journal = {Journal of Public Policy and Marketing},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {115-132},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/07439156221145696},
   Abstract = {Despite the ubiquity of celebrations in everyday life,
             little is known about how celebrations may contribute to
             consumer well-being. In the current work, the authors
             propose that celebrations promote perceived social support,
             which prior work has conceptualized as the belief that
             others will be there for you for future negative life
             events. The authors further theorize that celebrations
             require three key characteristics that, in combination, are
             necessary for increasing perceived social support.
             Specifically, celebrations must (1) mark an individual's
             separate positive event and (2) involve consumption (3) with
             others (i.e., social). They test this theory across eight
             studies and demonstrate a process mechanism for this effect:
             these characteristics lead to increases in enacted support
             and perceived responsiveness, which in turn lead to
             increases in more general perceived social support. They
             then extend these findings by investigating virtually held
             celebrations, the individual's role at the celebration, and
             a downstream prosocial outcome. By doing so, this work
             highlights the broader benefits of celebrations beyond the
             focal individual and the immediate experience. Finally,
             specific policy implications and suggestions for enhancing
             consumer well-being are provided.},
   Doi = {10.1177/07439156221145696},
   Key = {fds369098}
}

@article{fds363803,
   Author = {Howe, HS and Zhou, L and Dias, RS and Fitzsimons,
             GJ},
   Title = {Aha over Haha: Brands benefit more from being clever than
             from being funny},
   Journal = {Journal of Consumer Psychology},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {107-114},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1307},
   Abstract = {We examine whether the cleverness of a brand's humor attempt
             affects consumers' brand attitudes and engagement. A clever
             humor attempt is any humor attempt wherein the consumer
             feels she must make mental connections to solve the joke
             (e.g., understand a cultural reference, understand the dual
             meaning of a pun). Across five studies, we demonstrate that
             as the cleverness of a humor attempt increases, consumers
             report higher brand attitudes and are more engaged with the
             brand. This effect is mediated by perceptions of brand
             warmth and competence and moderated by consumers' need for
             cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1002/jcpy.1307},
   Key = {fds363803}
}

@article{fds371304,
   Author = {Han, Q and Zheng, B and Cristea, M and Agostini, M and Bélanger, JJ and Gützkow, B and Kreienkamp, J and PsyCorona Collaboration, and Leander, NP},
   Title = {Trust in government regarding COVID-19 and its associations
             with preventive health behaviour and prosocial behaviour
             during the pandemic: a cross-sectional and longitudinal
             study.},
   Journal = {Psychological medicine},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {149-159},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291721001306},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>The effective implementation of
             government policies and measures for controlling the
             coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic requires
             compliance from the public. This study aimed to examine
             cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of trust in
             government regarding COVID-19 control with the adoption of
             recommended health behaviours and prosocial behaviours, and
             potential determinants of trust in government during the
             pandemic.<h4>Methods</h4>This study analysed data from the
             PsyCorona Survey, an international project on COVID-19 that
             included 23 733 participants from 23 countries
             (representative in age and gender distributions by country)
             at baseline survey and 7785 participants who also completed
             follow-up surveys. Specification curve analysis was used to
             examine concurrent associations between trust in government
             and self-reported behaviours. We further used structural
             equation model to explore potential determinants of trust in
             government. Multilevel linear regressions were used to
             examine associations between baseline trust and longitudinal
             behavioural changes.<h4>Results</h4>Higher trust in
             government regarding COVID-19 control was significantly
             associated with higher adoption of health behaviours
             (handwashing, avoiding crowded space, self-quarantine) and
             prosocial behaviours in specification curve analyses (median
             standardised <i>β</i> = 0.173 and 0.229, <i>p</i> < 0.001).
             Government perceived as well organised, disseminating clear
             messages and knowledge on COVID-19, and perceived fairness
             were positively associated with trust in government
             (standardised <i>β</i> = 0.358, 0.230, 0.056, and 0.249,
             <i>p</i> < 0.01). Higher trust at baseline survey was
             significantly associated with lower rate of decline in
             health behaviours over time (<i>p</i> for interaction =
             0.001).<h4>Conclusions</h4>These results highlighted the
             importance of trust in government in the control of
             COVID-19.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0033291721001306},
   Key = {fds371304}
}

@article{fds372793,
   Author = {Wight, KG and Liu, PJ and Zhou, L and Fitzsimons,
             GJ},
   Title = {Sharing Food Can Backfire: When Healthy Choices for Children
             Lead Parents to Make Unhealthy Choices for
             Themselves},
   Journal = {Journal of Marketing Research},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222437231184830},
   Abstract = {Many consumers are caregivers and, as part of caregiving,
             frequently make food choices for their dependents. This
             research examines how food choices made for children
             influence the healthiness of parents’ subsequent
             self-choices. Whereas prior work focuses on choices for the
             self (others) as based on self-needs (other-needs), the
             authors theorize when and why self-choices involve
             consideration of other-needs. Five studies, including a
             nursery school field study, test the effect of choosing
             healthy food for a child on the healthiness of parents’
             self-choices, focusing on the role of anticipating
             potentially sharing self-choices with one's child. Potential
             sharing increased parents’ likelihood of making an
             unhealthy subsequent self-choice if they first made a
             healthy choice for their child. This effect was driven by
             parents’ present-focused parenting concerns about whether
             one's child would eat and enjoy healthy options chosen for
             them. This effect was mitigated when parents instead had
             future-focused parenting concerns. Additionally, this effect
             was mitigated after making an initial choice for the child
             that was (1) unhealthy or (2) healthy but relatively liked
             by the child. This research contributes to understanding how
             choices for others shape choices for the self and offers
             important marketing and policy implications.},
   Doi = {10.1177/00222437231184830},
   Key = {fds372793}
}


%% Ubel, Peter A   
@article{fds374446,
   Author = {Khouri, A and Stephens, MJ and Young, J and Galyean, P and Knettel, BA and Cherenack, EM and Zickmund, S and Watt, MH and Bartlett, J and Pollak,
             KI and Ubel, PA and Fagerlin, A and Suneja, G},
   Title = {Cancer Treatment Decision-Making for People Living With HIV:
             Physician-Reported Barriers, Facilitators, and
             Recommendations.},
   Journal = {J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr},
   Volume = {94},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {482-489},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/QAI.0000000000003300},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND: Compared with the general cancer population,
             people living with HIV (PLWH) and cancer are less likely to
             receive treatment and have significantly elevated
             cancer-specific mortality for many common cancer types.
             Physician recommendations drive the cancer therapy that
             patients receive, yet there is limited information assessing
             how cancer treatment decisions are made for people living
             with HIV and cancer. We sought to understand oncologist
             decision-making in PLWH and cancer by eliciting barriers,
             facilitators, and recommendations for enhancing care
             delivery. SETTING: Participants were recruited between May
             2019 and May 2021 from one academic medical center in the
             western United States (n = 13), another in the southeastern
             United States (n = 7), and community practices nationwide (n
             = 5). METHODS: Using an inductive qualitative approach, we
             conducted in-depth interviews with 25 oncologists from two
             academic medical centers and community practices. RESULTS:
             Facilitators of cancer care delivery included readily
             available information regarding HIV status and stage,
             interdepartmental communication, and antiviral therapy
             adherence. Barriers included a lack of formal education on
             HIV malignancies, perceptions of decreased life expectancy,
             fear of inadvertent disclosure, and drug-drug interactions.
             Recommendations included improved provider communication,
             patient social and mental health resources, and continuing
             education opportunities. CONCLUSION: The study revealed
             drivers of cancer treatment decision-making, highlighting
             physician-reported barriers and facilitators, and
             recommendations to support treatment decision-making. This
             is the first known study examining oncologists' perceptions
             of caring for PLWH. Given that cancer is a leading cause of
             death among PLWH, there is an urgent need to improve care
             and outcomes.},
   Doi = {10.1097/QAI.0000000000003300},
   Key = {fds374446}
}

@article{fds369309,
   Author = {Makara, A and Howe, H and Cooper, M and Heckert, K and Weiss, S and Kellom,
             K and Scharf, D and Ubel, P and Orloff, N and Timko,
             CA},
   Title = {Modifying an Open Science Online Grocery for parents of
             youth with anorexia nervosa: A proof-of-concept
             study.},
   Journal = {The International journal of eating disorders},
   Volume = {56},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1011-1020},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.23902},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objective</h4>For youth with anorexia nervosa (AN),
             remission requires high caloric goals to achieve weight
             restoration, consumption of a wide variety of calorically
             dense foods, and reintroduction of eliminated foods.
             Family-based treatment (FBT), the gold-standard treatment
             for youth with AN, empowers parents to renourish their child
             and restore them to health; yet, parents often report
             struggling with shifting meal planning and grocery shopping
             behaviors to focus on nutritional rehabilitation and weight
             restoration.<h4>Methods</h4>This proof-of-concept study
             aimed to modify a simulated grocery store (Open Science
             Online Grocery [OSOG]) for parents of youth with AN and
             explore the acceptability and feasibility of its use as part
             of standard care. Study staff collaborated with six parent
             research partners to modify the OSOG prior to piloting it
             with participants. Participants were 10 parents of youth
             undergoing a first-time hospitalization for medical
             stabilization of AN or atypical AN. Parents completed a
             battery of measures and a semistructured interview assessing
             the acceptability and feasibility of OSOG.<h4>Results</h4>Parents
             described the tool as credible and acceptable. Qualitative
             feedback highlighted common themes of caregiver burden,
             nutrition education, and acceptability of the
             tool.<h4>Discussion</h4>Results point to the need for more
             work in supporting parents in Phase I of FBT.<h4>Public
             significance</h4>Families are instrumental in supporting
             youth to recover from anorexia nervosa. During treatment,
             parents are charged with selecting and serving their
             adolescent's meals, often requiring them to change grocery
             shopping and food preparation habits to meet their child's
             high caloric needs. Parents reported feeling overwhelmed by
             this task and noted struggling with learning different
             approaches to nourish their adolescent during an already
             stressful time. Collaboratively with parents, we modified a
             tool to support parents in shifting thier shopping habits,
             which they reported as being a helpful springboard in the
             early phase of treatment.},
   Doi = {10.1002/eat.23902},
   Key = {fds369309}
}

@article{fds369928,
   Author = {Henry, V and Stephens, MJ and Galyean, P and Young, J and Zickmund, S and Knettel, BA and Bartlett, J and Watt, MH and Pollak, KI and Ubel, PA and Fagerlin, A and Suneja, G},
   Title = {Improving Cancer Care for People Living With HIV: A
             Qualitative Study of Provider Knowledge, Attitudes, and
             Practice.},
   Journal = {Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys},
   Volume = {116},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {60-67},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijrobp.2023.01.045},
   Abstract = {PURPOSE: Cancer is now the leading cause of non-AIDS death
             in the US population with HIV. People living with HIV (PLWH)
             are known to have lower cancer treatment rates and worse
             cancer outcomes. Disparate cancer treatment is driven by
             health system, patient, and clinician factors. Little
             attention has been given to the factors oncologists consider
             when making cancer treatment recommendations to PLWH. This
             study sought to examine oncologists' knowledge, attitudes,
             and practices that influence cancer treatment
             decision-making. METHODS AND MATERIALS: This study used
             qualitative methods to explore oncologists' treatment
             decision-making processes for PLWH and cancer. The sample
             included 25 radiation, medical, and surgical oncologists
             from 2 academic centers and 5 community practices. The
             interview domains were developed from the Andersen
             Healthcare Utilization Model, the Health Belief Model, and
             the PEN-3 Model, as well as our prior survey research.
             RESULTS: This study describes elements of cancer treatment
             decision-making for PLWH. Oncologists highlighted the need
             for formal HIV education to support cancer treatment. One
             main concern with patient-provider interactions pertained to
             maintaining patient confidentiality during clinical
             encounters. Lastly, the importance of multidisciplinary care
             among health care providers allowed oncologists to
             facilitate both cancer care and logistical support.
             CONCLUSIONS: As cancer becomes an increasingly common cause
             of death among PLWH, it is critical to understand the
             drivers of the observed disparities in cancer treatment. To
             our knowledge, this is the first qualitative study to
             describe oncologists' knowledge, attitudes, and practices
             toward patients who have a comorbid diagnosis of HIV and
             cancer. Several themes for future interventions emerge,
             including HIV training for cancer care providers, fostering
             interdisciplinary collaboration, enhancing HIV education for
             oncology learners and clinicians, and minimizing implicit
             bias.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.ijrobp.2023.01.045},
   Key = {fds369928}
}

@article{fds370177,
   Author = {Sloan, CE and Ubel, PA},
   Title = {Patients want to talk about their out-of-pocket costs-Can
             real-time benefit tools help?},
   Journal = {J Am Geriatr Soc},
   Volume = {71},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1365-1368},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jgs.18342},
   Doi = {10.1111/jgs.18342},
   Key = {fds370177}
}

@article{fds370334,
   Author = {Kaye, DR and Lee, H-J and Gordee, A and George, DJ and Ubel, PA and Scales,
             CD and Bundorf, MK},
   Title = {Medication Payments by Insurers and Patients for the
             Treatment of Metastatic Castrate-Resistant Prostate
             Cancer.},
   Journal = {JCO Oncol Pract},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {e600-e617},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1200/OP.22.00645},
   Abstract = {PURPOSE: The implications of high prices for cancer drugs on
             health care costs and patients' financial burdens are a
             growing concern. Patients with metastatic castrate-resistant
             prostate cancer (mCRPC) are often candidates for multiple
             first-line systemic therapies with similar impacts on life
             expectancy. However, little is known about the gross and
             out-of-pocket (OOP) payments associated with each of these
             drugs for patients with employer-sponsored health insurance.
             We therefore aimed to determine the gross and OOP payments
             of first-line drugs for mCRPC and how the payments vary
             across drugs. METHODS: This retrospective cohort study
             included 4,298 patients with prostate cancer who initiated
             therapy with one of six drugs approved for first-line
             treatment of mCRPC between July 1, 2013, and June 30, 2019.
             We compared gross and OOP payments during the 6 months after
             initiation of treatment for mCRPC using private payer claims
             data across patients using different first-line drugs.
             RESULTS: Gross payments varied across drugs. Over the 6
             months after the index prescription, mean unadjusted gross
             drug payments were highest for patients receiving
             sipuleucel-T ($115,525 USD) and lowest for patients using
             docetaxel ($12,804 USD). OOP payments were lower than gross
             drug payments; mean 6-month OOP payments were highest for
             cabazitaxel ($1,044 USD) and lowest for docetaxel ($296
             USD). There was a wide distribution of OOP payments within
             drug types. CONCLUSION: Drugs for mCRPC are expensive with
             large differences in payments by drug type. OOP payments
             among patients with employer-sponsored health insurance are
             much lower than gross drug payments, and they vary both
             across and within first-line drug types, with some patients
             making very high OOP payments. Although lowering drug prices
             would reduce pharmaceutical spending for patients with
             mCRPC, decreasing patient financial burden requires
             understanding an individual patient's benefit
             design.},
   Doi = {10.1200/OP.22.00645},
   Key = {fds370334}
}

@article{fds370353,
   Author = {Rao, BR and Akrobetu, DJ and Dickert, NW and Nguyen, T-V and Davis, JK and Campagna, A and Mitchell, AR and Sharma, A and Speight, CD and Barks,
             MC and Farley, S and Gutterman, S and Santanam, T and Ubel,
             PA},
   Title = {Deciding Whether to Take Sacubitril/Valsartan: How
             Cardiologists and Patients Discuss Out-of-Pocket
             Costs.},
   Journal = {Journal of the American Heart Association},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {e028278},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/jaha.122.028278},
   Abstract = {Background Out-of-pocket costs have significant implications
             for patients with heart failure and should ideally be
             incorporated into shared decision-making for clinical care.
             High out-of-pocket cost is one potential reason for the slow
             uptake of newer guideline-directed medical therapies for
             heart failure with reduced ejection fraction. This study
             aims to characterize patient-cardiologist discussions
             involving out-of-pocket costs associated with
             sacubitril/valsartan during the early postapproval period.
             Methods and Results We conducted content analysis on 222
             deidentified transcripts of audio-recorded outpatient
             encounters taking place between 2015 and 2018 in which
             cardiologists (n=16) and their patients discussed whether to
             initiate, continue, or discontinue sacubitril/valsartan. In
             the 222 included encounters, 100 (45%) contained discussions
             about cost. Cost was discussed in a variety of contexts:
             when sacubitril/valsartan was initiated, not initiated,
             continued, and discontinued. Of the 97 cost conversations
             analyzed, the majority involved isolated discussions about
             insurance coverage (64/97 encounters; 66%) and few addressed
             specific out-of-pocket costs or affordability (28/97
             encounters; 29%). Discussion of free samples of
             sacubitril/valsartan was common (52/97 encounters; 54%),
             often with no discussion of a longer-term plan for
             addressing cost. Conclusions Although cost conversations
             were somewhat common in patient-cardiologist encounters in
             which sacubitril/valsartan was discussed, these
             conversations were generally superficial, rarely addressing
             affordability or cost-value judgments. Cardiologists
             frequently provided patients with a course of free
             sacubitril/valsartan samples without a plan to address the
             cost after the samples ran out.},
   Doi = {10.1161/jaha.122.028278},
   Key = {fds370353}
}

@article{fds368032,
   Author = {Chettri, SR and Pignone, MP and Deal, AM and Sepucha, KR and Blizard,
             LB and Huh, R and Liu, Y-J and Ubel, PA and Lee, CN},
   Title = {Patient-Reported Outcomes of Breast Reconstruction: Does the
             Quality of Decisions Matter?},
   Journal = {Ann Surg Oncol},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {1891-1900},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1245/s10434-022-12785-6},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND: Little is known about how the quality of
             decisions influences patient-reported outcomes (PROs). We
             hypothesized that higher decision quality for breast
             reconstruction would be independently associated with better
             PROs. METHODS: We conducted a prospective cohort study of
             patients undergoing mastectomy with or without
             reconstruction. Patients were enrolled before surgery and
             followed for 18 months. We used BREAST-Q scales to measure
             PROs and linear regression models to explore the
             relationship between decision quality (based on knowledge
             and preference concordance) and PROs. Final models were
             adjusted for baseline BREAST-Q score, radiation,
             chemotherapy, and major complications. RESULTS: The cohort
             included 101 patients who completed baseline and 18-month
             surveys. Breast reconstruction was independently associated
             with higher satisfaction with breasts (β = 20.2,
             p = 0.0002), psychosocial well-being (β = 14.4,
             p = 0.006), and sexual well-being (β = 15.7,
             p = 0.007), but not physical well-being. Patients who made
             a high-quality decision had similar PROs as patients who did
             not. Among patients undergoing mastectomy with
             reconstruction, higher decision quality was associated with
             lower psychosocial well-being (β = -14.2, p = 0.01).
             CONCLUSIONS: Breast reconstruction was associated with
             better PROs in some but not all domains. Overall, making a
             high-quality decision was not associated with better PROs.
             However, patients who did not have reconstruction had a
             trend toward better well-being after making a high-quality
             decision, whereas patients who did have reconstruction had
             poorer well-being after making a high-quality decision.
             Additional research on the relationship between decision
             quality and PROs is needed.},
   Doi = {10.1245/s10434-022-12785-6},
   Key = {fds368032}
}

@article{fds369666,
   Author = {Lemmon, ME and Barks, MC and Bansal, S and Bernstein, S and Kaye, EC and Glass, HC and Ubel, PA and Brandon, D and Pollak,
             KI},
   Title = {The ALIGN Framework: A Parent-Informed Approach to
             Prognostic Communication for Infants With Neurologic
             Conditions.},
   Journal = {Neurology},
   Volume = {100},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {e800-e807},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000201600},
   Abstract = {BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Clinicians often communicate
             complex, uncertain, and distressing information about
             neurologic prognosis to parents of critically ill infants.
             Although communication tools have been developed in other
             disciplines and settings, none address the unique needs of
             the neonatal and pediatric neurology context. We aimed to
             develop a parent-informed framework to guide clinicians in
             communicating information about neurologic prognosis.
             METHODS: Parents of infants with neurologic conditions in
             the intensive care unit were enrolled in a longitudinal
             study of shared decision-making from 2018 to 2020. Parents
             completed semistructured interviews following recorded
             family meetings with the health care team, at hospital
             discharge, and 6 months after discharge. All interviews
             targeted information about parent preferences for prognostic
             disclosure. We analyzed the data using a conventional
             content analysis approach. Two study team members
             independently coded all interview transcripts, and
             discrepancies were resolved in consensus. We used NVIVO 12
             qualitative software to index and organize codes. RESULTS:
             Fifty-two parents of 37 infants completed 123 interviews.
             Parents were predominantly mothers (n = 37/52, 71%) with a
             median age of 31 (range 19-46) years. Half were Black (n =
             26/52, 50%), and a minority reported Hispanic ethnicity (n =
             2/52, 4%). Inductive analysis resulted in the emergence of 5
             phases of prognostic communication (Approach, Learn, Inform,
             Give support, and Next steps: ALIGN): (1) Approach: parents
             appreciated receiving consistent information about their
             child's neurologic outcome from clinicians who knew their
             child well. (2) Learn: parents valued when clinicians asked
             them how they preferred receiving information and what they
             already knew about their child's outcome prior to
             information delivery. (3) Inform: parents valued honest,
             thorough, and balanced information that disclosed prognostic
             uncertainty and acknowledged room for hope. (4) Give
             support: parents valued empathic communication and
             appreciated clinicians who offered real-time emotional
             support. (5) Next steps: parents appreciated clinicians who
             connected them to resources, including peer support.
             DISCUSSION: The ALIGN framework offers a novel,
             parent-informed strategy to effectively communicate
             neurologic prognosis. Although ALIGN represents key elements
             of a conversation about prognosis, each clinician can adapt
             this framework to their own approach. Future work will
             assess the effectiveness of this framework on communication
             quality and prognostic understanding.},
   Doi = {10.1212/WNL.0000000000201600},
   Key = {fds369666}
}

@article{fds371097,
   Author = {Mitchell, T and Abdelgadir, J and Oshotse, C and Ubel, PA and Williamson, T},
   Title = {Definitely, Maybe: Helping Patients Make Decisions about
             Surgery When Prognosis Is Uncertain.},
   Journal = {The Journal of clinical ethics},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {169-174},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/724770},
   Abstract = {AbstractThe sudden onset of severe traumatic brain injury
             (sTBI) is an event suffered by millions of individuals each
             year. Regardless of this frequency in occurrence, accurate
             prognostication remains difficult to achieve among
             physicians. There are many variables that affect this
             prognosis. Physicians are expected to assess the clinical
             indications of the brain injury while considering other
             factors such as patient quality of life, patient
             preferences, and environmental context. However, this lack
             of certainty in prognosis can ultimately affect treatment
             recommendations and prompt clinical ethical issues at the
             bedside, as it leaves room for physician bias and
             interpretation. In this article, we introduce data on
             neurosurgeon values that may shed light on the process
             physicians and patients involved in sTBI undergo. In doing
             so, we highlight the many nuances in decision-making for
             patients suffering from sTBI and discuss potential solutions
             to better patient-physician or surrogate-physician
             interactions.},
   Doi = {10.1086/724770},
   Key = {fds371097}
}


Duke University * Marketing * Faculty * Affiliated * Staff * Reload * Login