Evolutionary Anthropology Faculty Database
Evolutionary Anthropology
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > BAA > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 
Evaluations

Publications of Michael Tomasello    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Books   
@book{fds352651,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The gestural communication of apes and monkeys},
   Pages = {1-256},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9780805853650},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003064541},
   Abstract = {The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys is an
             intriguing compilation of naturalistic and experimental
             research conducted over the course of 20 years on gestural
             communication in primates, as well as a comparison to what
             is known about the vocal communication of nonhuman primates.
             The editors also make systematic comparisons to the gestural
             communication of prelinguistic and just-linguistic human
             children. An enlightening exploration unfolds into what may
             represent the starting point for the evolution of human
             communication and language. This especially significant read
             is organized into nine chapters that discuss: *the gestural
             repertoire of chimpanzees; *gestures in orangutans, subadult
             gorillas, and siamangs; *gestural communication in Barbary
             macaques; and *a comparison of the gestures of apes and
             monkeys. This book will appeal to psychologists,
             anthropologists, and linguists interested in the
             evolutionary origins of language and/or gestures, as well as
             to all primatologists. A CD insert offers video of gestures
             for each of the species.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781003064541},
   Key = {fds352651}
}

@book{fds359683,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Foreword},
   Volume = {27},
   Pages = {VII-IX},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9789027261007},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/tilar.27.for},
   Doi = {10.1075/tilar.27.for},
   Key = {fds359683}
}

@book{fds351574,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction: A cognitive-functional perspective on language
             structure},
   Volume = {1},
   Pages = {vii-xxiii},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780585115191},
   Key = {fds351574}
}

@book{fds351575,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional
             approaches to language structure},
   Volume = {1},
   Pages = {1-292},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780585115191},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315085678},
   Abstract = {This book, which gathers in one place the theories of 10
             leading cognitive and functional linguists, represents a new
             approach that may define the next era in the history of
             psychology: It promises to give psychologists a new
             appreciation of what this variety of linguistics can offer
             their study of language and communication. In addition, it
             provides cognitive-functional linguists new models for
             presenting their work to audiences outside the boundaries of
             traditional linguistics. Thus, it serves as an excellent
             text for courses in psycholinguistics, and appeal to
             students and researchers in cognitive science and functional
             linguistics.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315085678},
   Key = {fds351575}
}

@book{fds320791,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A Natural History of Human Morality},
   Pages = {180 pages},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780674088641},
   Abstract = {Michael Tomasello offers the most detailed account to date
             of the evolution of human moral psychology.},
   Key = {fds320791}
}

@book{fds321686,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A Natural History of Human Thinking},
   Pages = {1-192},
   Publisher = {Harvard University Press},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9780674724778},
   Abstract = {Tool-making or culture, language or religious belief: ever
             since Darwin, thinkers have struggled to identify what
             fundamentally differentiates human beings from other
             animals. In this much-anticipated book, Michael Tomasello
             weaves his twenty years of comparative studies of humans and
             great apes into a compelling argument that cooperative
             social interaction is the key to our cognitive uniqueness.
             Once our ancestors learned to put their heads together with
             others to pursue shared goals, humankind was on an
             evolutionary path all its own. Tomasello argues that our
             prehuman ancestors, like today’s great apes, were social
             beings who could solve problems by thinking. But they were
             almost entirely competitive, aiming only at their individual
             goals. As ecological changes forced them into more
             cooperative living arrangements, early humans had to
             coordinate their actions and communicate their thoughts with
             collaborative partners. Tomasello’s “shared
             intentionality hypothesis” captures how these more
             socially complex forms of life led to more conceptually
             complex forms of thinking. In order to survive, humans had
             to learn to see the world from multiple social perspectives,
             to draw socially recursive inferences, and to monitor their
             own thinking via the normative standards of the group. Even
             language and culture arose from the preexisting need to work
             together. What differentiates us most from other great apes,
             Tomasello proposes, are the new forms of thinking engendered
             by our new forms of collaborative and communicative
             interaction. A Natural History of Human Thinking is the most
             detailed scientific analysis to date of the connection
             between human sociality and cognition.},
   Key = {fds321686}
}

@book{fds351638,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction to the classic edition},
   Pages = {vii-xiii},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848725911},
   Key = {fds351638}
}

@book{fds351639,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction: A cognitive-–functional perspective on
             language structure},
   Pages = {xiv-xxix},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848725911},
   Key = {fds351639}
}

@book{fds351640,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional
             approaches to language structure, volume ii classic
             edition},
   Pages = {1-278},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848725935},
   Abstract = {From the point of view of psychology and cognitive science,
             much of modern linguistics is too formal and mathematical to
             be of much use. The New Psychology of Language volumes broke
             new ground by introducing functional and cognitive
             approaches to language structure in terms already familiar
             to psychologists, thus defining the next era in the
             scientific study of language. The Classic Edition volumes
             re-introduce some of the most important cognitive and
             functional linguists working in the field. They include a
             new introduction by Michael Tomasello in which he reviews
             what has changed since the volumes were first published and
             highlights the fundamental insights of the original authors.
             The New Psychology of Language volumes are a must-read for
             anyone interested in understanding how cognitive and
             functional linguistics has become the thriving perspective
             on the scientific study of language that it is
             today.},
   Key = {fds351640}
}

@book{fds351641,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction to the classic edition},
   Pages = {vii-xiii},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848725935},
   Key = {fds351641}
}

@book{fds351642,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional
             approaches to language structure, volume I classic
             edition},
   Pages = {1-268},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848725911},
   Abstract = {From the point of view of psychology and cognitive science,
             much of modern linguistics is too formal and mathematical to
             be of much use. The New Psychology of Language volumes broke
             new ground by introducing functional and cognitive
             approaches to language structure in terms already familiar
             to psychologists, thus defining the next era in the
             scientific study of language. The Classic Edition volumes
             re-introduce some of the most important cognitive and
             functional linguists working in the field. They include a
             new introduction by Michael Tomasello in which he reviews
             what has changed since the volumes first published and
             highlights the fundamental insights of the original authors.
             The New Psychology of Language volumes are a must-read for
             anyone interested in understanding how cognitive and
             functional linguistics has become the thriving perspective
             on the scientific study of language that it is
             today.},
   Key = {fds351642}
}

@book{fds351643,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction: Some surprises for psychologists},
   Pages = {1-14},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848725935},
   Key = {fds351643}
}

@book{fds351911,
   Author = {Slobin, DI and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction},
   Pages = {xv-xxiv},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9781410611192},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781410611192},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781410611192},
   Key = {fds351911}
}


%% Book Sections/Chapters   
@misc{fds371506,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Having Intentions, Understanding Intentions, and
             Understanding Communicative Intentions},
   Pages = {63-75},
   Booktitle = {Developing Theories of Intention: Social Understanding and
             Self-Control},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780805831412},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003417927-5},
   Abstract = {This chapter looks at a major cause and a major consequence
             of the 9-month social-cognitive revolution; and both of
             these also concern infant intentionality. It argues that
             young children’s understanding of other persons as
             intentional agents results in large part from newly emerging
             forms of intentionality in their own sensory-motor actions.
             The chapter explores young children’s understanding of a
             special type of intention that emerges directly on the heels
             of the 9-month revolution, namely, communicative intentions.
             Intentional agents have goals and make active choices among
             behavioral means for attaining those goals. Important,
             intentional agents also make active choices about what they
             pay attention to in pursuing those goals. ntentional agents
             have goals and make active choices among behavioral means
             for attaining those goals. Important, intentional agents
             also make active choices about what they pay attention to in
             pursuing those goals.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781003417927-5},
   Key = {fds371506}
}

@misc{fds368121,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {THE EARLY ONTOGENY OF HUMAN COOPERATION AND
             MORALITY},
   Pages = {200-216},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of Moral Development, Third Edition},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780367497569},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003047247-16},
   Abstract = {In this chapter, we consider children’s moral development
             from an evolutionary perspective. We propose that human
             morality arose evolutionarily as a set of skills and motives
             for cooperating with others. Following recent accounts by
             Tomasello and colleagues of a two-step sequence in the
             evolution of human cooperation and morality, we propose and
             review empirical evidence in support of a two-step sequence
             in the ontogeny of human cooperation and morality: first, a
             second-personal morality that emerges in infancy and
             toddlerhood, in which children are sympathetic or fair to
             particular others, and second, a norm-based morality that
             emerges during the preschool years, in which children follow
             and enforce group-wide social norms. These prosocial and
             moral tendencies compete throughout ontogeny with
             children’s selfish tendencies and are modified
             significantly by socialization and culture.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781003047247-16},
   Key = {fds368121}
}

@misc{fds358297,
   Author = {Mannle, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Fathers, siblings, and the bridge hypothesis},
   Pages = {23-41},
   Booktitle = {Children’s Language: Volume 6},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780898597608},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315792668-2},
   Abstract = {The underlying assumption of the Bridge Hypothesis-which is
             not shared by all developmental psycholinguists-is that
             communication pressure is beneficial to the child’s
             development of communicative competence, including the
             acquisition of linguistic skills. This chapter reviews the
             existing research on fathers’ and siblings’ linguistic
             interactions with young children, including some recent
             research from our own laboratory. In the traditional family
             situation, fathers spend significantly less time than
             mothers interacting with their children, and so presumably
             are less familiar with the everyday behavioral routines that
             are so important for early language acquisition. As with
             fathers, the research on siblings’ communicative
             interactions with young children has just begun. Most of the
             relevant research on sibling speech to infants is concerned
             with structural/linguistic features, and it is thus unclear
             in this case as well whether siblings do in fact place
             communication pressure on the language learning child and
             thereby act as a linguistic bridge.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315792668-2},
   Key = {fds358297}
}

@misc{fds353328,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Why don't apes point?},
   Pages = {506-524},
   Booktitle = {Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and
             Interaction},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9781845203948},
   Key = {fds353328}
}

@misc{fds352650,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Introduction: Intentional communication in nonhuman
             primates},
   Pages = {1-15},
   Booktitle = {The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9780805853650},
   Key = {fds352650}
}

@misc{fds352649,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Ape gestures and the origins of language},
   Pages = {221-239},
   Booktitle = {The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9780805853650},
   Key = {fds352649}
}

@misc{fds352648,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The gestural repertoire of chimpanzees (Pan
             troglodytes)},
   Pages = {17-39},
   Booktitle = {The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9780805853650},
   Key = {fds352648}
}

@misc{fds352652,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Comparing the gestures of apes and monkeys},
   Pages = {197-220},
   Booktitle = {The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9780805853650},
   Key = {fds352652}
}

@misc{fds351572,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Punishment},
   Pages = {214-221},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780128132517},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809633-8.01093-1},
   Abstract = {Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior
             of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no
             regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the
             other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be
             motivated by its effects on others. Altruistic punishment,
             third-party punishment, and norm enforcement are special
             cases of punishment that can maintain cooperation, and these
             may not have analogs in animals other than humans. More
             socially sophisticated forms of punishment will require more
             flexible and complex cognitive processes. Of particular
             interest are social (other-regarding) preferences, since
             these may have allowed the evolution of the large-scale
             non-kin cooperation seen only in humans. However, little is
             known about the cognitive mechanisms underlying punishment
             in other animals.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-809633-8.01093-1},
   Key = {fds351572}
}

@misc{fds372674,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Punishment},
   Volume = {1},
   Pages = {V1-214-V1-219},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, Second Edition: Volume
             1-5},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780128132517},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809633-8.01093-1},
   Abstract = {Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior
             of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no
             regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the
             other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be
             motivated by its effects on others. Altruistic punishment,
             third-party punishment, and norm enforcement are special
             cases of punishment that can maintain cooperation, and these
             may not have analogs in animals other than humans. More
             socially sophisticated forms of punishment will require more
             flexible and complex cognitive processes. Of particular
             interest are social (other-regarding) preferences, since
             these may have allowed the evolution of the large-scale
             non-kin cooperation seen only in humans. However, little is
             known about the cognitive mechanisms underlying punishment
             in other animals.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-809633-8.01093-1},
   Key = {fds372674}
}

@misc{fds349846,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What did we learn from theape language studies?},
   Pages = {95-104},
   Booktitle = {Bonobos: Unique in Mind, Brain, and Behavior},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780198728511},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198728511.003.0007},
   Abstract = {The ‘ape language’ studies have come and gone, with
             wildly divergent claims about what they have shown. Without
             question, the most sophisticated skills have been displayed
             by Kanzi, a male bonobo exposed from youth to a human-like
             communicative system. This chapter attempts to assess, in an
             objective a manner as possible, the nature of the
             communicative skills that Kanzi and other great apes
             acquired during the various ape language projects. The
             overall conclusion is that bonobos and other apes possess
             most of the requisite cognitive skills for something like a
             human language, including such things as basic symbol
             learning, categorization, sequential (statistical) learning,
             etc. What they lack are the skills and motivations of shared
             intentionality-such things as joint attention,
             perspective-taking and cooperative motives- for adjusting
             their communicative acts for others pragmatically, or for
             learning symbols whose main function is pragmatic.},
   Doi = {10.1093/oso/9780198728511.003.0007},
   Key = {fds349846}
}

@misc{fds361197,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M and Striano, T},
   Title = {How children turn objects into symbols: A cultural learning
             account},
   Pages = {69-97},
   Booktitle = {Symbol Use and Symbolic Representation: Developmental and
             Comparative Perspectives},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {September},
   ISBN = {9780805845976},
   Key = {fds361197}
}

@misc{fds365126,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Middle Step: Joint Intentionality as a Human-Unique Form
             of Second-Personal Engagement},
   Pages = {433-446},
   Booktitle = {The Routledge Handbook of Collective Intentionality},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781138783638},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315768571-41},
   Abstract = {The crucial middle step consists in second-personal
             engagement with engagement others. In this chapter, the
             authors present face-to-face interactions joint
             intentionality. On the other hand there are capacities for
             acting collectively within a social group or culture,
             relying on an understanding and sensitivity to
             supra-individual constructions as cultural conventions,
             norms, and institutional reality. Much attention has been
             paid to processes of collective intentionality, often under
             the rubric of “social ontology”. While the social life
             of even our nearest living relatives, chimpanzees and
             bonobos is characterized by individual intentions, humans
             routinely coordinate with others to form intricate modes of
             collective intentions and enduring cultural practices.
             Chimpanzees have robust and selective intentions to behave
             cooperatively toward their bond partners, including a
             tendency to preferentially trust those partners. The authors
             discuss how early humans evolved new cognitive adaptations,
             most importantly and fundamentally the dual-level cognitive
             structure of joint intentionality.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315768571-41},
   Key = {fds365126}
}

@misc{fds366595,
   Author = {Engelmann, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Prosociality and morality in children and
             chimpanzees},
   Pages = {15-32},
   Booktitle = {New Perspectives on Moral Development},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781138188013},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315642758},
   Abstract = {The modern study of moral development began with Piaget’s
             (1932) The Moral Judgment of the Child, which although
             originally published in the 1930s only became
             internationally known in the 1960s through its influence on
             Kohlberg’s theory (e.g. Kohlberg, 1981). Piaget and
             Kohlberg were both explicit that they were not studying
             children’s moral motivations or behaviour, but only their
             judgements, indeed typically judgements about other
             people’s interactions from a third-party perspective. Much
             of the work in social domain theory championed by Turiel
             (1983), though coming from a somewhat different theoretical
             perspective, also focused on children’s judgements about
             third parties’ interactions.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315642758},
   Key = {fds366595}
}

@misc{fds359905,
   Author = {Quick, AE and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Mixed NPs in German-English and German-Russian bilingual
             children},
   Pages = {127-146},
   Booktitle = {Cognitive Perspectives on Bilingualism},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9781614515852},
   Abstract = {Both cross-linguistic priming methodologies and research on
             codemixed utterances have been concerned with the nature of
             the underlying syntactic representations of bilinguals. The
             present paper investigated code-mixing at the
             morphosyntactic level (NP) by comparing German-English (G-E)
             and German- Russian (G-R) bilingual children between the
             ages of 3;6 and 5;6. Using a language priming paradigm and a
             monolingual interlocutor in each language, we attempted to
             elicit mixed NPs from these children. Results showed that
             G-E bilingual children produced mixed NPs significantly more
             often than G-R bilinguals, providing support for the
             importance of structural similarity in this type of mixing.
             A second finding was that children who were reported as
             code-mixing at home were significantly more likely to
             provide answers while children who did not code-mix remained
             silent. Explanations in terms of individual differences
             and/or balanced competence are discussed.},
   Key = {fds359905}
}

@misc{fds351595,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Grammar},
   Pages = {38-50},
   Booktitle = {The Curated Reference Collection in Neuroscience and
             Biobehavioral Psychology},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780128093245},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.05819-3},
   Abstract = {To acquire competence with a natural language, young
             children must master the grammatical constructions of their
             language(s). In this article we outline the main theoretical
             issues in the field and trace the developmental path
             children follow from talking in single-unit
             “holophrases” to using complex, abstract constructions.
             We describe the development of children’s initial skills
             with word order, case marking, and morphology as abstract
             elements in early constructions, and we discuss the level of
             abstraction characteristic of young children’s grammatical
             constructions at different stages of development and in some
             different languages of the world. Finally, we consider the
             learning processes that enable young children both to
             acquire and to abstract across grammatical
             constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.05819-3},
   Key = {fds351595}
}

@misc{fds351681,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The early ontogeny of human cooperation and
             morality},
   Pages = {279-298},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of Moral Development, Second Edition},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781848729599},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203581957},
   Abstract = {The seminal work in the modern study of children’s moral
             development is Piaget’s (1932/1997) The Moral Judgment of
             the Child. As is well known, Piaget claimed that before the
             age of 8 or 9 years children make moral judgments based only
             on a respect for authority and the social norms emanating
             from this authority-and so they are not really autonomous
             moral agents. But, as is also well known, Piaget focused
             exclusively on the explicit moral judgments that children
             were capable of formulating in language. Kohlberg’s
             extension of Piaget’s framework (e.g., Colby & Kohlberg,
             1987; Kohlberg, 1969, 1976) also asked children to express
             their reasoned moral judgments linguistically, and also
             found that preschool children were essentially premoral
             (i.e., preconventional).},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203581957},
   Key = {fds351681}
}

@misc{fds351692,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cognitive Linguistics and First Language
             Acquisition},
   Booktitle = {The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   ISBN = {9780199738632},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738632.013.0041},
   Abstract = {This article reviews some of the best-known and most
             interesting work on language acquisition from within the
             framework of functional-cognitive linguistics, particularly
             those on meaning and conceptualization as well as usage and
             grammar (grammatical constructions). Although the term is
             often used more narrowly, the article calls this general
             theoretical approach "usage-based" to emphasize the
             assumption common to all functional and cognitive approaches
             that linguistic structure emerges from use, both
             historically and ontogenetically. This is as opposed to the
             dominant view in the field of language acquisition today in
             which "core" grammatical competence is innately given, and
             all that develops is peripheral skills involving the
             lexicon, pragmatics, information processing, and the like.
             The article discusses meaning and conceptualization in child
             language, focusing on image schemas and word meanings as
             well as social cognition, perspective-taking, and culture.
             It also considers usage and grammar in child language,
             including usage-based syntax.},
   Doi = {10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738632.013.0041},
   Key = {fds351692}
}

@misc{fds351693,
   Author = {Wyman, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The ontogenetic origins of human cooperation},
   Booktitle = {Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   ISBN = {9780198568308},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568308.013.0017},
   Abstract = {This article suggests that the ontogenesis of shared
             intentionality depends on the developmentally primitive
             phenomenon of 'joint attention'. This is the ability of the
             infant to understand that they and other individuals can
             attend to the same object and each other's attention
             simultaneously and provides a shared, interpersonal frame in
             which young infants can share experience with others. This
             article compares the skills and motivations involved in
             shared intentionality between humans and chimpanzees. It
             also emphasises a marked difference in their joint attention
             abilities. It suggests that this may explain various
             differences in social-cognitive skills between the two
             species, and proposes that the phylogenesis of joint
             attention may account for the evolution of complex forms of
             cooperation and uniquely human cultural practices.},
   Doi = {10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568308.013.0017},
   Key = {fds351693}
}

@misc{fds351706,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Do chimpanzees know what others see-or only what they are
             looking at?},
   Booktitle = {Rational Animals?},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {March},
   ISBN = {9780198528272},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528272.003.0017},
   Abstract = {This chapter examines the distinction between two approaches
             in interpreting the behaviour in non-human animals. It
             explains the 'boosters' interpret behaviour in
             psychologically rich ways while 'scoffers' prefer
             psychologically lean interpretations. It compares richer and
             leaner interpretations of recent data from four experimental
             paradigms concerning whether chimpanzees know what others
             can or cannot see and argues that the 'booster' hypothesis
             is better supported by the experimental results.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528272.003.0017},
   Key = {fds351706}
}

@misc{fds351707,
   Author = {Lohmann, H and Tomasello, M and Meyer, S},
   Title = {Linguistic Communication and Social Understanding},
   Booktitle = {Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {March},
   ISBN = {9780195159912},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0012},
   Abstract = {This chapter explores the early stages of pragmatic language
             acquisition before taking up the issue of syntax and
             semantics. It suggests that the relation between language
             and theory of mind is different depending on which aspect of
             social understanding is at issue. In particular, it argues
             that an appreciation of other persons as intentional
             agents-the first level of social understanding-is a
             prerequisite for language acquisition. The chapter presents
             evidence from a training study aimed at developing
             false-belief reasoning to show that both conversation about
             deceptive objects and training on the syntax of
             complementation (in the absence of deceptive objects)
             promote three-year-olds' falsebelief understanding. The
             largest training effect occurred in a condition that
             combined conversation and complements. The fact that
             manipulating the deceptive objects without any conversation
             about them was ineffective leads to the conclusion that
             language is a necessary condition for children to make
             progress in their understanding of false beliefs, lending
             support to the claim that language plays a causal role in
             the ontogeny of social understanding.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0012},
   Key = {fds351707}
}

@misc{fds351710,
   Author = {Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Gräfenhain, M and Liebal, K and Liszkowski, U and Moll, H and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M and Warneken,
             F and Wyman, E},
   Title = {Cultural learning and cultural creation},
   Pages = {65-101},
   Booktitle = {Social Life and Social Knowledge: Toward a Process Account
             of Development},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780203809587},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203809587},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203809587},
   Key = {fds351710}
}

@misc{fds375279,
   Author = {Ibbotson, P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Analogical mapping in construction learning},
   Pages = {21-22},
   Booktitle = {The Routledge Encyclopedia of Second Language
             Acquisition},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780415877510},
   Key = {fds375279}
}

@misc{fds351752,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Human Culture in Evolutionary Perspective},
   Volume = {1},
   Booktitle = {Advances in Culture and Psychology},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {December},
   ISBN = {9780195380392},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195380392.003.0001},
   Abstract = {Many animal species live in complex social groups, some of
             whom transmit information across generations "culturally".
             Humans' uniquely cultural way of life began with this kind
             of social organization but then acquired novel
             characteristics as a result of biological adaptations for
             interacting with other persons in species-unique forms of
             cooperative activity, including collaborative
             problem-solving, cooperative communication, and instructed
             learning. These more cooperative, cultural ways of doing
             things have as their psychological foundation various skills
             and motivations for shared intentionality.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195380392.003.0001},
   Key = {fds351752}
}

@misc{fds351760,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What Chimpanzees Know about Seeing, Revisited: An
             Explanation of the Third Kind},
   Booktitle = {Joint Attention Communication and Other Minds: Issues in
             Philosophy and Psychology},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {September},
   ISBN = {9780199245635},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199245635.003.0003},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees follow the gaze of conspecifics and humans
             -follow it past distractors and behind barriers, 'check
             back' with humans when gaze following does not yield
             interesting sights, use gestures appropriately depending on
             the visual access of their recipient, and select different
             pieces of food depending on whether their competitor has
             visual access to them. Taken together, these findings make a
             strong case for the hypothesis that chimpanzees have some
             understanding of what other individuals can and cannot see.
             However, chimpanzees do not seem nearly so skillful in the
             Gesture Choice and Object Choice experimental paradigms.
             Neither behavioral conditioning nor theory of mind
             explanations can account for these results satisfactorily.
             Instead this chapter proposes the idea that chimpanzees have
             the cognitive skills to recall, represent, categorize, and
             reason about the behavior and perception of others, but not
             their intentional or mental states, because they do not know
             that others have such states since they cannot make a link
             to their own. Human beings began their own evolutionary
             trajectory with these same skills, but then at some point in
             their evolution (probably quite recently) they began to
             understand that their own experience could serve as some
             kind of model for that of other persons. This allowed for
             even better prediction and control of the behavior of others
             and better communication and cooperation with them as well,
             and so it was an adaptation with immediate adaptive
             consequences that ensured its survival.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199245635.003.0003},
   Key = {fds351760}
}

@misc{fds351764,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Language Development},
   Pages = {239-257},
   Booktitle = {The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive
             Development, Second edition},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {July},
   ISBN = {9781405191166},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781444325485.ch9},
   Doi = {10.1002/9781444325485.ch9},
   Key = {fds351764}
}

@misc{fds351769,
   Author = {Golinkoff, RM and Hirsh-Pasek, K and Bloom, L and Smith, LB and Woodward, AL and Akhtar, N and Tomasello, M and Hollich,
             G},
   Title = {Counterpoint commentary},
   Booktitle = {Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical
             Acquisition},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9780195130324},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130324.003.007},
   Abstract = {This chapter contains the authors' reactions to the previous
             chapters and stresses the similarities and differences
             between these theoretical views. It discusses that part of
             the debate concerns whether the first words are more like
             the indexical signs of most nonhumans or like the symbols of
             the human 4-year-old. It argues that although there are
             marked contrasts between the views, the work of all the
             authors focuses on a central issue: an understanding of how
             infants break the language barrier by learning
             words.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130324.003.007},
   Key = {fds351769}
}

@misc{fds351770,
   Author = {Akhtar, N and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Social Nature of Words and Word Learning},
   Booktitle = {Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical
             Acquisition},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9780195130324},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130324.003.005},
   Abstract = {This chapter describes research findings from the
             social-pragmatic approach. It discusses that Nameera Akhtar
             and Michael Tomasello's dramatic findings demonstrate how
             word learning occurs in some fairly complex, nonostensive
             situations amid the flow of social interaction. It states
             that current models of word learning, as suggested by Akhtar
             and Tomasello, undervalue the role of social interaction. It
             explains that because language has social goals as its
             ultimate purpose, social interactions are the outcome of
             word learning.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130324.003.005},
   Key = {fds351770}
}

@misc{fds351771,
   Author = {Childers, JB and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Are Nouns Easier to Learn Than Verbs? Three Experimental
             Studies},
   Booktitle = {Action Meets Word: How Children Learn Verbs},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9780195170009},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195170009.003.0013},
   Abstract = {A current controversy in the study of word learning is
             whether it is conceptually easier to learn nouns as compared
             to verbs early in development. This chapter describes three
             experiments which address the noun-verb question in
             different ways. In the first experiment, researchers asked
             how many times (and on how many days) does a 2-yearold need
             to hear a word to be able to learn it, and does this differ
             for nouns and verbs? This second study investigates
             whether-when nouns and verbs are presented in comparable
             sentence contexts, controlling the number of exposures, and
             presenting a dynamic event in both the noun and verb
             conditions-nouns are easier to learn than are verbs. In
             Study 3, researchers compared children's ability to learn
             intransitive and transitive verbs and their ability to
             understand verbs for self-action as opposed to other action,
             to determine whether some of these verb and referent types
             are learned more quickly than are others.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195170009.003.0013},
   Key = {fds351771}
}

@misc{fds351773,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {On the Different Origins of Symbols and Grammar},
   Booktitle = {Language Evolution},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780199244843},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199244843.003.0006},
   Abstract = {This chapter emphasises the role of psychology in language
             evolution, but claims that it was the separate evolution of
             capacities for using symbols and grammar (that is, syntactic
             structure) that distinguishes human communication from the
             communication of other primates. It suggests that there was
             no specific biological adaptation for linguistic
             communication. Rather, there was an adaptation for a broader
             kind of complex social cognition that enabled human culture
             and, as a special case of that, human symbolic
             communication. A crucial part of this adaptation was an
             evolved ability to recognise other individuals as
             intentional agents whose attention and behaviour could be
             shared and manipulated. The capacity for grammar
             subsequently developed, and became refined through processes
             of grammaticalisation occurring across generations - but
             with no additional biological adaptations. In support of
             this perspective, psychological data from the study of
             language development in young children and from comparisons
             with the linguistic, social, and mental capacities of
             nonhuman primates are presented. More generally, this
             chapter sees the origin and emergence of language as merely
             one part in the much larger process of the evolution of
             human culture.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199244843.003.0006},
   Key = {fds351773}
}

@misc{fds351774,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Moll, H},
   Title = {The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and
             culture},
   Pages = {331-349},
   Booktitle = {Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human
             Universals},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9783642027246},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-02725-3_16},
   Abstract = {Human beings share many cognitive skills with their nearest
             primate relatives, especially those for dealing with the
             physical world of objects (and categories and quantities of
             objects) in space and their causal interrelations. But
             humans are, in addition, biologically adapted for cultural
             life in ways that other primates are not. Specifically,
             humans have evolved unique motivations and cognitive skills
             for understanding other persons as cooperative agents with
             whom one can share emotions, experience, and collaborative
             actions (shared intentionality). These motivations and
             skills first emerge in human ontogeny at around one year of
             age, as infants begin to participate with other persons in
             various kinds of collaborative and joint attentional
             activities. Participation in such activities leads humans to
             construct during ontogeny, perspectival and dialogical
             cognitive representations.},
   Doi = {10.1007/978-3-642-02725-3_16},
   Key = {fds351774}
}

@misc{fds351811,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Punishment},
   Volume = {2},
   Pages = {800-805},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780080453378},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-045337-8.00106-6},
   Abstract = {Animals can use punishment as a means to change the behavior
             of others. Punishment can be done for selfish ends with no
             regard for how the target of the act is affected. On the
             other extreme, it can benefit others in a society and be
             motivated by its effects on others. Altruistic punishment,
             third-party punishment, and norm enforcement are special
             cases of punishment that can maintain cooperation, and these
             may not have analogs in animals other than humans. More
             socially sophisticated forms of punishment will require more
             flexible and complex cognitive processes. Of particular
             interest are social (other-regarding) preferences, since
             these may have allowed the evolution of the large-scale
             nonkin cooperation seen only in humans. However, little is
             known about the cognitive mechanisms underlying punishment
             in other animals.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-08-045337-8.00106-6},
   Key = {fds351811}
}

@misc{fds351821,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {First steps toward a usage-based theory of language
             acquisition},
   Pages = {439-458},
   Booktitle = {Cognitive Linguistics: Basic Readings},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {August},
   ISBN = {9783110190847},
   Key = {fds351821}
}

@misc{fds351834,
   Author = {Kruger, AC and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cultural Learning and Learning Culture},
   Pages = {353-372},
   Booktitle = {The Handbook of Education and Human Development: New Models
             of Learning, Teaching and Schooling},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9780631211860},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/b.9780631211860.1998.00018.x},
   Abstract = {The universals and cultural variations of human development
             have been the focus of fruitful study by anthropologists for
             decades. In recent years psychologists also have directed
             their attention, long overdue, to understanding development
             in cultural context. There are striking differences among
             psychologists, however, in the approaches they take to
             culture and development. Most markedly, Cole (1989)
             distinguishes two very different theoretical perspectives on
             cultural psychology and its approach to human development.
             In one perspective the focus is on culture as a collective
             enterprise (e.g., Gauvain, in press: Shweder, 1990; Super
             and Harkness, 1986). There is no need in this view for
             focusing on the individual development of individual
             children since all important forms of learning are socially
             distributed; children simply become more skillful over time
             at participating in various collective activities (Lave and
             Wenger, 1991). Indeed, in some versions of this more
             sociological view of cultural psychology the focus on the
             cultural collective is so strong that there is really no
             justification for reference to the development of
             individuals at all: "Individual, interpersonal, and
             sociocultural processes constitute each other and cannot be
             separated" (Rogoff, Chavajay, and Matusov, 1993, p.
             533).},
   Doi = {10.1111/b.9780631211860.1998.00018.x},
   Key = {fds351834}
}

@misc{fds351835,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Pages = {477-487},
   Booktitle = {A Companion to Cognitive Science},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9780631218517},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781405164535.ch37},
   Abstract = {A central goal of cognitive science is to understand how
             human beings comprehend, produce, and acquire natural
             languages. Throughout the brief history of modern cognitive
             science, the linguistic theory that has been most prominent
             in this endeavor is generative grammar as espoused by Noam
             Chomsky and colleagues. Generative grammar is a theoretical
             approach that seeks to describe and explain natural language
             in terms of its mathematical form, using formal languages
             such as propositional logic and automata theory. The most
             fundamental distinction in generative grammar is therefore
             the formal distinction between semantics and syntax. The
             semantics of a linguistic proposition are the objective
             conditions under which it may truthfully be stated, and the
             syntax of that proposition is the mathematical structure of
             its linguistic elements and relations irrespective of their
             semantics.},
   Doi = {10.1002/9781405164535.ch37},
   Key = {fds351835}
}

@misc{fds351839,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cultural transmission: A view from chimpanzees and human
             infants},
   Pages = {33-47},
   Booktitle = {Cultural Transmission: Psychological, Developmental, Social,
             and Methodological Aspects},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780521880435},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511804670.004},
   Abstract = {Introduction Primates are highly social beings. They begin
             their lives clinging to their mother and nursing, and they
             spend their next few months, or even years, still in
             proximity to her. Adult primates live in close-knit social
             groups, for the most part, in which members individually
             recognize one another and form various types of long-term
             social relationships (Tomasello & Call, 1994, 1997). As
             primates, human beings follow this same pattern, of course,
             but they also have unique forms of sociality that may be
             characterized as “ultrasocial” or, in more common
             parlance, “cultural” (Tomasello, Krüger, & Ratner,
             1993). The forms of sociality that are mostly clearly unique
             to human beings emerge in their ontogeny at approximately 9
             months of age - what I have called the 9-month
             social-cognitive revolution (Tomasello, 1995). This is the
             age at which infants typically begin to engage in the kinds
             of joint-attentional interactions in which they master the
             use of cultural artifacts, including tools and language, and
             become fully active participants in all types of cultural
             rituals, scripts, and games. In this chapter, my goals are
             to (1) characterize the primate and human forms of sociality
             and cultural transmission, and (2) characterize in more
             detail the ontogeny of human cultural propensities.},
   Doi = {10.1017/CBO9780511804670.004},
   Key = {fds351839}
}

@misc{fds368904,
   Author = {Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {CHILDREN’S FIRST LANGUAGE ACQUISITION FROM A USAGE-BASED
             PERSPECTIVE1},
   Pages = {168-196},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language
             Acquisition},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780203938560},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203938560-16},
   Abstract = {There are, however, major debates as to what they bring to
             this language learning: do they come with innate,
             specifically syntactic skills or, rather, with more general
             cognitive and interactive skills? In this chapter, we will
             argue for the latter and suggest that children’s language
             development can be explained in terms of species-specific
             learning and intentional communication. We argue that the
             child learns language from actual “usage events,” i.e.
             from particular utterances in particular contexts, and
             builds up increasingly complex and abstract linguistic
             representations from these.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203938560-16},
   Key = {fds368904}
}

@misc{fds376753,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Grammar},
   Volume = {2},
   Pages = {V2-38-V2-50},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development,
             Three-Volume Set},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780123704603},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012370877-9.00073-6},
   Abstract = {To acquire competence with a natural language, young
             children must master the grammatical constructions of their
             language(s). In this article we outline the main theoretical
             issues in the field and trace the developmental path
             children follow from talking in single-unit
             ‘holophrases’ to using complex, abstract constructions.
             We describe the development of children’s initial skills
             with word order, case marking, and morphology as abstract
             elements in early constructions, and we discuss the level of
             abstraction characteristic of young children’s grammatical
             constructions at different stages of development and in some
             different languages of the world. Finally, we consider the
             learning processes that enable young children both to
             acquire and to abstract across grammatical
             constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-012370877-9.00073-6},
   Key = {fds376753}
}

@misc{fds351988,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Understanding the self as social agent},
   Volume = {112},
   Pages = {449-460},
   Booktitle = {Advances in Psychology},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(05)80024-X},
   Abstract = {This chapter discusses the early development of the
             understanding of self as social agent in the human species,
             and briefly discusses its ontogenetic and phylogenetic
             origins. The chapter outlines the canonical developmental
             sequence, focusing especially on the social-cognitive
             revolution that occurs at around the infant's first
             birthday. The chapter also focuses on the early ontogeny of
             self and investigates in more detail the ontogenetic
             processes that might lead to the social-cognitive
             revolution. The process of understanding one's self as a
             social agent involves: 1) an early identification with but
             differentiation from others in the first 6–8 months of
             life; 2) a clear demonstration of intentionality toward the
             world in one's own behavior; 3) the combination of these two
             developments leading to an understanding of others as
             intentional agents at 9–12 months; and 4) the application
             of that understanding when others regard the self. This is
             the developmental foundation for the uniquely human version
             of self-concept in which the self is understood as a social
             agent in the midst of other social agents, all of whom are
             regarding one another simultaneously. © 1995 Elsevier
             B.V.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0166-4115(05)80024-X},
   Key = {fds351988}
}


%% Chapters in Books   
@misc{fds366592,
   Author = {Sanchez-Amaro, A and Duguid, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Coordination Strategies of Chimpanzees and Children in a
             Prisoner's Dilemma},
   Journal = {FOLIA PRIMATOLOGICA},
   Volume = {89},
   Number = {3-4},
   Pages = {186-186},
   Publisher = {KARGER},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   Key = {fds366592}
}

@misc{fds330415,
   Author = {Krupenye, C and Kano, F and Hirata, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Great apes anticipate actions based on agents' (false)
             beliefs},
   Journal = {INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY},
   Volume = {51},
   Pages = {255-255},
   Publisher = {ROUTLEDGE JOURNALS, TAYLOR & FRANCIS LTD},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {July},
   Key = {fds330415}
}

@misc{fds367354,
   Author = {Bara, BG and Chater, N and Tomasello, M and Varley,
             R},
   Title = {Symposium Communicative Intentions in the
             Mind/Brain},
   Journal = {Cooperative Minds: Social Interaction and Group Dynamics -
             Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive
             Science Society, CogSci 2013},
   Pages = {65-66},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780976831891},
   Key = {fds367354}
}

@misc{fds367355,
   Author = {Call, J and Goldin-Meadow, S and Hobaiter, C and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Language and Gesture Evolution},
   Journal = {Cooperative Minds: Social Interaction and Group Dynamics -
             Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive
             Science Society, CogSci 2013},
   Pages = {57-58},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780976831891},
   Key = {fds367355}
}

@misc{fds320796,
   Author = {Wobber, V and Herrmann, E and Hare, B and Wrangham, R and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Species differences in the rate of cognitive ontogeny among
             humans, chimpanzees, and bonobos},
   Journal = {AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY},
   Volume = {144},
   Pages = {313-314},
   Publisher = {WILEY-BLACKWELL},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   Key = {fds320796}
}

@misc{fds351855,
   Author = {Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence
             hypothesis.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {362},
   Number = {1480},
   Pages = {639-648},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2000},
   Abstract = {Nicholas Humphrey's social intelligence hypothesis proposed
             that the major engine of primate cognitive evolution was
             social competition. Lev Vygotsky also emphasized the social
             dimension of intelligence, but he focused on human primates
             and cultural things such as collaboration, communication and
             teaching. A reasonable proposal is that primate cognition in
             general was driven mainly by social competition, but beyond
             that the unique aspects of human cognition were driven by,
             or even constituted by, social cooperation. In the present
             paper, we provide evidence for this Vygotskian intelligence
             hypothesis by comparing the social-cognitive skills of great
             apes with those of young human children in several domains
             of activity involving cooperation and communication with
             others. We argue, finally, that regular participation in
             cooperative, cultural interactions during ontogeny leads
             children to construct uniquely powerful forms of
             perspectival cognitive representation.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2006.2000},
   Key = {fds351855}
}


%% Journal Articles   
@article{fds374401,
   Author = {Winter Née Grocke and P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {From what I want to do to what we decided to do:
             5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, honor their agreements
             with peers.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {239},
   Pages = {105811},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105811},
   Abstract = {Sometimes we have a personal preference but we agree with
             others to follow a different course of action. In this
             study, 3- and 5-year-old children (N = 160) expressed a
             preference for playing a game one way and were then
             confronted with peers who expressed a different preference.
             The experimenter then either got the participants to agree
             with the peers explicitly or just shrugged her shoulders and
             moved on. The children were then left alone to play the game
             unobserved. Only the older children stuck to their agreement
             to play the game as the peers wished. These results suggest
             that by 5 years of age children's sense of commitment to
             agreements is strong enough to override their personal
             preferences.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105811},
   Key = {fds374401}
}

@article{fds374171,
   Author = {Katz, T and Kushnir, T and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children are eager to take credit for prosocial acts, and
             cost affects this tendency.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {237},
   Pages = {105764},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105764},
   Abstract = {We report two experiments on children's tendency to enhance
             their reputations through communicative acts. In the
             experiments, 4-year-olds (N = 120) had the opportunity to
             inform a social partner that they had helped him in his
             absence. In a first experiment, we pitted a prosocial act
             ("Let's help clean up for Doggie!") against an instrumental
             act ("Let's move these out of our way"). Children in the
             prosocial condition were quicker to inform their partner of
             the act and more likely to protest when another individual
             was given credit for it. In a second experiment, we
             replicated the prosocial condition but with a new
             manipulation: high-cost versus low-cost helping. We
             manipulated both the language surrounding cost (i.e., "This
             will be pretty tough to clean up" vs. "It will be really
             easy to clean this up") and how difficult the task itself
             was. As predicted, children in the high-cost condition were
             quicker to inform their partner of the act and more likely
             to take back credit for it. These results suggest that even
             4-year-old children make active attempts to elicit positive
             reputational judgments for their prosocial acts, with cost
             as a moderating factor.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105764},
   Key = {fds374171}
}

@article{fds374236,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Differences in the Social Motivations and Emotions of Humans
             and Other Great Apes.},
   Journal = {Human nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {588-604},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0},
   Abstract = {Humans share with other mammals and primates many social
             motivations and emotions, but they are also much more
             cooperative than even their closest primate relatives. Here
             I review recent comparative experiments and analyses that
             illustrate humans' species-typical social motivations and
             emotions for cooperation in comparison with those of other
             great apes. These may be classified most generally as (i)
             'you > me' (e.g., prosocial sympathy, informative and
             pedagogical motives in communication); (ii) 'you = me'
             (e.g., feelings of mutual respect, fairness, resentment);
             (iii) 'we > me' (e.g., feelings of obligation and guilt);
             and (iv) 'WE (in the group) > me' (e.g., in-group
             loyalty and conformity to norms, shame, and many in-group
             biases). The existence of these species-typical and
             species-universal motivations and emotions provides
             compelling evidence for the importance of cooperative
             activities in the human species.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0},
   Key = {fds374236}
}

@article{fds374400,
   Author = {Vasil, J and Price, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Thought and language: Effects of group-mindedness on young
             children's interpretation of exclusive we.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14049},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated whether age-related changes
             in the conceptualization of social groups influences
             interpretation of the pronoun we. Sixty-four 2- and
             4-year-olds (N = 29 female, 50 White-identifying) viewed
             scenarios in which it was ambiguous how many puppets
             performed an activity together. When asked who performed the
             activity, a speaker puppet responded, "We did!" In one
             condition, the speaker was near one and distant from another
             puppet, implying a dyadic interpretation of we. In another
             condition, the speaker was distant from both, thus pulling
             for a group interpretation. In the former condition, 2- and
             4-year-olds favored the dyadic interpretation. In the latter
             condition, only 4-year-olds favored the group
             interpretation. Age-related conceptual development "expands"
             the set of conceivable plural person referents.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.14049},
   Key = {fds374400}
}

@article{fds373982,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A Shared Intentionality Account of Uniquely Human Social
             Bonding.},
   Journal = {Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the
             Association for Psychological Science},
   Pages = {17456916231201795},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/17456916231201795},
   Abstract = {Many mechanisms of social bonding are common to all
             primates, but humans seemingly have developed some that are
             unique to the species. These involve various kinds of
             interactive experiences-from taking a walk together to
             having a conversation-whose common feature is the triadic
             sharing of experience. Current theories of social bonding
             have no explanation for why humans should have these unique
             bonding mechanisms. Here we propose a shared intentionality
             account of uniquely human social bonding. Humans evolved to
             participate with others in unique forms of cooperative and
             communicative activities that both depend on and create
             shared experience. Sharing experience in these activities
             causes partners to feel closer because it allows them to
             assess their partner's cooperative competence and motivation
             toward them and because the shared representations created
             during such interactions make subsequent cooperative
             interactions easier and more effective.},
   Doi = {10.1177/17456916231201795},
   Key = {fds373982}
}

@article{fds370890,
   Author = {Vasil, J and Moore, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Thought and language: association of groupmindedness with
             young English-speaking children’s production of
             pronouns},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {516-538},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01427237231169398},
   Abstract = {Shared intentionality theory posits that at age 3, children
             expand their conception of plural agency to include 3- or
             more-person groups. We sought to determine whether this
             conceptual shift is detectable in children’s pronoun use.
             We report the results of a series of Bayesian hierarchical
             generative models fitted to 479 English-speaking
             children’s first-person plural, first-person singular,
             second-person, third-person plural, and third-person
             singular pronouns. As a proportion of pronouns, children
             used more first-person plural pronouns, only, after 3;0
             compared to before. Additionally, children used more 1pp.
             pronouns when their mothers used more 1pp. pronouns. As a
             proportion of total utterances, all pronoun classes were
             used more often as children aged. These findings suggest
             that a shift in children’s social conceptualizations at
             age 3 is reflected in their use of 1pp. pronouns.},
   Doi = {10.1177/01427237231169398},
   Key = {fds370890}
}

@article{fds370629,
   Author = {Benozio, A and House, BR and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Apes reciprocate food positively and negatively.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {290},
   Number = {1998},
   Pages = {20222541},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.2541},
   Abstract = {Reciprocal food exchange is widespread in human societies
             but not among great apes, who may view food mainly as a
             target for competition. Understanding the similarities and
             differences between great apes' and humans' willingness to
             exchange food is important for our models regarding the
             origins of uniquely human forms of cooperation. Here, we
             demonstrate in-kind food exchanges in experimental settings
             with great apes for the first time. The initial sample
             consisted of 13 chimpanzees and 5 bonobos in the control
             phases, and the test phases included 10 chimpanzees and 2
             bonobos, compared with a sample of 48 human children aged 4
             years. First, we replicated prior findings showing no
             spontaneous food exchanges in great apes. Second, we
             discovered that when apes believe that conspecifics have
             'intentionally' transferred food to them, positive
             reciprocal food exchanges (food-for-food) are not only
             possible but reach the same levels as in young children
             (approx. 75-80%). Third, we found that great apes engage in
             negative reciprocal food exchanges (no-food for no-food) but
             to a lower extent than children. This provides evidence for
             reciprocal food exchange in great apes in experimental
             settings and suggests that while a potential mechanism of
             <i>fostering</i> cooperation (via positive reciprocal
             exchanges) may be shared across species, a stabilizing
             mechanism (via negative reciprocity) is not.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2022.2541},
   Key = {fds370629}
}

@article{fds368903,
   Author = {Schäfer, M and B M Haun and D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's consideration of collaboration and merit when
             making sharing decisions in private.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {228},
   Pages = {105609},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105609},
   Abstract = {Young children share equally when they acquire resources
             through collaboration with a partner, yet it is unclear
             whether they do so because in such contexts resources are
             encountered as common and distributed in front of the
             recipient or because collaboration promotes a sense of
             work-based fairness. In the current studies, 5- and
             8-year-old children from Germany (N = 193) acquired
             resources either by working individually alongside or by
             collaborating with a peer. After finding out that the
             partner's container was empty, they decided in private
             whether they wanted to donate some resources to the peer.
             When both partners had worked with equal efforts (Study 1),
             children shared more after collaboration than after
             individual work. When one partner had worked with much more
             effort than the other (Study 2), children shared more with a
             harder-working partner than with a less-working partner
             independently of whether they had collaborated or worked
             individually. Younger children were more generous than older
             children, in particular after collaboration. These findings
             support the view that collaboration promotes a genuine sense
             of fairness in young children, but they also indicate that
             merit-based notions of fairness in the context of work may
             develop independently of collaboration, at least by the
             beginning of middle childhood and in Western
             societies.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105609},
   Key = {fds368903}
}

@article{fds367773,
   Author = {Colle, L and Grosse, G and Behne, T and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Just teasing! - Infants' and toddlers' understanding of
             teasing interactions and its effect on social
             bonding.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {231},
   Pages = {105314},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105314},
   Abstract = {The current study investigates infants' and toddlers'
             understanding of teasing interactions and its effect on
             subsequent social interactions. Teasing is a special kind of
             social interaction due to its dual nature: It consists of a
             slightly provocative contingent action accompanied by
             positive ostensive emotional cues. Teasing thus presents an
             especially interesting test case to inform us about young
             children's abilities to deal with complex social intentions.
             In a first experiment, we looked at 9-, 12-, and
             18-month-old infants' ability to understand and
             differentiate a teasing intention from a trying intention
             and a refuse intention. We found that by 12 months of age,
             infants react differently (gaze, reach) and by 18 months
             they smile more in reaction to the Tease condition. In the
             second experiment, we tested 13-, 20- and 30-month-old
             children in closely matched purely playful and teasing
             situations. We also investigated potential social effects of
             teasing interactions on a subsequent affiliation sequence.
             Twenty- and 30-month-old children smile more in the Teasing
             than in the Play condition. For the 30-month-old toddlers,
             additionally, number of laughs is much higher in the Tease
             than in the Play condition. No effect on affiliation could
             be found. Thus, from very early in development, infants and
             toddlers are able to differentiate teasing from
             superficially similar but serious behavior and from around
             18 months of age they enjoy it more. Infants and toddlers
             are able to process a complex social intention like teasing.
             Findings are discussed regarding infant and toddler
             intention understanding abilities.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105314},
   Key = {fds367773}
}

@article{fds362755,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Gerdemann, SC and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Evidence for a developmental shift in the motivation
             underlying helping in early childhood.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e13253},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13253},
   Abstract = {We investigated children's positive emotions as an indicator
             of their underlying prosocial motivation. In Study 1, 2-,
             and 5-year-old children (N = 64) could either help an
             individual or watch as another person provided help.
             Following the helping event and using depth sensor imaging,
             we measured children's positive emotions through changes in
             postural elevation. For 2-year-olds, helping the individual
             and watching another person help was equally rewarding;
             5-year-olds showed greater postural elevation after actively
             helping. In Study 2, 5-year-olds' (N = 59) positive
             emotions following helping were greater when an audience was
             watching. Together, these results suggest that 2-year-old
             children have an intrinsic concern that individuals be
             helped whereas 5-year-old children have an additional,
             strategic motivation to improve their reputation by
             helping.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.13253},
   Key = {fds362755}
}

@article{fds365125,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Social cognition and metacognition in great apes: a
             theory.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {25-35},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01662-0},
   Abstract = {Twenty-five years ago, at the founding of this journal,
             there existed only a few conflicting findings about great
             apes' social-cognitive skills (theory of mind). In the 2 ½
             decades since, we have discovered that great apes understand
             the goals, intentions, perceptions, and knowledge of others,
             and they use this knowledge to their advantage in
             competitive interactions. Twenty-five years ago there
             existed basically no studies on great apes' metacognitive
             skills. In the 2 ½ decades since, we have discovered that
             great apes monitor their uncertainty and base their
             decisions on that, or else decide to gather more information
             to make better decisions. The current paper reviews the past
             25 years of research on great ape social cognition and
             metacognition and proposes a theory about how the two are
             evolutionarily related.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-022-01662-0},
   Key = {fds365125}
}

@article{fds371813,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Thielhelm, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Five-year-old children show cooperative preferences for
             faces with white sclera.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {225},
   Pages = {105532},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105532},
   Abstract = {The cooperative eye hypothesis posits that human eye
             morphology evolved to facilitate cooperation. Although it is
             known that young children prefer stimuli with eyes that
             contain white sclera, it is unknown whether white sclera
             influences children's perception of a partner's
             cooperativeness specifically. In the current studies, we
             used an online methodology to present 5-year-old children
             with moving three-dimensional face models in which facial
             morphology was manipulated. Children found "alien" faces
             with human eyes more cooperative than faces with dark sclera
             (Study 2) but not faces with enlarged irises (Study 1). For
             more human-like faces (Study 3), children found human eyes
             more cooperative than either enlarged irises or dark sclera
             and found faces with enlarged irises cuter (but not more
             cooperative) than eyes with dark sclera. Together, these
             results provide strong support for the cooperative eye
             hypothesis.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105532},
   Key = {fds371813}
}

@article{fds364183,
   Author = {Li, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Disagreement, justification, and equitable moral judgments:
             A brief training study.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {223},
   Pages = {105494},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105494},
   Abstract = {Although theorists agree that social interactions play a
             major role in moral development, previous research has not
             experimentally assessed how specific features of social
             interactions affect children's moral judgments and
             reasoning. The current study assessed two features:
             disagreement and justification. In a brief training phase,
             children aged 4-5.5 years (N = 129) discussed simple
             moral scenarios about issues of fairness (how to allocate
             things between individuals) with a puppet who, in a
             between-participants factorial design, either agreed or
             disagreed with the children's ideas and either asked or did
             not ask the children to justify their ideas. Children then
             responded to another set of moral scenarios in a test phase
             that was the same for all children. Children in the "agree
             and do not justify" baseline condition showed an inflexible
             equality bias (preferring only equal allocations regardless
             of context), but children who had experiences of
             disagreement or experiences of being asked to justify
             themselves shifted toward making equitable decisions based
             on common ground norms and values. Furthermore, false belief
             competence was related to children's decisions and
             justifications. These findings support the classic Piagetian
             hypothesis that social interactions are a catalyst of
             cognitive disequilibrium and moral development.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105494},
   Key = {fds364183}
}

@article{fds367772,
   Author = {Li, L and Tucker, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children judge defection less negatively when there's
             a good justification},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {64},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101268},
   Abstract = {Morality includes a common ground ranking of values, of
             which a central theme is that prosocial actions are more
             justifiable than selfish ones. Learning to distinguish
             between good versus bad justifications for actions based on
             a common ground ranking of values is a key aspect of moral
             development. The current study assessed this type of
             understanding in young children. In a within-participants
             design, young children (N = 64) saw puppets who promised to
             show them a cool toy, failed to fulfill their promise, and
             then gave either a good (prosocial), bad (selfish), or no
             justification for their defection. Children's judgments
             about defection following good justifications were less
             negative than their judgments about defection following bad
             or no justifications, which did not differ. When asked to
             justify their judgments, 5-year-olds (but not 3-year-olds)
             made more normative or promise-referencing statements when
             reasoning about puppets who gave good justifications as
             opposed to bad or no justifications. Children's rates of
             tattling on, liking, and inviting puppets to play did not
             vary by the type of justification that a puppet gave.
             Overall, the findings suggest that the capacity to reference
             a common ground ranking of values, a key component of human
             cooperation and morality, is present in young
             children.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101268},
   Key = {fds367772}
}

@article{fds362987,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How fairness and dominance guide young children's bargaining
             decisions.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {93},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1318-1333},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13757},
   Abstract = {Reaching agreements in conflicts is an important
             developmental challenge. Here, German 5-year-olds
             (N = 284, 49% female, mostly White, mixed socioeconomic
             backgrounds; data collection: June 2016-November 2017) faced
             repeated face-to-face bargaining problems in which they
             chose between fair and unfair reward divisions. Across three
             studies, children mostly settled on fair divisions. However,
             dominant children tended to benefit more from bargaining
             outcomes (in Study 1 and 2 but not Study 3) and children
             mostly failed to use leverage to enforce fairness.
             Communication analyses revealed that children giving orders
             to their partner had a bargaining advantage and that
             children provided and responded to fairness reasons. These
             findings indicate that fairness concerns and dominance are
             both key factors that shape young children's bargaining
             decisions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.13757},
   Key = {fds362987}
}

@article{fds364336,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The coordination of attention and action in great apes and
             humans.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {377},
   Number = {1859},
   Pages = {20210093},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0093},
   Abstract = {Great apes can discern what others are attending to and even
             direct others' attention to themselves in flexible ways. But
             they seemingly do not coordinate their attention with one
             another recursively-understanding that the other is
             monitoring their attention just as they are monitoring
             hers-in acts of joint attention, at least not in the same
             way as young human children. Similarly, great apes
             collaborate with partners in many flexible ways, but they
             seemingly do not coordinate with others to form mutually
             obligating joint goals and commitments, nor regulate the
             collaboration via acts of intentional communication, at
             least not in the same way as young human children. The
             hypothesis defended here is that it is precisely in their
             capacities to coordinate attention and action with
             others-that is, in their capacities for shared
             intentionality-that humans are most clearly distinguished
             from other great apes. This article is part of the theme
             issue 'Revisiting the human 'interaction engine':
             comparative approaches to social action coordination'.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2021.0093},
   Key = {fds364336}
}

@article{fds362986,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What is it like to be a chimpanzee?},
   Journal = {Synthese},
   Volume = {200},
   Number = {2},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11229-022-03574-5},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees and humans are close evolutionary relatives who
             behave in many of the same ways based on a similar type of
             agentive organization. To what degree do they experience the
             world in similar ways as well? Using contemporary research
             in evolutionarily biology and animal cognition, I explicitly
             compare the kinds of experience the two species of capable
             of having. I conclude that chimpanzees’ experience of the
             world, their experiential niche as I call it, is: (i)
             intentional in basically the same way as humans’; (ii)
             rational in the sense that it is self-critical and operates
             with logically structured causal and intentional inferences;
             but (iii) not normative at all in that it does not operate
             with “objective” evaluative standards. Scientific data
             do not answer philosophical questions, but they provide rich
             raw material for scientists and philosophers alike to
             reflect on and clarify fundamental psychological
             concepts.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11229-022-03574-5},
   Key = {fds362986}
}

@article{fds362632,
   Author = {O'Madagain, C and Helming, KA and Schmidt, MFH and Shupe, E and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Great apes and human children rationally monitor their
             decisions.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {289},
   Number = {1971},
   Pages = {20212686},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2686},
   Abstract = {Several species can detect when they are uncertain about
             what decision to make-revealed by opting out of the choice,
             or by seeking more information before deciding. However, we
             do not know whether any nonhuman animals recognize when they
             need more information to make a decision because new
             evidence contradicts an already-formed belief. Here, we
             explore this ability in great apes and human children.
             First, we show that after great apes saw new evidence
             contradicting their belief about which of two rewards was
             greater, they stopped to recheck the evidence for their
             belief before deciding. This indicates the ability to keep
             track of the reasons for their decisions, or 'rational
             monitoring' of the decision-making process. Children did the
             same at 5 years of age, but not at 3 years. In a second
             study, participants formed a belief about a reward's
             location, but then a social partner contradicted them, by
             picking the opposite location. This time even 3-year-old
             children rechecked the evidence, while apes ignored the
             disagreement. While apes were sensitive only to the conflict
             in physical evidence, the youngest children were more
             sensitive to peer disagreement than conflicting physical
             evidence.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2021.2686},
   Key = {fds362632}
}

@article{fds359682,
   Author = {Vasil, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Effects of "we"-framing on young children's commitment,
             sharing, and helping.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {214},
   Pages = {105278},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105278},
   Abstract = {By around 3 years of age, collaboration induces in young
             children a normative sense of "we" that creates a sense of
             obligation (e.g., commitment, fairness) toward their
             collaborative partner. The current study investigated
             whether this normative sense of we could be induced purely
             verbally in 3- and 4-year-old children. Children joined a
             puppet at a table to draw. In one condition the puppet
             repeatedly framed things as "we" are going to sit at the
             table, "we" are going to draw, and so forth, whereas in the
             other condition the pronoun used was always "you." Dependent
             measures gauged children's commitment, resource
             distribution, and helping behavior toward their partner.
             Results showed that both 3- and 4-year-olds felt a greater
             sense of commitment to their partner after "we"-framing than
             after "you"-framing. The 4-year-olds evidenced this
             commitment by showing a greater reluctance to abandon their
             partner for a more fun game compared with the 3-year-olds.
             The 3-year-olds did not share this reluctance, but when they
             did abandon their partner they more often took leave
             following we-framing by "announcing" their leaving. There
             were no effects of we-framing on children's sharing with
             their partner or helping behavior. These results suggest
             that verbal we-framing, as compared with you-framing, is an
             effective means of inducing in children a sense of shared
             agency and commitment with a partner.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105278},
   Key = {fds359682}
}

@article{fds360579,
   Author = {O'Madagain, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Shared intentionality, reason-giving and the evolution of
             human culture.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {377},
   Number = {1843},
   Pages = {20200320},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0320},
   Abstract = {The biological approach to culture focuses almost
             exclusively on processes of social learning, to the neglect
             of processes of cultural coordination including joint action
             and shared intentionality. In this paper, we argue that the
             distinctive features of human culture derive from humans'
             unique skills and motivations for coordinating with one
             another around different types of action and information. As
             different levels of these skills of 'shared intentionality'
             emerged over the last several hundred thousand years, human
             culture became characterized first by such things as
             collaborative activities and pedagogy based on cooperative
             communication, and then by such things as collaborative
             innovations and normatively structured pedagogy. As a kind
             of capstone of this trajectory, humans began to coordinate
             not just on joint actions and shared beliefs, but on the
             reasons for what we believe or how we act. Coordinating on
             reasons powered the kinds of extremely rapid innovation and
             stable cumulative cultural evolution especially
             characteristic of the human species in the last several tens
             of thousands of years. This article is part of a discussion
             meeting issue 'The emergence of collective knowledge and
             cumulative culture in animals, humans and
             machines'.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2020.0320},
   Key = {fds360579}
}

@article{fds362756,
   Author = {Kanngiesser, P and Schäfer, M and Herrmann, E and Zeidler, H and Haun,
             D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children across societies enforce conventional norms but in
             culturally variable ways.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {119},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e2112521118},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2112521118},
   Abstract = {Individuals in all societies conform to their cultural
             group's conventional norms, from how to dress on certain
             occasions to how to play certain games. It is an open
             question, however, whether individuals in all societies
             actively enforce the group's conventional norms when others
             break them. We investigated third-party enforcement of
             conventional norms in 5- to 8-y-old children (<i>n</i> =
             376) from eight diverse small-scale and large-scale
             societies. Children learned the rules for playing a new
             sorting game and then, observed a peer who was apparently
             breaking them. Across societies, observer children
             intervened frequently to correct their misguided peer (i.e.,
             more frequently than when the peer was following the rules).
             However, both the magnitude and the style of interventions
             varied across societies. Detailed analyses of children's
             interactions revealed societal differences in children's
             verbal protest styles as well as in their use of actions,
             gestures, and nonverbal expressions to intervene. Observers'
             interventions predicted whether their peer adopted the
             observer's sorting rule. Enforcement of conventional norms
             appears to be an early emerging human universal that comes
             to be expressed in culturally variable ways.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.2112521118},
   Key = {fds362756}
}

@article{fds359904,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Knowledge-by-acquaintance before propositional
             knowledge/belief.},
   Journal = {The Behavioral and brain sciences},
   Volume = {44},
   Pages = {e173},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x20001387},
   Abstract = {More basic than the authors' distinction between knowing and
             believing is a distinction between knowledge-by-acquaintance
             (I know John Smith) and propositional knowledge/belief (I
             know/believe that John Smith lives in Durham). This
             distinction provides a better account of both the
             comparative and developmental data.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x20001387},
   Key = {fds359904}
}

@article{fds359733,
   Author = {Kanngiesser, P and Mammen, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of justifications for
             breaking a promise},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {60},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101127},
   Abstract = {There are sometimes legitimate reasons for breaking a
             promise when circumstances change. We investigated 3- and
             5-year-old German children's understanding of promise
             breaking in prosocial (helping someone else) and selfish
             (playing with someone else) conditions. In Study 1 (n = 80,
             50% girls), preschoolers initially kept their own promise in
             all conditions. When they eventually broke their promise,
             3-year-olds’ justifications mostly referenced salient
             events, whereas 5-year-olds also referenced social norms. In
             Study 2 (n = 65, 49% girls), 5-year-olds preferred others’
             promise-breaking more in prosocial than selfish conditions;
             3-year-olds showed the reverse pattern. Three-year-olds’
             justifications focused on desires, whereas 5-year-olds
             focused on relevant events. Overall, 3-year-olds were able
             to offer justifications, but 5-year-olds started to
             distinguish what counted in the eyes of others as “good”
             and “bad” reasons for promise breaking.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101127},
   Key = {fds359733}
}

@article{fds359681,
   Author = {Straka, BC and Stanaland, A and Tomasello, M and Gaither,
             SE},
   Title = {Who can be in a group? 3- to 5-year-old children construe
             realistic social groups through mutual intentionality},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {60},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101097},
   Abstract = {Recent research suggests that young children's causal
             justification for minimal group membership can be induced
             via a cognitive framework of mutual intentionality. That is,
             an individual can become a group member when both the
             individual and group agree to membership. Here, we
             investigated if children ages 3–5 understand groups formed
             by mutual intentions and whether they apply mutual
             intentions to realistic groups with varying entitative and
             essentialized qualities. In two studies (N = 197), we asked
             3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children if a novel character could
             join an existing group based on intentionality (mutual,
             individual-, group-only) and group type (task, friends,
             family). We find that 4- and 5-year-olds robustly relied on
             mutual intentions to constitute group membership and
             3-year-olds also demonstrated emerging usage of this
             cognitive framework. Moreover, children employed mutual
             intentionality across different group types, suggesting a
             general framework for children's understanding of different
             social groups.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101097},
   Key = {fds359681}
}

@article{fds355156,
   Author = {Kachel, G and Moore, R and Hepach, R and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Toddlers Prefer Adults as Informants: 2- and 3-Year-Olds'
             Use of and Attention to Pointing Gestures From Peer and
             Adult Partners.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {92},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {e635-e652},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13544},
   Abstract = {Two- and 3-year-old children (N = 96) were tested in an
             object-choice task with video presentations of peer and
             adult partners. An immersive, semi-interactive procedure
             enabled both the close matching of adult and peer conditions
             and the combination of participants' choice behavior with
             looking time measures. Children were more likely to use
             information provided by adults. As the effect was more
             pronounced in the younger age-group, the observed bias may
             fade during toddlerhood. As there were no differences in
             children's propensity to follow peer and adult gestures with
             their gaze, these findings provide some of the earliest
             evidence to date that young children take an interlocutor's
             age into account when judging ostensively communicated
             testimony.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.13544},
   Key = {fds355156}
}

@article{fds352448,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Response to: Rethinking Human Development and the Shared
             Intentionality Hypothesis},
   Journal = {Review of Philosophy and Psychology},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {465-468},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13164-020-00510-9},
   Abstract = {I respond to Moll, Nichols, and Mackey’s review of my book
             Becoming Human. I agree with many of their points, but have
             my own point of view on some others.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s13164-020-00510-9},
   Key = {fds352448}
}

@article{fds357565,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Norms Require Not Just Technical Skill and Social Learning,
             but Real Cooperation},
   Journal = {Analyse und Kritik},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {219-223},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/auk-2021-0012},
   Abstract = {Birch's account of the evolutionary origins of social norms
             is essentially individualistic. It begins with individuals
             regulating their own actions toward internally represented
             goals, as evaluative standards, and adds in a social
             dimension only secondarily. I argue that a better account
             begins at the outset with uniquely human collaborative
             activity in which individuals share evaluative standards
             about how anyone who would play a given role must behave
             both toward their joint goal and toward one another. This
             then scaled up to the shared normative standards for anyone
             who would be a member of 'our' social group.},
   Doi = {10.1515/auk-2021-0012},
   Key = {fds357565}
}

@article{fds358692,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Kano, F and Albiach-Serrano, A and Benziad,
             L and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) internal arousal remains
             elevated if they cannot themselves help a
             conspecific.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {135},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {196-207},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000255},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees help conspecifics achieve their goals in
             instrumental situations, but neither their immediate
             motivation nor the evolutionary basis of their motivation is
             clear. In the current study, we gave chimpanzees the
             opportunity to instrumentally help a conspecific to obtain
             food. Following recent studies with human children, we
             measured their pupil diameter at various points in the
             process. Like young children, chimpanzees' pupil diameter
             decreased soon after they had helped. However, unlike
             children, chimpanzees' pupils remained more dilated upon
             watching a third party provide the needed help instead of
             them. Our interpretation is that chimpanzees are motivated
             to help others, and the evolutionary basis is direct or
             indirect reciprocity, as providing help oneself sets the
             conditions for a payback. This is in contrast to young
             children whose goal is to see others being helped-by
             whomever-presumably because their helping is not based on
             reciprocity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/com0000255},
   Key = {fds358692}
}

@article{fds356461,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Nafe, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Development of the Liking Gap: Children Older Than 5
             Years Think That Partners Evaluate Them Less Positively Than
             They Evaluate Their Partners.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {789-798},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797620980754},
   Abstract = {After two strangers have briefly interacted with one
             another, both believe that they like their partner more than
             their partner likes them. A plausible explanation for this
             <i>liking gap</i> is that people are constantly worrying
             about how others are evaluating them. If so, one would
             expect the liking gap to emerge in young children as they
             become more concerned with their reputations and the
             impression they make on other people. The current study
             (<i>N</i> = 241 U.S. children; age range = 4-11 years)
             supported this hypothesis, showing a liking gap beginning
             when children were 5 years old, the age at which they first
             become concerned with other people's evaluations of them.
             Moreover, the liking gap became more pronounced as children
             got older. These findings provide the first developmental
             description of the liking gap and support the hypothesis
             that this phenomenon is related to individuals' concerns for
             how others evaluate them.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797620980754},
   Key = {fds356461}
}

@article{fds356996,
   Author = {O’Madagain, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Joint attention to mental content and the social origin of
             reasoning},
   Journal = {Synthese},
   Volume = {198},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {4057-4078},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02327-1},
   Abstract = {Growing evidence indicates that our higher rational
             capacities depend on social interaction—that only through
             engaging with others do we acquire the ability to evaluate
             beliefs as true or false, or to reflect on and evaluate the
             reasons that support our beliefs. Up to now, however, we
             have had little understanding of how this works. Here we
             argue that a uniquely human socio-linguistic phenomenon
             which we call ‘joint attention to mental content’ (JAM)
             plays a key role. JAM is the ability to focus together in
             conversation on the content of our mental states, such as
             beliefs and reasons. In such conversations it can be made
             clear that our attitudes to beliefs or reasons may
             conflict—that what I think is true, you might think is
             false, or that what I think is a good reason for believing
             something, you might think is a bad reason. We argue that
             through JAM, children discover that mental contents can be
             evaluated under various attitudes, and that this discovery
             transforms their mind-reading and reasoning
             abilities.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11229-019-02327-1},
   Key = {fds356996}
}

@article{fds352900,
   Author = {Siposova, B and Grueneisen, S and Helming, K and Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M},
   Title = {Common knowledge that help is needed increases helping
             behavior in children.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {201},
   Pages = {104973},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104973},
   Abstract = {Although there is considerable evidence that at least some
             helping behavior is motivated by genuine concern for others'
             well-being, sometimes we also help solely out of a sense of
             obligation to the persons in need. Our sense of obligation
             to help may be particularly strong when there is common
             knowledge between the helper and the helpee that the helpee
             needs help. To test whether children's helping behavior is
             affected by having common knowledge with the recipient about
             the recipient's need, 6-year-olds faced a dilemma: They
             could either collect stickers or help an experimenter.
             Children were more likely to help when they and the
             experimenter had common knowledge about the experimenter's
             plight (because they heard it together) than when they each
             had private knowledge about it (because they heard it
             individually). These results suggest that already in young
             children common knowledge can heighten the sense of
             obligation to help others in need.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104973},
   Key = {fds352900}
}

@article{fds354956,
   Author = {Mammen, M and Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's moral judgments depend on the social
             relationship between agents},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {57},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100973},
   Abstract = {Moral judgments can vary depending on the social
             relationship between agents. We presented 4- and 6-year-old
             peer dyads (N = 128) with stories, in which a parent (parent
             condition) or a peer protagonist (peer condition) faced a
             child in need of help (e.g., the child is thirsty). The
             dyads had to decide whether the protagonist helped at a cost
             (e.g., by giving up their water) or not. 6-year-olds
             expected a parent to help their child more than they
             expected a child to help their peer. Moreover, children
             justified their expectations more often with normative
             statements (e.g., “She has to help”) in the parent
             condition than in the peer condition. Thus, refusal to help
             a child was more acceptable coming from a peer than from a
             parent. The results suggest that young children take into
             account multiple perspectives and form different normative
             expectations for different social agents when making moral
             judgments.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100973},
   Key = {fds354956}
}

@article{fds355300,
   Author = {Domberg, A and Tomasello, M and Köymen, B},
   Title = {Collaborative reasoning in the context of group
             competition.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {e0246589},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0246589},
   Abstract = {A key skill in collaborative problem-solving is to
             communicate and evaluate reasons for proposals to arrive at
             the decision benefiting all group members. Although it is
             well-documented that collaborative contexts facilitate young
             children's reasoning, less is known about whether
             competition with other groups contributes to children's
             collaborative reasoning. We investigated whether
             between-group competition facilitates children's
             within-group collaborative reasoning, regarding their
             production of reasons and their use of transacts,
             communicative acts that operate on one another's proposals
             and reasoning. We presented 5- and 7-year-old peer dyads
             with two collaborative problem-solving tasks (decorating a
             zoo and a dollhouse). In one task, children competed against
             another group (the competitive condition); whereas in the
             other task, they did not (non-competitive condition). Our
             results suggest that children's sensitivity to group
             competition as reflected in their reasoning changed
             depending on the task. When they decorated a house, they
             produced more transacts in the competitive condition than in
             the non-competitive condition; whereas when they decorated a
             zoo, this pattern was reversed. Thus, our results highlight
             that group competition did not influence children's
             collaborative reasoning consistently across different
             contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0246589},
   Key = {fds355300}
}

@article{fds355723,
   Author = {Plötner, M and Hepach, R and Over, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children share more under time pressure than after a
             delay.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e0248121},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248121},
   Abstract = {Adults under time pressure share with others generously, but
             with more time they act more selfishly. In the current
             study, we investigated whether young children already
             operate in this same way, and, if so, whether this changes
             over the preschool and early school age years. We tested 144
             children in three age groups (3-, 5-, and 7-year olds) in a
             one-shot dictator game: Children were given nine stickers
             and had the possibility to share stickers with another child
             who was absent. Children in the Time Pressure condition were
             instructed to share quickly, whereas children in the Delay
             condition were instructed to take time and consider their
             decision carefully. Across ages, children in the Time
             Pressure condition shared significantly more stickers than
             children in the Delay condition. Moreover, the longer
             children waited, the less they shared. Thus, children, like
             adults, are more prosocial when acting spontaneously than
             after considering their decision more carefully.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0248121},
   Key = {fds355723}
}

@article{fds356997,
   Author = {Li, L and Britvan, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children conform more to norms than to
             preferences.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {e0251228},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251228},
   Abstract = {As members of cultural groups, humans continually adhere to
             social norms and conventions. Researchers have hypothesized
             that even young children are motivated to act
             conventionally, but support for this hypothesis has been
             indirect and open to other interpretations. To further test
             this hypothesis, we invited 3.5-year-old children (N = 104)
             to help set up items for a tea party. Children first
             indicated which items they preferred but then heard an
             informant (either an adult or another child) endorse other
             items in terms of either conventional norms or personal
             preferences. Children conformed (i.e., overrode their own
             preference to follow the endorsement) more when the
             endorsements were framed as norms than when they were framed
             as preferences, and this was the case whether the informant
             was an adult or another child. The priority of norms even
             when stated by another child opposes the interpretation that
             children only conformed in deference to adult authority.
             These findings suggest that children are motivated to act
             conventionally, possibly as an adaptation for living in
             cultural groups.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0251228},
   Key = {fds356997}
}

@article{fds355827,
   Author = {Li, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {On the moral functions of language},
   Journal = {Social Cognition},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {99-116},
   Publisher = {Guilford Press},
   Year = {2021},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2021.39.1.99},
   Abstract = {Previous comparisons of language and morality have taken a
             cognitively internalist (i.e., within-minds) perspective. We
             take a socially externalist (i.e., between-minds)
             perspective, viewing both language and morality as forms of
             social action. During human evolution, social cognitive
             adaptations for cooperation evolved, including cooperative
             communication (social acts to mentally coordinate with
             others for common goals) and social normativity (social acts
             to regulate cooperative social relationships). As human
             cooperation scaled up in complexity, cooperative
             communication and social normativity scaled up as well,
             leading to the development of culturally elaborated forms of
             language and morality. Language facilitates all aspects of
             morality and is even necessary for certain aspects. Humans
             use language to (1) initiate, (2) preserve, (3) revise, and
             (4) act on morality in ways such as forming joint
             commitments, teaching norms, modifying social realities, and
             engaging in moral reason-giving.},
   Doi = {10.1521/soco.2021.39.1.99},
   Key = {fds355827}
}

@article{fds352447,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Early Ontogeny of Reason Giving},
   Journal = {Child Development Perspectives},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {215-220},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12384},
   Abstract = {The key context within which preschool children learn to
             justify beliefs with reasons is collaborative
             problem-solving and decision-making with peers, including in
             the moral domain, in which they engage with another coequal
             mind in a cooperative spirit. Evidence for this proposal
             comes from recent studies in which children demonstrated
             sensitivity to the common ground assumptions they shared
             with their peer partners in decision-making, as well as an
             ability to provide reasons relevant to their shared
             understanding. Training studies suggest that discourse with
             others about reasons for beliefs provides children with the
             appropriate learning experiences. Internalizing this
             communicative process may be crucial for individual
             deliberative reasoning.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdep.12384},
   Key = {fds352447}
}

@article{fds351010,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Human children, but not great apes, become socially closer
             by sharing an experience in common ground.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {199},
   Pages = {104930},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104930},
   Abstract = {To create social closeness, humans engage in a variety of
             social activities centered around shared experiences. Even
             simply watching the same video side by side creates social
             closeness in adults and children. However, perhaps
             surprisingly, a similar psychological mechanism was recently
             shown in great apes. Here we asked whether the process by
             which this social closeness is created is the same for
             children and great apes. Each participant entered a room to
             see an experimenter (E1) watching a video. In one condition,
             E1 looked to the participant at the start of the video to
             establish common ground that they were watching the video
             together. In another condition, E1 did not look to the
             participant in this way so that the participant knew they
             were watching the same video, but the participant did not
             know whether E1 was aware of this as well, so there was no
             common ground (E1 looked to the participant later in the
             procedure). Children, but not great apes, approached the
             experimenter faster after the common ground condition,
             suggesting that although both humans and great apes create
             social closeness by co-attending to something in close
             proximity, creating social closeness by sharing experiences
             in common ground may be a uniquely human social-cognitive
             process.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104930},
   Key = {fds351010}
}

@article{fds351568,
   Author = {Sánchez-Amaro, A and Duguid, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Do 7-year-old children understand social
             leverage?},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {199},
   Pages = {104963},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104963},
   Abstract = {Individuals with an advantageous position during a
             negotiation possess leverage over their partners. Several
             studies with adults have investigated how leverage can
             influence the coordination strategies of individuals when
             conflicts of interest arise. In this study, we explored how
             pairs of 7-year-old children solved a coordination game
             (based on the Snowdrift scenario) when one child had
             leverage over the other child. We presented a social dilemma
             in the form of an unequal reward distribution on a rotating
             tray. The rotating tray could be accessed by both children.
             The child who waited longer to act received the best
             outcome, but if both children waited too long, they would
             lose the rewards. In addition, one child could forgo the
             access to the rotating tray for an alternative option-the
             leverage. Although children rarely used their leverage
             strategically, children with access to the alternative were
             less likely to play the social dilemma, especially when
             their leverage was larger. Furthermore, children waited
             longer to act as the leverage decreased. Finally, children
             almost never failed to coordinate. The results hint to a
             trade-off between maximizing benefits while maintaining
             long-term collaboration in complex scenarios where
             strategies such as turn taking are hard to
             implement.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104963},
   Key = {fds351568}
}

@article{fds351570,
   Author = {Li, J and Hou, W and Zhu, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The development of intent-based moral judgment and moral
             behavior in the context of indirect reciprocity: A
             cross-cultural study},
   Journal = {International Journal of Behavioral Development},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {525-533},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025420935636},
   Abstract = {The current study aimed to investigate the cultural
             differences in the developmental origins of children’s
             intent-based moral judgment and moral behavior in the
             context of indirect reciprocity. To this end, we compared
             how German and Chinese children interpret and react to
             antisocial and prosocial interactions between puppets. An
             actor puppet performed either a positive or negative act
             toward a prosocial or antisocial target puppet with the
             intention to cause harm or not; 197 three and five-year-old
             children participated as a third party and were asked to
             judge the actor puppet’s behavior and to distribute
             stickers. Results showed that 3-year-old Chinese children
             were able to take intention and context into account when
             making moral judgments and distributing resources, whereas
             German children did not show sensitivity to intention until
             the age of 5. These findings suggest that culture may
             mediate children’s intent-based moral judgment and moral
             behavior in the context of indirect reciprocity.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0165025420935636},
   Key = {fds351570}
}

@article{fds351569,
   Author = {Ulber, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's prosocial responses toward peers and adults
             in two social contexts.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {198},
   Pages = {104888},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104888},
   Abstract = {Young children help and share with others, but little is
             known about the "how" and "who" of this early prosocial
             behavior. In the current study, we compared 2- and
             3-year-old children's (N = 203; 101 girls) prosocial
             behavior of sharing and helping. We asked whether the
             process was different (a) if the social partner was an adult
             or a same-age peer and (b) if the child was actively
             interacting and engaged with the partner or not. The highest
             prosocial responses were found in bilateral joint tasks such
             as sharing the spoils after a collaborative effort and
             helping a partner finish a mutual activity. Prosocial
             responses were lower in unilateral autonomous tasks such as
             assisting another person in opening a locked box and
             distributing a windfall of resources. Children did not show
             an overall preference for helping or sharing with adults
             versus peers except that they were more likely to support a
             peer than an adult in an instrumental helping task.
             Together, these findings suggest that toddlers' early
             prosocial skills and motivations are more sensitive to how
             toddlers are engaged with a partner than to who that partner
             is, implying that children have a nondiscriminatory general
             inclination to benefit others, especially in bilateral
             interactive scenarios.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104888},
   Key = {fds351569}
}

@article{fds353880,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children show positive emotions when seeing someone
             get the help they deserve},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {56},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100935},
   Abstract = {Little is known about the underlying emotional bases of
             children's prosociality. Here we engaged 32 dyads of
             4-year-old children in a reward-collecting task at the end
             of which one child was more in need of help. An adult then
             either helped the needier child (deserving outcome) or the
             less needy child (less deserving outcome). Both children
             expressed elevated upper-body posture (positively valenced
             emotions) when the more needy (but not the less needy) child
             was helped, whether it was themselves or not. In contrast,
             both children showed decreased elevation when the less needy
             (but not the more needy) child received the help, again
             whether it was themselves or not. These results suggest that
             preschool children's prosocial emotions are regulated not
             only by sympathy for those needing help, but also by a sense
             of deservingness as determined by social
             comparison.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100935},
   Key = {fds353880}
}

@article{fds349814,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Ontogenetic Foundations of Epistemic
             Norms},
   Journal = {Episteme},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {301-315},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/epi.2019.50},
   Abstract = {In this paper, I approach epistemic norms from an
             ontogenetic point of view. I argue and present evidence that
             to understand epistemic norms - e.g., scientific norms of
             methodology and the evaluation of evidence - children must
             first develop through their social interactions with others
             three key concepts. First is the concept of belief, which
             provides the most basic distinction on which scientific
             investigations rest: the distinction between individual
             subjective perspectives and an objective reality. Second is
             the concept of reason, which in the context of science
             obligates practitioners to justify their claims to others
             with reasons by grounding them in beliefs that are
             universally shared within the community. Third is the
             concept of social norm, which is not primarily epistemic,
             but provides children with an understanding of norms as
             collective agreements. The theoretical argument is that all
             three of these concepts emerge not from just any kind of
             social interaction, but specifically from social
             interactions structured by the human species' unique
             capacities for shared intentionality.},
   Doi = {10.1017/epi.2019.50},
   Key = {fds349814}
}

@article{fds349805,
   Author = {Gopnik, A and Frankenhuis, WE and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Introduction to special issue: 'Life history and learning:
             how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and
             culture in humans and other animals'.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {375},
   Number = {1803},
   Pages = {20190489},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0489},
   Abstract = {This special issue focuses on the relationship between life
             history and learning, especially during human evolution.
             'Life history' refers to the developmental programme of an
             organism, including its period of immaturity, reproductive
             rate and timing, caregiving investment and longevity. Across
             many species an extended childhood and high caregiving
             investment appear to be correlated with particular kinds of
             plasticity and learning. Human life history is particularly
             distinctive; humans evolved an exceptionally long childhood
             and old age, and an unusually high level of caregiving
             investment, at the same time that they evolved distinctive
             capacities for cognition and culture. The contributors
             explore the relations between life history, plasticity and
             learning across a wide range of methods and populations,
             including theoretical and empirical work in biology,
             anthropology and developmental psychology. This article is
             part of the theme issue 'Life history and learning: how
             childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and
             culture in humans and other animals'.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2019.0489},
   Key = {fds349805}
}

@article{fds349806,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The adaptive origins of uniquely human sociality.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {375},
   Number = {1803},
   Pages = {20190493},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0493},
   Abstract = {Humans possess some unique social-cognitive skills and
             motivations, involving such things as joint attention,
             cooperative communication, dual-level collaboration and
             cultural learning. These are almost certainly adaptations
             for humans' especially complex sociocultural lives. The
             common assumption has been that these unique skills and
             motivations emerge in human infancy and early childhood as
             preparations for the challenges of adult life, for example,
             in collaborative foraging. In the current paper, I propose
             that the curiously early emergence of these skills in
             infancy--well before they are needed in adulthood--along
             with other pieces of evidence (such as almost exclusive use
             with adults not peers) suggests that aspects of the
             evolution of these skills represent ontogenetic adaptations
             to the unique socio-ecological challenges human infants face
             in the context of a regime of cooperative breeding and
             childcare. This article is part of the theme issue 'Life
             history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age
             shape cognition and culture in humans and other
             animals'.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2019.0493},
   Key = {fds349806}
}

@article{fds349808,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Kordt, C and Braun, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Learning Novel Skills From Iconic Gestures: A Developmental
             and Evolutionary Perspective.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {873-880},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797620921519},
   Abstract = {Cumulative cultural learning has been argued to rely on
             high-fidelity copying of other individuals' actions. Iconic
             gestures of actions have no physical effect on objects in
             the world but merely represent actions that would have an
             effect. Learning from iconic gestures thus requires paying
             close attention to the teacher's precise bodily movements-a
             prerequisite for high-fidelity copying. In three studies, we
             investigated whether 2- and 3-year-old children (<i>N</i> =
             122) and great apes (<i>N</i> = 36) learn novel skills from
             iconic gestures. When faced with a novel apparatus,
             participants watched an experimenter perform either an
             iconic gesture depicting the action necessary to open the
             apparatus or a gesture depicting a different action.
             Children, but not great apes, profited from iconic gestures,
             with older children doing so to a larger extent. These
             results suggest that high-fidelity copying abilities are
             firmly in place in humans by at least 3 years of
             age.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797620921519},
   Key = {fds349808}
}

@article{fds349807,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The development of coordination via joint expectations for
             shared benefits.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {56},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1149-1156},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000936},
   Abstract = {People frequently need to cooperate despite having strong
             self-serving motives. In the current study, pairs of 5- and
             7-year-olds (<i>N</i> = 160) faced a one-shot coordination
             problem: To benefit, children had to choose the same of 3
             reward divisions. They could not communicate or see each
             other and thus had to accurately predict each other's
             choices to succeed. One division split the rewards evenly,
             while the others each favored one child. Five-year-olds
             mostly chose the division favorable to themselves, resulting
             in coordination failure. By contrast, 7-year-olds mostly
             coordinated successfully by choosing the division that split
             the rewards equally (even though they behaved selfishly in a
             control condition in which they could choose independently).
             This suggests that by age 7, children jointly expect
             benefits to be shared among interdependent social partners
             "fairly" and that fair compromises can emanate from a
             cooperative rationality adapted for social coordination.
             (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000936},
   Key = {fds349807}
}

@article{fds349811,
   Author = {Schmelz, M and Grueneisen, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The psychological mechanisms underlying reciprocal
             prosociality in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {134},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {149-157},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000200},
   Abstract = {In both the wild and captivity, chimpanzees engage in
             reciprocal patterns of prosocial behavior. However, the
             proximate mechanisms underlying these patterns are unclear.
             In the current study, we investigated whether chimpanzees
             prefer to act prosocially toward conspecifics who have
             directly benefited them (perhaps based on an affective bond)
             or whether they simply observe the prosocial behavior of
             others in general (including indirectly to third parties)
             and preferentially interact with and behave prosocially
             toward the most prosocial individuals. We found good
             evidence for direct reciprocity but little evidence for a
             general (indirect) preference for prosocial individuals.
             These results suggest that cooperative reciprocity in
             chimpanzees may be based mostly on social-affective
             processes and direct interactions. (PsycInfo Database Record
             (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/com0000200},
   Key = {fds349811}
}

@article{fds349809,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Benziad, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees help others with what they want; children help
             them with what they need.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e12922},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12922},
   Abstract = {Humans, including young children, are strongly motivated to
             help others, even paying a cost to do so. Humans' nearest
             primate relatives, great apes, are likewise motivated to
             help others, raising the question of whether the motivations
             of humans and apes are the same. Here we compared the
             underlying motivation to help in human children and
             chimpanzees. Both species understood the situation and
             helped a conspecific in a straightforward situation.
             However, when helpers knew that what the other was
             requesting would not actually help her, only children gave
             her what she needed instead of giving her what she
             requested. These results suggest that both chimpanzees and
             human children help others but the underlying motivation for
             why they help differs. In comparison to chimpanzees, young
             children help in a paternalistic manner. The evolutionary
             hypothesis is that uniquely human socio-ecologies based on
             interdependent cooperation gave rise to uniquely human
             prosocial motivations to help others paternalistically.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12922},
   Key = {fds349809}
}

@article{fds349810,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Jurkat, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Preschoolers refer to direct and indirect evidence in their
             collaborative reasoning.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {193},
   Pages = {104806},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104806},
   Abstract = {Collaborative reasoning requires partners to evaluate
             options and the evidence for or against each option. We
             investigated whether preschoolers can explain why one option
             is best (direct reasons) and why the other option is not
             (indirect reasons), looking at both problems that have a
             correct answer and those that require choosing the better
             option. In Study 1, both age groups produced direct reasons
             equally frequently in both problems. However, 5-year-olds
             produced indirect reasons more often than 3-year-olds,
             especially when there was a correct answer. In Study 2 with
             a nonverbal task with a correct answer, 3-year-olds produced
             indirect reasons more often than in Study 1, although
             5-year-olds' indirect reasons were more efficiently stated.
             These results demonstrate that even 3-year-olds, and even
             nonverbally, can point out to a partner a fact that
             constitutes a reason for them to arrive at a correct joint
             decision.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104806},
   Key = {fds349810}
}

@article{fds349820,
   Author = {Köymen, B and O'Madagain, C and Domberg, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young Children's Ability to Produce Valid and Relevant
             Counter-Arguments.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {91},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {685-693},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13338},
   Abstract = {In collaborative problem solving, children produce and
             evaluate arguments for proposals. We investigated whether 3-
             and 5-year-olds (N = 192) can produce and evaluate
             arguments against those arguments (i.e., counter-arguments).
             In Study 1, each child within a peer dyad was privately
             given a reason to prefer one over another solution to a
             task. One child, however, was given further information that
             would refute the reasoning of their partner. Five-year-olds,
             but not 3-year-olds, identified and produced valid and
             relevant counter-arguments. In Study 2, 3-year-olds were
             given discourse training (discourse that contrasted valid
             and invalid counter-arguments) and then given the same
             problem-solving tasks. After training, 3-year-olds could
             also identify and produce valid and relevant
             counter-arguments. Thus, participating in discourse about
             reasons facilitates children's counter-argumentation.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.13338},
   Key = {fds349820}
}

@article{fds351571,
   Author = {Duguid, S and Wyman, E and Grueneisen, S and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The strategies used by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and
             children (Homo sapiens) to solve a simple coordination
             problem.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000220},
   Abstract = {One of the challenges of collaboration is to coordinate
             decisions with others, and recent theories have proposed
             that humans, in particular, evolved skills to address this
             challenge. To test this hypothesis, we compared the
             coordination abilities of 4-year-old children and
             chimpanzees with a simple coordination problem. To retrieve
             a reward from a "puzzle box," pairs of individuals were
             simply required to choose the same 1 of 4 options. If
             successful, they each received the same reward, so there
             were no conflicts of interest. Individuals were paired with
             multiple partners over time. Both species were able to
             coordinate, but there were marked differences in the way
             they did so. Children were able to coordinate quickly and
             flexibly, adjusting easily to new partners, suggesting an
             understanding of the coordination process. In contrast,
             chimpanzees took time to converge on a single solution with
             each new partner, with no gains across partners, suggesting
             that their coordination was based only on repeating
             successful past choices. Together, these results support the
             hypothesis that humans have evolved unique skills for
             coordinating decisions and actions with others in the
             pursuit of common interests. (PsycInfo Database Record (c)
             2020 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/com0000220},
   Key = {fds351571}
}

@article{fds349812,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The many faces of obligation.},
   Journal = {The Behavioral and brain sciences},
   Volume = {43},
   Pages = {e89},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x19002620},
   Abstract = {My response to the commentaries focuses on four issues: (1)
             the diversity both within and between cultures of the many
             different faces of obligation; (2) the possible evolutionary
             roots of the sense of obligation, including possible sources
             that I did not consider; (3) the possible ontogenetic roots
             of the sense of obligation, including especially children's
             understanding of groups from a third-party perspective
             (rather than through participation, as in my account); and
             (4) the relation between philosophical accounts of normative
             phenomena in general - which are pitched as not totally
             empirical - and empirical accounts such as my own. I have
             tried to distinguish comments that argue for extensions of
             the theory from those that represent genuine
             disagreement.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x19002620},
   Key = {fds349812}
}

@article{fds349813,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The role of roles in uniquely human cognition and
             sociality},
   Journal = {Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {2-19},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12223},
   Abstract = {To understand themselves as playing a social role,
             individuals must understand themselves to be contributing to
             a cooperative endeavor. Psychologically, the form of
             cooperation required is a specific type that only humans may
             possess, namely, one in which individuals form a joint or
             collective agency to pursue a common end. This begins
             ontogenetically not with the societal level but rather with
             more local collaboration between individuals. Participating
             in collaborative endeavors of this type leads young
             children, cognitively, to think in terms of different
             perspectives on a joint focus of attention - including
             ultimately an objective perspective - and to organize their
             experience in terms of a relational-thematic-narrative
             dimension. Socially, such participation leads young children
             to an understanding of self-other equivalence with mutual
             respect among collaborative partners and, ultimately, to a
             normative (i.e. moral) stance toward “we” in the
             community within which one is forming a moral role or
             identity. The dual-level structure of shared
             endeavors/realities with individual roles/perspectives is
             responsible for many aspects of the human species' most
             distinctive psychology.},
   Doi = {10.1111/jtsb.12223},
   Key = {fds349813}
}

@article{fds349815,
   Author = {Kanngiesser, P and Rossano, F and Frickel, R and Tomm, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children, but not great apes, respect ownership.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e12842},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12842},
   Abstract = {Access to and control of resources is a major source of
             costly conflicts. Animals, under some conditions, respect
             what others control and use (i.e. possession). Humans not
             only respect possession of resources, they also respect
             ownership. Ownership can be viewed as a cooperative
             arrangement, where individuals inhibit their tendency to
             take others' property on the condition that those others
             will do the same. We investigated to what degree great apes
             follow this principle, as compared to human children. We
             conducted two experiments, in which dyads of individuals
             could access the same food resources. The main test of
             respect for ownership was whether individuals would refrain
             from taking their partner's resources even when the partner
             could not immediately access and control them. Captive apes
             (N = 14 dyads) failed to respect their partner's claim on
             food resources and frequently monopolized the resources when
             given the opportunity. Human children (N = 14 dyads),
             tested with a similar apparatus and procedure, respected
             their partner's claim and made spontaneous verbal references
             to ownership. Such respect for the property of others
             highlights the uniquely cooperative nature of human
             ownership arrangements.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12842},
   Key = {fds349815}
}

@article{fds349816,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Watching a video together creates social closeness between
             children and adults.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {189},
   Pages = {104712},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104712},
   Abstract = {Human social relationships are often formed through shared
             social activities in which individuals share mental states
             about external stimuli. Previous work on joint attention has
             shown that even minimal shared experiences such as watching
             something together facilitates social closeness between
             individuals. Here, we examined whether young children
             already connect with others through joint attention. In the
             current studies, children sat next to a novel adult who
             either watched a film with them or was not able to see the
             film and read a book instead. After the video, we measured
             children's willingness (i.e., latency) to approach the
             experimenter holding out a toy. In both studies, the
             2.5-year-olds who watched the film together approached more
             quickly than the other children. These results show that
             both minimally interactive shared experiences and
             noninteractive shared experiences lead children to feel more
             comfortable with a novel adult. This suggests that joint
             attention interactions, and shared experiences in general,
             play an important role not only in children's cognitive
             development but also in their social development and the
             formation of their social relationships.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104712},
   Key = {fds349816}
}

@article{fds349817,
   Author = {Pouscoulous, N and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Early birds: Metaphor understanding in 3-year-olds},
   Journal = {Journal of Pragmatics},
   Volume = {156},
   Pages = {160-167},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2019.05.021},
   Abstract = {To assess children's cognitive capacities to understand
             (rather than explain or paraphrase) metaphors, we
             investigated how 3-year-olds (n = 36; 3;0–3;3) fare with
             novel metaphors corresponding to their world knowledge and
             linguistic competences using a behavioural choice paradigm.
             In a game, participants had to give the experimenter one of
             two objects referred to by a metaphorical expression. Unlike
             what previous literature suggests, our results indicate that
             3-year-olds are able to understand novel metaphors that are
             appropriate for their vocabulary and world knowledge, based
             on action measures rather than metalinguistic responses. We
             discuss how factors other than incompetence with pragmatic
             inferencing can explain difficulties with metaphor
             comprehension.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.pragma.2019.05.021},
   Key = {fds349817}
}

@article{fds349818,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Kachel, G and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children spontaneously recreate core properties of
             language in a new modality.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {116},
   Number = {51},
   Pages = {26072-26077},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1904871116},
   Abstract = {How the world's 6,000+ natural languages have arisen is
             mostly unknown. Yet, new sign languages have emerged
             recently among deaf people brought together in a community,
             offering insights into the dynamics of language evolution.
             However, documenting the emergence of these languages has
             mostly consisted of studying the end product; the process by
             which ad hoc signs are transformed into a structured
             communication system has not been directly observed. Here we
             show how young children create new communication systems
             that exhibit core features of natural languages in less than
             30 min. In a controlled setting, we blocked the possibility
             of using spoken language. In order to communicate novel
             messages, including abstract concepts, dyads of children
             spontaneously created novel gestural signs. Over usage,
             these signs became increasingly arbitrary and
             conventionalized. When confronted with the need to
             communicate more complex meanings, children began to
             grammatically structure their gestures. Together with
             previous work, these results suggest that children have the
             basic skills necessary, not only to acquire a natural
             language, but also to spontaneously create a new one. The
             speed with which children create these structured systems
             has profound implications for theorizing about language
             evolution, a process which is generally thought to span
             across many generations, if not millennia.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1904871116},
   Key = {fds349818}
}

@article{fds349819,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Müller, K and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Toddlers' intrinsic motivation to return help to their
             benefactor.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {188},
   Pages = {104658},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.011},
   Abstract = {A natural reaction to receiving help from someone is to help
             that person in return. In two studies, we investigated the
             developmental origins of children's motivation to return
             help. In Study 1, 18- and 24-month-old toddlers were either
             helped or not helped by an adult, and they could
             subsequently provide that adult with help or else observe
             another person providing help. We measured children's
             internal arousal, via changes in pupil dilation, both before
             and after help was provided. At both ages, children's
             internal arousal was higher when they could not help the
             adult who had previously helped them (and was lower when
             they could). On the other hand, if the adult needing help
             had not previously helped children, their internal arousal
             was equally low regardless of whether they or another person
             provided the help. Study 2 replicated this result and also
             found that if children had previously been helped but the
             person needing help was a different adult (not their
             benefactor), children's internal arousal was equally low
             regardless of whether they or another person provided the
             help. Together, these results suggest that young children
             are intrinsically motivated to return a received favor
             specifically to the previous benefactor, perhaps indicating
             a nascent sense of gratitude.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.011},
   Key = {fds349819}
}

@article{fds349821,
   Author = {Isella, M and Kanngiesser, P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's Selective Trust in Promises.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {90},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {e868-e887},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13105},
   Abstract = {There has been extensive research into the development of
             selective trust in testimony, but little is known about the
             development of selective trust in promises. The present
             research investigates children's (N = 264) selective trust
             in others' promises to help. In Study 1, 6-year-olds
             selectively trusted speakers who had previously kept a
             promise. In Study 2, 5-year-olds displayed selective trust
             for speakers who had previously kept a prosocial promise
             (promise to help). In Study 3, 5-year-olds trusted a
             speaker, who kept a prosocial promise, over a helper. These
             data suggest that from the age of 5 children show selective
             trust in others' promises using prosociality, promise
             keeping, or both to inform their judgments.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.13105},
   Key = {fds349821}
}

@article{fds349822,
   Author = {Zhang, Z and Grocke, P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The influence of intention and outcome on young children's
             reciprocal sharing.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {187},
   Pages = {104645},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.012},
   Abstract = {This study investigated the influence of underlying
             intentions and outcomes of a partner's sharing behavior on
             young children's reciprocity. We provided 3- and 5-year-old
             children with the opportunity to share with a partner
             following different treatments of a partner's intention (to
             share or not to share) that led to different outcomes
             (children got or did not get stickers from their partner).
             For the 3-year-olds, we found that the outcome of the
             previous interaction influenced how much they shared,
             whereas the intention of their partner affected how readily
             they initiated sharing in response to social cues. For the
             5-year-olds, we found that both outcome and intention
             affected how much they shared as well as how readily they
             initiated sharing. This suggests that already 3-year-olds
             are able to take into account outcome and intention
             information separately in reciprocal sharing. However, only
             5-year-olds can combine both to flexibly maintain social
             interactions without running the risk of being exploited by
             others.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.012},
   Key = {fds349822}
}

@article{fds349823,
   Author = {Hardecker, S and Buryn-Weitzel, JC and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Adult instruction limits children's flexibility in moral
             decision making.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {187},
   Pages = {104652},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.005},
   Abstract = {Children's moral behavior is guided, in part, by adults
             teaching children how to treat others. However, when
             circumstances change, such instructions may become either
             unhelpful or limiting. In the current study, 48 dyads of
             5-year-olds played a collaborative game and either (a)
             received an instruction by an adult to share the spoils of
             the game equally, (b) did not receive any instruction (but
             still chose to share equally), or (c) agreed between
             themselves on a rule to share equally. Afterward, each child
             played with a new partner who was needier or worked harder
             in his or her collaboration and so plausibly deserved more
             than just half of the spoils. Results showed that children
             who were instructed by an adult shared less with their more
             deserving partner than children who did not receive any
             instruction. Thus, moral instruction by adults may, in some
             circumstances, make children more rigid in their moral
             decisions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.005},
   Key = {fds349823}
}

@article{fds349824,
   Author = {Kanngiesser, P and Rossano, F and Zeidler, H and Haun, D and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children's respect for ownership across diverse
             societies.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {2286-2298},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000787},
   Abstract = {Ownership is a cornerstone of many human societies and can
             be understood as a cooperative arrangement, where
             individuals refrain from taking each other's property.
             Owners can thus trust others to respect their property even
             in their absence. We investigated this principle in 5- to
             7-year-olds (N = 152) from 4 diverse societies. Children
             participated in a resource task with a peer-partner, where
             we established ownership by assigning children to one side
             or the other of an apparatus and by marking resources with
             colors to help children keep track of them. When retrieving
             resources in the partner's presence, the majority of
             children took their own things and respected what belonged
             to their partner. A proportion of children in all societies
             also respected ownership in their partner's absence,
             although the strength of respect varied considerably across
             societies. We discuss implications for the development of
             ownership concepts and possible explanations for societal
             differences. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000787},
   Key = {fds349824}
}

@article{fds349825,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Völter, CJ and Vonau, V and Hanus, D and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees use observed temporal directionality to learn
             novel causal relations.},
   Journal = {Primates; journal of primatology},
   Volume = {60},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {517-524},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00754-9},
   Abstract = {We investigated whether chimpanzees use the temporal
             sequence of external events to determine causation.
             Seventeen chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) witnessed a human
             experimenter press a button in two different conditions.
             When she pressed the "causal button" the delivery of juice
             and a sound immediately followed (cause-then-effect). In
             contrast, she pressed the "non-causal button" only after the
             delivery of juice and sound (effect-then-cause). When given
             the opportunity to produce the desired juice delivery
             themselves, the chimpanzees preferentially pressed the
             causal button, i.e., the one that preceded the effect.
             Importantly, they did so in their first test trial and even
             though both buttons were equally associated with juice
             delivery. This outcome suggests that chimpanzees, like human
             children, do not rely solely on their own actions to make
             use of novel causal relations, but they can learn causal
             sequences based on observation alone. We discuss these
             findings in relation to the literature on causal inferences
             as well as associative learning.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10329-019-00754-9},
   Key = {fds349825}
}

@article{fds349826,
   Author = {Mammen, M and Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's reasoning with peers and parents about moral
             dilemmas.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {2324-2335},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000807},
   Abstract = {Children encounter moral norms in several different social
             contexts. Often it is in hierarchically structured
             interactions with parents or other adults, but sometimes it
             is in more symmetrically structured interactions with peers.
             Our question was whether children's discussions of moral
             norms differ in these two contexts. Consequently, we had 4-
             and 6-year-old children (<i>N</i> = 72) reason about moral
             dilemmas with their mothers or peers. Both age groups
             opposed their partner's views and explicitly justified their
             own views more often with peers than with mothers. Mothers
             adapted their discussions to the cognitive levels of their
             children (e.g., focused more on the abstract moral norms
             with 6-year-old children than with 4-year-old children), but
             almost always with a pedagogical intent. Our results suggest
             that with mothers, moral judgments are experienced mostly as
             non-negotiable dictums, but with coequal peers they are
             experienced more as personal beliefs that can be actively
             negotiated. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000807},
   Key = {fds349826}
}

@article{fds349827,
   Author = {Domberg, A and Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children choose to reason with partners who submit to
             reason},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {52},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100824},
   Abstract = {When reasoning with others, the reasons used in an exchange
             can have varying degrees of quality, irrespective of the
             facts under discussion. Partners often evaluate one
             another's evaluation of reasons – one another's reasoning.
             Can children evaluate their partner's judgment of the
             quality of reasons independent of objective truth? 5- and
             7-year-olds (N = 122) chose among two partners for
             cooperation. In the experimental condition, one acceded to a
             good reason, the other to a poor reason. In the control
             condition, each agreed to a different good reason.
             Crucially, in both conditions, both partners arrived at the
             wrong conclusion. Results suggested that 7-year-olds, and
             5-year-olds to a lesser degree, chose the partner who
             endorsed the good reason in the experimental condition, but
             showed no preference for partners in the control condition.
             Thus, young children distinguish good from poor reasons,
             even if neither leads to success, and choose partners who do
             the same.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100824},
   Key = {fds349827}
}

@article{fds349828,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Respect Defended.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {716-717},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.06.001},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2019.06.001},
   Key = {fds349828}
}

@article{fds349829,
   Author = {Kachel, U and Svetlova, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Three- and 5-year-old children's understanding of how to
             dissolve a joint commitment.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {184},
   Pages = {34-47},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.03.008},
   Abstract = {When young children form a joint commitment with a partner,
             they understand that this agreement generates obligations.
             In this study, we investigated whether young children
             understand that joint commitments, and their associated
             obligations, may likewise be dissolved by agreement. The
             participants (3- and 5-year-olds; N = 144) formed a
             joint commitment with a puppet to play a collaborative game.
             In one condition, the puppet asked permission to break off
             and the children agreed; in a second condition, the puppet
             notified the children of his or her leaving; and in a third
             condition, the puppet just left abruptly. Children at both
             ages protested more and waited longer for the puppet's
             return (and said that the puppet deserved scolding and no
             prize at the end) when the puppet left abruptly than in the
             other two conditions (with "asking permission" leading to
             the least protest of all). Overall, 3-year-olds protested
             more, and waited longer for the partner's return, than
             5-year-olds. Preschool children understand that the
             obligations of a joint commitment may be dissolved by
             agreement or, to a lesser degree, by notification.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2019.03.008},
   Key = {fds349829}
}

@article{fds332984,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Thirty years of great ape gestures.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {461-469},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1167-1},
   Abstract = {We and our colleagues have been doing studies of great ape
             gestural communication for more than 30 years. Here we
             attempt to spell out what we have learned. Some aspects of
             the process have been reliably established by multiple
             researchers, for example, its intentional structure and its
             sensitivity to the attentional state of the recipient. Other
             aspects are more controversial. We argue here that it is a
             mistake to assimilate great ape gestures to the
             species-typical displays of other mammals by claiming that
             they are fixed action patterns, as there are many
             differences, including the use of attention-getters. It is
             also a mistake, we argue, to assimilate great ape gestures
             to human gestures by claiming that they are used
             referentially and declaratively in a human-like manner, as
             apes' "pointing" gesture has many limitations and they do
             not gesture iconically. Great ape gestures constitute a
             unique form of primate communication with their own unique
             qualities.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-018-1167-1},
   Key = {fds332984}
}

@article{fds349830,
   Author = {Wolf, W and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Visually attending to a video together facilitates great ape
             social closeness.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {286},
   Number = {1907},
   Pages = {20190488},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0488},
   Abstract = {Humans create social closeness with one another through a
             variety of shared social activities in which they align
             their emotions or mental states towards an external stimulus
             such as dancing to music together, playing board games
             together or even engaging in minimal shared experiences such
             as watching a movie together. Although these specific
             behaviours would seem to be uniquely human, it is unclear
             whether the underlying psychology is unique to the species,
             or if other species might possess some form of this
             psychological mechanism as well. Here we show that great
             apes who have visually attended to a video together with a
             human (study 1) and a conspecific (study 2) subsequently
             approach that individual faster (study 1) or spend more time
             in their proximity (study 2) than when they had attended to
             something different. Our results suggest that one of the
             most basic mechanisms of human social bonding-feeling closer
             to those with whom we act or attend together-is present in
             both humans and great apes, and thus has deeper evolutionary
             roots than previously suspected.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2019.0488},
   Key = {fds349830}
}

@article{fds349831,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Eighteen-Month-Old Infants Correct Non-Conforming Actions by
             Others.},
   Journal = {Infancy : the official journal of the International Society
             on Infant Studies},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {613-635},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/infa.12292},
   Abstract = {At around their third birthday, children begin to enforce
             social norms on others impersonally, often using generic
             normative language, but little is known about the
             developmental building blocks of this abstract norm
             understanding. Here, we investigate whether even toddlers
             show signs of enforcing on others interpersonally how "we"
             do things. In an initial dyad, 18-month-old infants learnt a
             simple game-like action from an adult. In two experiments,
             the adult either engaged infants in a normative interactive
             activity (stressing that this is the way "we" do it) or, as
             a non-normative control, marked the same action as
             idiosyncratic, based on individual preference. In a test
             dyad, infants had the opportunity to spontaneously intervene
             when a puppet partner performed an alternative action.
             Infants intervened, corrected, and directed the puppet more
             in the normative than in the non-normative conditions. These
             findings suggest that, during the second year of life,
             infants develop second-personal normative expectations about
             their partner's behavior ("You should do X!") in social
             interactions, thus making an important step toward
             understanding the normative structure of human cultural
             activities. These simple normative expectations will later
             be scaled up to group-minded and abstract social
             norms.},
   Doi = {10.1111/infa.12292},
   Key = {fds349831}
}

@article{fds349832,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's Sense of Fairness as Equal Respect.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {454-463},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.001},
   Abstract = {One influential view holds that children's sense of fairness
             emerges at age 8 and is rooted in the development of an
             aversion to unequal resource distributions. Here, we suggest
             two amendments to this view. First, we argue and present
             evidence that children's sense of fairness emerges already
             at age 3 in (and only in) the context of collaborative
             activities. This is because, in our theoretical view,
             collaboration creates a sense of equal respect among
             partners. Second, we argue and present evidence that
             children's judgments about what is fair are essentially
             judgments about the social meaning of the distributive act;
             for example, children accept unequal distributions if the
             procedure gave everyone an equal chance (so-called
             distributive justice). Children thus respond to unequal (and
             other) distributions not based on material concerns, but
             rather based on interpersonal concerns: they want equal
             respect.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.001},
   Key = {fds349832}
}

@article{fds349833,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The moral psychology of obligation.},
   Journal = {The Behavioral and brain sciences},
   Volume = {43},
   Pages = {e56},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x19001742},
   Abstract = {Although psychologists have paid scant attention to the
             sense of obligation as a distinctly human motivation, moral
             philosophers have identified two of its key features: First,
             it has a peremptory, demanding force, with a kind of
             coercive quality, and second, it is often tied to
             agreement-like social interactions (e.g., promises) in which
             breaches prompt normative protest, on the one side, and
             apologies, excuses, justifications, and guilt on the other.
             Drawing on empirical research in comparative and
             developmental psychology, I provide here a psychological
             foundation for these unique features by showing that the
             human sense of obligation is intimately connected
             developmentally with the formation of a shared agent "we,"
             which not only directs collaborative efforts but also
             self-regulates them. Thus, children's sense of obligation is
             first evident inside, but not outside, of collaborative
             activities structured by joint agency with a partner, and it
             is later evident in attitudes toward in-group, but not
             out-group, members connected by collective agency. When you
             and I voluntarily place our fate in one another's hands in
             interdependent collaboration - scaled up to our lives
             together in an interdependent cultural group - this
             transforms the instrumental pressure that individuals feel
             when pursuing individual goals into the pressure that "we"
             put on me (who needs to preserve my cooperative identity in
             this "we") to live up to our shared expectations: a we > me
             self-regulation. The human sense of obligation may therefore
             be seen as a kind of self-conscious motivation.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x19001742},
   Key = {fds349833}
}

@article{fds349834,
   Author = {Knofe, H and Engelmann, J and Tomasello, M and Herrmann,
             E},
   Title = {Chimpanzees monopolize and children take turns in a limited
             resource problem.},
   Journal = {Scientific reports},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {7597},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44096-4},
   Abstract = {Competition over scarce resources is common across the
             animal kingdom. Here we investigate the strategies of
             chimpanzees and children in a limited resource problem. Both
             species were presented with a tug-of-war apparatus in which
             each individual in a dyad received a tool to access a
             reward, but tools could not be used simultaneously. We
             assessed the equality of tool use as well as the frequency
             of turn taking. Both species managed to overcome this
             conflict of interest but used different strategies to do so.
             While there was substantial variation in chimpanzee
             behaviour, monopolization was the common course of action:
             tool use was asymmetric with individual chimpanzees
             monopolizing the resource. In children, turn-taking emerged
             as the dominant strategy: tool use was symmetric and
             children alternated access to the tool at a high rate. These
             results suggest that while both species possess strategies
             for solving limited resource problems, humans might have
             evolved species unique motivations and socio-cognitive
             skills for dealing with such conflicts of
             interest.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41598-019-44096-4},
   Key = {fds349834}
}

@article{fds349835,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) coordinate by communicating
             in a collaborative problem-solving task.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {286},
   Number = {1901},
   Pages = {20190408},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0408},
   Abstract = {Successful collaboration often relies on individuals'
             capacity to communicate with each other. Despite extensive
             research on chimpanzee communication, there is little
             evidence that chimpanzees are capable, without extensive
             human training, of regulating collaborative activities via
             communication. This study investigated whether pairs of
             chimpanzees were capable of communicating to ensure
             coordination during collaborative problem-solving. The
             chimpanzee pairs needed two tools to extract fruits from an
             apparatus. The communicator in each pair could see the
             location of the tools (hidden in one of two boxes), whereas
             only the recipient could open the boxes. The subjects were
             first successfully tested for their capacity to understand
             the pointing gestures of a human who indicated the location
             of the tools. In a subsequent conspecifics test, the
             communicator increasingly communicated the tools' location,
             by approaching the baited box and giving the key needed to
             open it to the recipients. The recipient used these signals
             and obtained the tools, transferring one of the tools to the
             communicator so that the pair could collaborate in obtaining
             the fruits. The study suggests that chimpanzees have the
             necessary socio-cognitive skills to naturally develop a
             simple communicative strategy to ensure coordination in a
             collaborative task.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2019.0408},
   Key = {fds349835}
}

@article{fds329385,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Müller, K and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The relation between young children's physiological arousal
             and their motivation to help others.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologia},
   Volume = {126},
   Pages = {113-119},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.010},
   Abstract = {Children are motivated to help others from an early age.
             However, little is known about the internal biological
             mechanisms underlying their motivation to help. Here, we
             compiled data from five separate studies in which children,
             ranging in age from 18 months to 5.5 years, witnessed an
             adult needing help. In all studies, we assessed both (1)
             children's internal physiological arousal via changes in
             their pupil dilation, and (2) the latency and likelihood of
             them providing help. The results showed that the greater the
             baseline-corrected change in children's internal arousal in
             response to witnessing the need situation, the faster and
             more likely children were to help the adult. This was not
             the case for the baseline measure of children's tonic
             arousal state. Together, these results suggest that
             children's propensity to help is systematically related to
             their physiological arousal after they witness others
             needing help. This sheds new light on the biological
             mechanisms underlying not only young children's social
             perception but also their prosocial motivation more
             generally.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.010},
   Key = {fds329385}
}

@article{fds349836,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Natural reference: A phylo- and ontogenetic perspective on
             the comprehension of iconic gestures and
             vocalizations.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {e12757},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12757},
   Abstract = {The recognition of iconic correspondence between signal and
             referent has been argued to bootstrap the acquisition and
             emergence of language. Here, we study the ontogeny, and to
             some extent the phylogeny, of the ability to spontaneously
             relate iconic signals, gestures, and/or vocalizations, to
             previous experience. Children at 18, 24, and 36 months of
             age (N = 216) and great apes (N = 13) interacted with
             two apparatuses, each comprising a distinct action and
             sound. Subsequently, an experimenter mimicked either the
             action, the sound, or both in combination to refer to one of
             the apparatuses. Experiments 1 and 2 found no spontaneous
             comprehension in great apes and in 18-month-old children. At
             24 months of age, children were successful with a composite
             vocalization-gesture signal but not with either vocalization
             or gesture alone. At 36 months, children succeeded both
             with a composite vocalization-gesture signal and with
             gesture alone, but not with vocalization alone. In general,
             gestures were understood better compared to vocalizations.
             Experiment 4 showed that gestures were understood
             irrespective of how children learned about the corresponding
             action (through observation or self-experience). This
             pattern of results demonstrates that iconic signals can be a
             powerful way to establish reference in the absence of
             language, but they are not trivial for children to
             comprehend and not all iconic signals are created
             equal.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12757},
   Key = {fds349836}
}

@article{fds349837,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Engelmann, JM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children engage in competitive altruism.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {179},
   Pages = {176-189},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.008},
   Abstract = {Humans cultivate their reputations as good cooperators,
             sometimes even competing with group mates, to appear most
             cooperative to individuals during the process of selecting
             partners. To investigate the ontogenetic origins of such
             "competitive altruism," we presented 5- and 8-year-old
             children with a dyadic sharing game in which both children
             simultaneously decided how many rewards to share with each
             other. The children were either observed by a third-person
             peer or not. In addition, the children either knew that one
             of them would be picked for a subsequent collaborative game
             or had no such knowledge. We found that by 8 years of age,
             children were more generous in the sharing game not only
             when their behavior was observed by a third party but also
             when it could affect their chances of being chosen for a
             subsequent game. This is the first demonstration of
             competitive altruism in young children, and as such it
             underscores the important role of partner choice (and
             individual awareness of the process) in encouraging human
             cooperation from an early age.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.008},
   Key = {fds349837}
}

@article{fds349838,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children use rules to coordinate in a social
             dilemma.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {179},
   Pages = {362-374},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.001},
   Abstract = {Humans are frequently required to coordinate their actions
             in social dilemmas (e.g. when one of two drivers has to
             yield for the other at an intersection). This is commonly
             achieved by individuals following communally known rules
             that prescribe how people should behave. From relatively
             early in development, children swiftly pick up the rules of
             their culture and even start creating game rules among
             peers. Thus far, however, little is known about children's
             abilities create rules to regulate their own interactions in
             social dilemma situations in which individuals' interests
             are partially in conflict. Here, we repeatedly selected
             dyads of children (5- and 8-year-olds, N = 144) at
             random from a group and presented them with a chicken game -
             a social dilemma in which individuals have conflicting
             motives but coordination is required to avoid mutual
             failure. In game breaks, groups reconvened and had the
             opportunity to think of additional game rules. Eight- but
             not five-year-olds readily came up with and agreed upon
             impartial rules to guide their subsequent game behavior (but
             only after adult prompting). Moreover, when playing by the
             self-made rules, children achieved higher payoffs, had fewer
             conflicts, and coordinated with greater efficiency than when
             playing without a rule - which mimics the functional
             consequences of rules on a societal level. These findings
             suggest that by at least age 8, children are capable of
             using rules to independently self-regulate potential
             conflicts of interest with peers.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.001},
   Key = {fds349838}
}

@article{fds349839,
   Author = {Rapp, DJ and Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's reputational strategies in a peer group
             context.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {329-336},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000639},
   Abstract = {Reputational concerns are known to promote cooperation.
             Individuals regularly act more prosocially when their
             behavior is observable by others. Here, we investigate 4-
             and 5-year-old (N = 144) children's reputational strategies
             in a competitive group setting. The aim of the current study
             was to explore whether children's sharing behavior is
             affected by the future possibility of being singled out
             publicly as the most generous or, alternatively, the least
             generous member of the group. Children were told that they
             could share stickers with other children and that the
             picture of either the (1) most generous or (2) least
             generous donor would be displayed publicly. In both
             conditions, children shared significantly more than in a
             control condition. Moreover, 5-year-old, but not 4-year-old
             children's sharing was affected more by the possibility of
             being presented as the most generous than being presented as
             the least generous member of the group. This study is the
             first to show that children as young as 4 invest in their
             future reputation and that by age 5 children flexibly apply
             different reputational strategies depending on context.
             (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000639},
   Key = {fds349839}
}

@article{fds349840,
   Author = {Kachel, U and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {3- and 5-year-old children's adherence to explicit and
             implicit joint commitments.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {80-88},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000632},
   Abstract = {The problem with collaboration is that there are temptations
             to defect. Explicit joint commitments are designed to
             mitigate some of the risks, but people also feel committed
             to others implicitly when they both know together that they
             each hold the other's fate in their hands. In the current
             study, pairs of 3-year-old and 5-year-old children (<i>N</i>
             = 192) played a collaborative game. One child was offered
             individual rewards (bribed) to opt out of the collaboration.
             In 3 different conditions, the level of the commitment was
             manipulated. Three-year-old children were more likely to
             resist the bribes when there was an explicit joint
             commitment to the partner than when they were only playing
             in parallel, with their reactions to an implicit commitment
             falling in between. Five-year-olds were more likely to
             resist bribes in both the implicit and explicit commitment
             conditions than in the no-commitment condition. Thus,
             children at both ages showed some level of commitment to a
             collaborative partner in the face of bribes to defect, but
             only 5-year-olds clearly appreciated that a common-ground
             understanding of interdependence between partners generates
             an implicit commitment or obligation. (PsycINFO Database
             Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000632},
   Key = {fds349840}
}

@article{fds349841,
   Author = {Grocke, P and Rossano, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Preschoolers consider (absent) others when choosing a
             distribution procedure.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {e0221186},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221186},
   Abstract = {This study investigated how the presence of others and
             anticipated distributions for self influence children's
             fairness-related decisions in two different socio-moral
             contexts. In the first part, three- and five-year-old
             children (N = 120) decided between a fair and an unfair
             wheel of fortune to allocate resources (procedural justice).
             In the second part, they directly chose between two
             distributions of resources (distributive justice). While
             making a decision, each child was either observed by the
             affected group members (public), alone (private), or no
             others were introduced (non-social control). Children choose
             the fair option more often when others were affected
             (independently of their presence) only in the procedural
             justice task. These results suggest that using a fair
             procedure to distribute resources allows young preschoolers
             to overcome selfish tendencies.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0221186},
   Key = {fds349841}
}

@article{fds349842,
   Author = {John, M and Duguid, S and Tomasello, M and Melis,
             AP},
   Title = {How chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) share the spoils with
             collaborators and bystanders.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {e0222795},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0222795},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees hunt cooperatively in the wild, but the factors
             influencing food sharing after the hunt are not well
             understood. In an experimental study, groups of three
             captive chimpanzees obtained a monopolizable food resource,
             either via two individuals cooperating (with the third as
             bystander) or via one individual acting alone alongside two
             bystanders. The individual that obtained the resource first
             retained most of the food but the other two individuals
             attempted to obtain food from the "captor" by begging. We
             found the main predictor of the overall amount of food
             obtained by bystanders was proximity to the food at the
             moment it was obtained by the captor. Whether or not an
             individual had cooperated to obtain the food had no effect.
             Interestingly, however, cooperators begged more from captors
             than did bystanders, suggesting that they were more
             motivated or had a greater expectation to obtain food. These
             results suggest that while chimpanzee captors in cooperative
             hunting may not reward cooperative participation directly,
             cooperators may influence sharing behavior through increased
             begging.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0222795},
   Key = {fds349842}
}

@article{fds366586,
   Author = {Sánchez-Amaro, A and Duguid, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees and children avoid mutual defection in a social
             dilemma},
   Journal = {Evolution and Human Behavior},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {46-54},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.07.004},
   Abstract = {Cooperation often comes with the temptation to defect and
             benefit at the cost of others. This tension between
             cooperation and defection is best captured in social
             dilemmas like the Prisoner's Dilemma. Adult humans have
             specific strategies to maintain cooperation during
             Prisoner's Dilemma interactions. Yet, little is known about
             the ontogenetic and phylogenetic origins of human
             decision-making strategies in conflict scenarios. To shed
             light on this question, we compared the strategies used by
             chimpanzees and 5-year old children to overcome a social
             dilemma. In our task, waiting for the partner to act first
             produced the best results for the subject. Alternatively,
             they could mutually cooperate and divide the rewards. Our
             findings indicate that the two species differed
             substantially in their strategies to solve the task.
             Chimpanzees became more strategic across the study period by
             waiting longer to act in the social dilemma. Children
             developed a more efficient strategy of taking turns to
             reciprocate their rewards. Moreover, children used specific
             types of communication to coordinate with their partners.
             These results suggest that while both species behaved
             strategically to overcome a conflict situation, only
             children engaged in active cooperation to solve a social
             dilemma.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.07.004},
   Key = {fds366586}
}

@article{fds349843,
   Author = {Stengelin, R and Grueneisen, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Why should I trust you? Investigating young children's
             spontaneous mistrust in potential deceivers},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {48},
   Pages = {146-154},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.08.006},
   Abstract = {Children must learn not to trust everyone to avoid being
             taken advantage of. In the current study, 5- and 7-year-old
             children were paired with a partner whose incentives were
             either congruent (cooperative condition) or conflicting
             (competitive condition) with theirs. Children of both ages
             were more likely to mistrust information spontaneously
             provided by the competitive than the cooperative partner,
             showing a capacity for detecting contextual effects on
             incentives. However, a high proportion of children, even at
             age 7, initially trusted the competitive partner. After
             being misled once, almost all children mistrusted the
             partner on a second trial irrespective of the partner's
             incentives. These results demonstrate that while even school
             age children are mostly trusting, they are only beginning to
             spontaneously consider other's incentives when interpreting
             the truthfulness of their utterances. However, after
             receiving false information only once they immediately
             switch to an untrusting attitude.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.08.006},
   Key = {fds349843}
}

@article{fds349844,
   Author = {Siposova, B and Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M},
   Title = {Communicative eye contact signals a commitment to cooperate
             for young children.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {179},
   Pages = {192-201},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.010},
   Abstract = {Making commitments to cooperate facilitates cooperation.
             There is a long-standing theoretical debate about how
             promissory obligations come into existence, and whether
             linguistic acts (such as saying "I promise") are a necessary
             part of the process. To inform this debate we experimentally
             investigated whether even minimal, nonverbal behavior can be
             taken as a commitment to cooperate, as long as it is
             communicative. Five- to 7-year-old children played a Stag
             Hunt coordination game in which they needed to decide
             whether to cooperate or play individually. During the
             decision-making phase, children's partner made either
             ostensive, communicative eye contact or looked
             non-communicatively at them. In Study 1 we found that
             communicative looks produced an expectation of collaboration
             in children. In Study 2 we found that children in the
             communicative look condition normatively protested when
             their partner did not cooperate, thus showing an
             understanding of the communicative looks as a commitment to
             cooperate. This is the first experimental evidence, in
             adults or children, that in the right context,
             communicative, but not non-communicative, looks can signal a
             commitment.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.010},
   Key = {fds349844}
}

@article{fds366587,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The normative turn in early moral development},
   Journal = {Human Development},
   Volume = {61},
   Number = {4-5},
   Pages = {248-263},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000492802},
   Abstract = {The Cooperation Theory of moral development starts from the
             premise that morality is a special form of cooperation.
             Before 3 years of age, children help and share with others
             prosocially, and they collaborate with others in ways that
             foster a sense of equally deserving partners. But then, at
             around the age of 3, their social interactions are
             transformed by an emerging understanding of, and respect
             for, normative standards. Three-year-olds become capable of
             making and respecting joint commitments, treating
             collaborative partners fairly, enforcing social norms, and
             feeling guilty when they violate any of these. The almost
             simultaneous emergence of a normative attitude in all of
             these interactional contexts demands explanation. We suggest
             a transactional causal model: the maturation of capacities
             for shared intentionality (adaptations for cultural life)
             makes possible new forms of cooperative social interaction,
             and these new forms of cooperative social interaction foster
             and guide moral development.},
   Doi = {10.1159/000492802},
   Key = {fds366587}
}

@article{fds326700,
   Author = {Kachel, U and Svetlova, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Three-Year-Olds' Reactions to a Partner's Failure to Perform
             Her Role in a Joint Commitment.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {89},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1691-1703},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12816},
   Abstract = {When children make a joint commitment to collaborate,
             obligations are created. Pairs of 3-year-old children
             (N = 144) made a joint commitment to play a game. In three
             different conditions the game was interrupted in the middle
             either because: (a) the partner child intentionally
             defected, (b) the partner child was ignorant about how to
             play, or (c) the apparatus broke. The subject child reacted
             differently in the three cases, protesting normatively
             against defection (with emotional arousal and later
             tattling), teaching when the partner seemed to be ignorant,
             or simply blaming the apparatus when it broke. These results
             suggest that 3-year-old children are competent in making
             appropriate normative evaluations of intentions and
             obligations of collaborative partners.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12816},
   Key = {fds326700}
}

@article{fds333648,
   Author = {Kachel, G and Moore, R and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds use adults' but not peers'
             points.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {e12660},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12660},
   Abstract = {In the current study, 24- to 27-month-old children (N = 37)
             used pointing gestures in a cooperative object choice task
             with either peer or adult partners. When indicating the
             location of a hidden toy, children pointed equally
             accurately for adult and peer partners but more often for
             adult partners. When choosing from one of three hiding
             places, children used adults' pointing to find a hidden toy
             significantly more often than they used peers'. In
             interaction with peers, children's choice behavior was at
             chance level. These results suggest that toddlers ascribe
             informative value to adults' but not peers' pointing
             gestures, and highlight the role of children's social
             expectations in their communicative development.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12660},
   Key = {fds333648}
}

@article{fds332985,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Great Apes and Human Development: A Personal
             History},
   Journal = {Child Development Perspectives},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {189-193},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12281},
   Abstract = {In this article, I recount my history of research with great
             apes. From the beginning, the idea was to compare apes to
             human children, with an eye to discovering facts relevant to
             describing and explaining processes of human development.
             The research went through three more or less distinct
             stages, focusing on communication and social learning,
             social cognition and theory of mind, and cooperation and
             shared intentionality. I conclude by identifying problems
             and prospects for comparative research in developmental
             psychology.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdep.12281},
   Key = {fds332985}
}

@article{fds349845,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {HOW WE LEARNED TO PUT OUR FATE IN ONE ANOTHER'S HANDS THE
             ORIGINS OF MORALITY},
   Journal = {SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN},
   Volume = {319},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {70-75},
   Publisher = {SPRINGER},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0918-70},
   Doi = {10.1038/scientificamerican0918-70},
   Key = {fds349845}
}

@article{fds337395,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared
             intentionality account.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {115},
   Number = {34},
   Pages = {8491-8498},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115},
   Abstract = {To predict and explain the behavior of others, one must
             understand that their actions are determined not by reality
             but by their beliefs about reality. Classically, children
             come to understand beliefs, including false beliefs, at
             about 4-5 y of age, but recent studies using different
             response measures suggest that even infants (and apes!) have
             some skills as well. Resolving this discrepancy is not
             possible with current theories based on individual
             cognition. Instead, what is needed is an account recognizing
             that the key processes in constructing an understanding of
             belief are social and mental coordination with other persons
             and their (sometimes conflicting) perspectives. Engaging in
             such social and mental coordination involves species-unique
             skills and motivations of shared intentionality, especially
             as they are manifest in joint attention and linguistic
             communication, as well as sophisticated skills of executive
             function to coordinate the different perspectives involved.
             This shared intentionality account accords well with
             documented differences in the cognitive capacities of great
             apes and human children, and it explains why infants and
             apes pass some versions of false-belief tasks whereas only
             older children pass others.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1804761115},
   Key = {fds337395}
}

@article{fds335757,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Zimmermann, L and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The social-cognitive basis of infants' reference to absent
             entities.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {177},
   Pages = {41-48},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.024},
   Abstract = {Recent evidence suggests that infants as young as 12 month
             of age use pointing to communicate about absent entities.
             The tacit assumption underlying these studies is that
             infants do so based on tracking what their interlocutor
             experienced in a previous shared interaction. The present
             study addresses this assumption empirically. In three
             experiments, 12-month-old infants could request additional
             desired objects by pointing to the location in which these
             objects were previously located. We systematically varied
             whether the adult from whom infants were requesting had
             previously experienced the former content of the location
             with the infant. Infants systematically adjusted their
             pointing to the now empty location to what they experienced
             with the adult previously. These results suggest that
             infants' ability to communicate about absent referents is
             based on an incipient form of common ground.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.024},
   Key = {fds335757}
}

@article{fds366588,
   Author = {John, M and Melis, AP and Read, D and Rossano, F and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The preference for scarcity: A developmental and comparative
             perspective},
   Journal = {Psychology and Marketing},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {603-615},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.21109},
   Abstract = {Human adults often show a preference for scarce over
             abundant goods. In this paper, we investigate whether this
             preference was shared by 4- and 6-year-old children as well
             as chimpanzees, humans’ nearest primate relative. Neither
             chimpanzees nor 4-year-olds displayed a scarcity preference,
             but 6-year-olds did, especially in the presence of
             competitors. We conclude that scarcity preference is a
             human-unique preference that develops as humans increase
             their cognitive skills and social experiences with peers and
             competitors. We explore different potential psychological
             explanations for scarcity preference and conclude scarcity
             preference is based on children's fear of missing out an
             opportunity, especially when dealing with uncertainty or
             goods of unknown value in the presence of competitors.
             Furthermore, the results are in line with studies showing
             that supply-based scarcity increases the desirability of
             hedonic goods, suggesting that even as early as 6 years of
             age humans may use scarce goods to feel unique or
             special.},
   Doi = {10.1002/mar.21109},
   Key = {fds366588}
}

@article{fds366589,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Precís of a natural history of human morality},
   Journal = {Philosophical Psychology},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {661-668},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2018.1486605},
   Abstract = {Here I summarize the main points in my 2016 book, A Natural
             History of Human Morality. Taking an evolutionary point of
             view, I characterize human morality as a special form of
             cooperation. In particular, human morality represents a kind
             of we > me orientation and valuation that emanates from the
             logic of social interdependence, both at the level of
             individual collaboration and at the level of the cultural
             group. Human morality emanates from psychological processes
             of shared intentionality evolved to enable individuals to
             function effectively in ever more cooperative
             lifeways.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09515089.2018.1486605},
   Key = {fds366589}
}

@article{fds366590,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Response to commentators},
   Journal = {Philosophical Psychology},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {817-829},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2018.1486604},
   Doi = {10.1080/09515089.2018.1486604},
   Key = {fds366590}
}

@article{fds333647,
   Author = {House, BR and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Modeling social norms increasingly influences costly sharing
             in middle childhood.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {171},
   Pages = {84-98},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.014},
   Abstract = {Prosocial and normative behavior emerges in early childhood,
             but substantial changes in prosocial behavior in middle
             childhood may be due to it becoming integrated with
             children's understanding of what is normative. Here we show
             that information about what is normative begins influencing
             children's costly sharing in middle childhood in a sample of
             6- to 11-year-old German children. Information about what is
             normative was most influential when indicating what was
             "right" (i.e., "The right thing is to choose this"). It was
             less influential when indicating what was prescribed by a
             rule (i.e., "There is a rule that says to choose this") or
             when it indicated what the majority of people do (i.e.,
             "Most people choose this"). These findings support the idea
             that middle childhood is when social norms begin to shape
             children's costly sharing and provide insight into the
             psychological foundations of the relationship between norms
             and prosocial behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.014},
   Key = {fds333647}
}

@article{fds329386,
   Author = {Domberg, A and Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's reasoning with peers in cooperative and
             competitive contexts.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {64-77},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12213},
   Abstract = {We report two studies that demonstrate how five- and
             seven-year-olds adapt their production of arguments to
             either a cooperative or a competitive context. Two games
             elicited agreements from peer dyads about placing animals on
             either of two halves of a playing field owned by either
             child. Children had to produce arguments to justify these
             decisions. Played in a competitive context that encouraged
             placing animals on one's own half, children's arguments
             showed a bias that was the result of withholding known
             arguments. In a cooperative context, children produced not
             only more arguments, but also more 'two-sided' arguments.
             Also, seven-year-olds demonstrated a more frequent and
             strategic use of arguments that specifically refuted
             decisions that would favour their peers. The results suggest
             that cooperative contexts provide a more motivating context
             for children to produce arguments. Statement of contribution
             What is already known on this subject? Reasoning is a social
             skill that allows people to reach joint decisions.
             Preschoolers give reasons for their proposals in their peer
             conversations. By adolescence, children use sophisticated
             arguments (e.g., refutations and rebuttals). What the
             present study adds? Cooperation offers a more motivating
             context for children's argument production. Seven-year-olds
             are more strategic than five-year-olds in their reasoning
             with peers. Children's reasoning with others becomes more
             sophisticated after preschool years.},
   Doi = {10.1111/bjdp.12213},
   Key = {fds329386}
}

@article{fds331567,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Hepach, R and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The specificity of reciprocity: Young children reciprocate
             more generously to those who intentionally benefit
             them.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {167},
   Pages = {336-353},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.005},
   Abstract = {Young children engage in direct reciprocity, but the
             mechanisms underlying such reciprocity remain unclear. In
             particular, prior work leaves unclear whether children's
             reciprocity is simply a response to receiving benefits
             (regardless of whether the benefits were intended) or driven
             by a mechanism of rewarding or preferring all benefactors
             (regardless of whom they benefited). Alternatively, perhaps
             children engage in genuine reciprocity such that they are
             particularly prosocial toward benefactors who intentionally
             provided them with benefits. Our findings support this
             third, richer possibility; the 3-year-olds who received
             benefits through the good intentions of a benefactor were
             subsequently more generous toward the benefactor than
             children who either (a) received the same benefits from the
             benefactor unintentionally or (b) observed the benefactor
             bestow the same benefits on another individual. Thus, young
             children are especially motivated to benefit those who have
             demonstrated goodwill toward them, suggesting, as one
             possible mechanism, an early sense of gratitude.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.005},
   Key = {fds331567}
}

@article{fds351573,
   Author = {Li, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The development of intention-based sociomoral judgment and
             distribution behavior from a third-party
             stance.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {167},
   Pages = {78-92},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.021},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated children's intention-based
             sociomoral judgments and distribution behavior from a
             third-party stance. An actor puppet showed either positive
             or negative intention toward a target puppet, which had
             previously performed a prosocial or antisocial action toward
             others (i.e., children witnessed various types of indirect
             reciprocity). Children (3- and 5-year-olds) were asked to
             make sociomoral judgments and to distribute resources to the
             actor puppet. Results showed that 5-year-olds were more
             likely than 3-year-olds to be influenced by intention when
             they made their judgment and distributed resources. The
             target's previous actions affected only 5-year-olds'
             intent-based social preference. These results suggest that
             children's judgments about intent-based indirect reciprocity
             develop from ages 3 to 5 years.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.021},
   Key = {fds351573}
}

@article{fds330413,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's meta-talk in their collaborative decision making
             with peers.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {166},
   Pages = {549-566},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.018},
   Abstract = {In collaborative decision making, children must evaluate the
             evidence behind their respective claims and the rationality
             of their respective proposals with their partners. In the
             main study, 5- and 7-year-old peer dyads (N = 196) were
             presented with a novel animal. In the key condition,
             children in a dyad individually received conflicting
             information about what the animal needs (e.g., rocks vs.
             sand for food) from sources that differ in reliability (with
             first-hand vs. indirect evidence). Dyads in both age groups
             were able to reliably settle on the option with the best
             supporting evidence. Moreover, in making their decision,
             children, especially 7-year-olds, engaged in various kinds
             of meta-talk about the evidence and its validity. In a
             modified version of the key condition in Study 2, 3- and
             5-year-olds (N = 120) interacted with a puppet who tried
             to convince children to change their minds by producing
             meta-talk. When the puppet insisted and produced meta-talk,
             5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, were more likely to change
             their minds if their information was unreliable. These
             results suggest that even preschoolers can engage in
             collaborative reasoning successfully, but the ability to
             reflect on the process by stepping back to jointly examine
             the evidence emerges only during the early school
             years.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.018},
   Key = {fds330413}
}

@article{fds330414,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Concern for Group Reputation Increases Prosociality in Young
             Children.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {181-190},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797617733830},
   Abstract = {The motivation to build and maintain a positive personal
             reputation promotes prosocial behavior. But individuals also
             identify with their groups, and so it is possible that the
             desire to maintain or enhance group reputation may have
             similar effects. Here, we show that 5-year-old children
             actively invest in the reputation of their group by acting
             more generously when their group's reputation is at stake.
             Children shared significantly more resources with fictitious
             other children not only when their individual donations were
             public rather than private but also when their group's
             donations (effacing individual donations) were public rather
             than private. These results provide the first experimental
             evidence that concern for group reputation can lead to
             higher levels of prosociality.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797617733830},
   Key = {fds330414}
}

@article{fds332050,
   Author = {Mammen, M and Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The reasons young children give to peers when explaining
             their judgments of moral and conventional
             rules.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {54},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {254-262},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000424},
   Abstract = {Moral justifications work, when they do, by invoking values
             that are shared in the common ground of the interlocutors.
             We asked 3- and 5-year-old peer dyads (N = 144) to identify
             and punish norm transgressors. In the moral condition, the
             transgressor violated a moral norm (e.g., by stealing); in
             the social rules condition, she/he violated a
             context-specific rule (e.g., by placing a yellow toy in a
             green box, instead of a yellow box). Children in both age
             groups justified their punishment in the social rules
             condition mostly by referring to the rule (e.g., "He must
             put yellow toys in the yellow box"). In contrast, in the
             moral condition they mostly justified their punishment by
             simply referring to the observed fact (e.g., "He stole"),
             seeing no need to state the norm involved (e.g., "He must
             not steal"), presumably because they assumed this as part of
             their moral common ground with their partner. These results
             suggest that preschoolers assume certain common ground moral
             values with their peers and use these in formulating
             explicit moral judgments and justifications. (PsycINFO
             Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000424},
   Key = {fds332050}
}

@article{fds329017,
   Author = {Grocke, P and Rossano, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children are more willing to accept group decisions in
             which they have had a voice.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {166},
   Pages = {67-78},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.003},
   Abstract = {People accept an unequal distribution of resources if they
             judge that the decision-making process was fair. In this
             study, 3- and 5-year-old children played an allocation game
             with two puppets. The puppets decided against a fair
             distribution in all conditions, but they allowed children to
             have various degrees of participation in the decision-making
             process. Children of both ages protested less when they were
             first asked to agree with the puppets' decision compared
             with when there was no agreement. When ignored, the younger
             children protested less than the older children-perhaps
             because they did not expect to have a say in the
             process-whereas they protested more when they were given an
             opportunity to voice their opinion-perhaps because their
             stated opinion was ignored. These results suggest that
             during the preschool years, children begin to expect to be
             asked for their opinion in a decision, and they accept
             disadvantageous decisions if they feel that they have had a
             voice in the decision-making process.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.003},
   Key = {fds329017}
}

@article{fds335758,
   Author = {Halina, M and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The goal of ape pointing.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {e0195182},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195182},
   Abstract = {Captive great apes regularly use pointing gestures in their
             interactions with humans. However, the precise function of
             this gesture is unknown. One possibility is that apes use
             pointing primarily to direct attention (as in "please look
             at that"); another is that they point mainly as an action
             request (such as "can you give that to me?"). We
             investigated these two possibilities here by examining how
             the looking behavior of recipients affects pointing in
             chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus).
             Upon pointing to food, subjects were faced with a recipient
             who either looked at the indicated object (successful-look)
             or failed to look at the indicated object (failed-look). We
             predicted that, if apes point primarily to direct attention,
             subjects would spend more time pointing in the failed-look
             condition because the goal of their gesture had not been
             met. Alternatively, we expected that, if apes point
             primarily to request an object, subjects would not differ in
             their pointing behavior between the successful-look and
             failed-look conditions because these conditions differed
             only in the looking behavior of the recipient. We found that
             subjects did differ in their pointing behavior across the
             successful-look and failed-look conditions, but contrary to
             our prediction subjects spent more time pointing in the
             successful-look condition. These results suggest that apes
             are sensitive to the attentional states of gestural
             recipients, but their adjustments are aimed at multiple
             goals. We also found a greater number of individuals with a
             strong right-hand than left-hand preference for
             pointing.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0195182},
   Key = {fds335758}
}

@article{fds366591,
   Author = {Quick, AE and Lieven, E and Backus, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Constructively combining languages},
   Journal = {Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {393-409},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/lab.17008.qui},
   Abstract = {Language development in bilingual children is often related
             to differing levels of proficiency. Objective measurements
             of bilingual development include for example mean length of
             utterance (MLU). MLU is almost always calculated for each
             language context (including both monolingual and code-mixed
             utterances). In the current study, we analyzed the MLUs of
             three German-English bilingual children, aged 2;3-3;11
             separately for the monolingual and code-mixed utterances.
             Our results showed that language preference was reflected in
             MLU values: the more children spoke in one language the
             higher the MLU was in that language. However, it was the
             mixed utterances that had the highest MLU for all three
             children. We support the results with a construction type
             analysis and suggest a potential usage-based explanation for
             these results based on individual differences in each
             child's developmental inventory of words and
             constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1075/lab.17008.qui},
   Key = {fds366591}
}

@article{fds366593,
   Author = {Quick, AE and Lieven, E and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Identifying partially schematic units in the code-mixing of
             an English and German speaking child},
   Journal = {Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {477-501},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/lab.15049.qui},
   Abstract = {Intra-sentential code-mixing presents a number of puzzles
             for theories of bilingualism. In this paper, we examine the
             code-mixed English-German utterances of a young
             English-German-Spanish trilingual child between 1;10 –
             3;1, using both an extensive diary kept by the mother and
             audio recordings. We address the interplay between lexical
             and syntactic aspects of language use outlined in the
             usage-based approach (e.g. Tomasello, 2003). The data
             suggest that partially schematic constructions play an
             important role in the code-mixing of this child. In
             addition, we find, first, that the code-mixing was not
             mainly the result of lexical gaps. Second, there was more
             mixing of German function words than content words. Third,
             code-mixed utterances often consisted of the use of a
             partially schematic construction with the open slot filled
             by material from the other language. These results raise a
             number of important issues for all theoretical approaches to
             code mixing, which we discuss.},
   Doi = {10.1075/lab.15049.qui},
   Key = {fds366593}
}

@article{fds366594,
   Author = {Sánchez-Amaro, A and Duguid, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees' understanding of social leverage.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {e0207868},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207868},
   Abstract = {Social primates can influence others through the control of
             resources. For instance, dominant male chimpanzees might
             allow subordinates access to mate with females in exchange
             for social support. However, little is known about how
             chimpanzees strategically use a position of leverage to
             maximize their own benefits. We address this question by
             presenting dyads of captive chimpanzee (N = 6) with a task
             resulting in an unequal reward distribution. To gain the
             higher reward each individual should wait for their partner
             to act. In addition, one participant had leverage: access to
             an alternative secure reward. By varying the presence and
             value of the leverage we tested whether individuals used it
             strategically (e.g. by waiting longer for partners to act
             when they had leverage in the form of alternatives).
             Additionally, non-social controls served to show if
             chimpanzees understood the social dilemma. We measured the
             likelihood to choose the leverage and their latencies to
             act. The final decision made by the chimpanzees did not
             differ as a function of condition (test versus non-social
             control) or the value of the leverage, but they did wait
             longer to act when the leverage was smaller-particularly in
             test (versus non-social control) trials suggesting that they
             understood the conflict of interest involved. The
             chimpanzees thus recognized the existence of social
             leverage, but did not use it strategically to maximize their
             rewards.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0207868},
   Key = {fds366594}
}

@article{fds328848,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Gonzalez-Cabrera, I and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children's developing metaethical judgments.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {164},
   Pages = {163-177},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.008},
   Abstract = {Human adults incline toward moral objectivism but may
             approach things more relativistically if different cultures
             are involved. In this study, 4-, 6-, and 9-year-old children
             (N=136) witnessed two parties who disagreed about moral
             matters: a normative judge (e.g., judging that it is wrong
             to do X) and an antinormative judge (e.g., judging that it
             is okay to do X). We assessed children's metaethical
             judgment, that is, whether they judged that only one party
             (objectivism) or both parties (relativism) could be right.
             We found that 9-year-olds, but not younger children, were
             more likely to judge that both parties could be right when a
             normative ingroup judge disagreed with an antinormative
             extraterrestrial judge (with different preferences and
             background) than when the antinormative judge was another
             ingroup individual. This effect was not found in a
             comparison case where parties disagreed about the
             possibility of different physical laws. These findings
             suggest that although young children often exhibit moral
             objectivism, by early school age they begin to temper their
             objectivism with culturally relative metaethical
             judgments.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.008},
   Key = {fds328848}
}

@article{fds326493,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Gonzalez-Cabrera, I},
   Title = {The Role of Ontogeny in the Evolution of Human
             Cooperation.},
   Journal = {Human nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {274-288},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9291-1},
   Abstract = {To explain the evolutionary emergence of uniquely human
             skills and motivations for cooperation, Tomasello et al.
             (2012, in Current Anthropology 53(6):673-92) proposed the
             interdependence hypothesis. The key adaptive context in this
             account was the obligate collaborative foraging of early
             human adults. Hawkes (2014, in Human Nature 25(1):28-48),
             following Hrdy (Mothers and Others, Harvard University
             Press, 2009), provided an alternative account for the
             emergence of uniquely human cooperative skills in which the
             key was early human infants' attempts to solicit care and
             attention from adults in a cooperative breeding context.
             Here we attempt to reconcile these two accounts. Our
             composite account accepts Hrdy's and Hawkes's contention
             that the extremely early emergence of human infants'
             cooperative skills suggests an important role for
             cooperative breeding as adaptive context, perhaps in early
             Homo. But our account also insists that human cooperation
             goes well beyond these nascent skills to include such things
             as the communicative and cultural conventions, norms, and
             institutions created by later Homo and early modern humans
             to deal with adult problems of social coordination. As part
             of this account we hypothesize how each of the main stages
             of human ontogeny (infancy, childhood, adolescence) was
             transformed during evolution both by infants' cooperative
             skills "migrating up" in age and by adults' cooperative
             skills "migrating down" in age.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s12110-017-9291-1},
   Key = {fds326493}
}

@article{fds320781,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Kante, N and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Toddlers Help a Peer.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {88},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1642-1652},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12686},
   Abstract = {Toddlers are remarkably prosocial toward adults, yet little
             is known about their helping behavior toward peers. In the
             present study with 18- and 30-month-old toddlers (n = 192,
             48 dyads per age group), one child needed help reaching an
             object to continue a task that was engaging for both
             children. The object was within reach of the second child
             who helped significantly more often compared to a no-need
             control condition. The helper also fulfilled the peer's need
             when the task was engaging only for the child needing help.
             These findings suggest that toddlers' skills and motivations
             of helping do not depend on having a competent and helpful
             recipient, such as an adult, but rather they are much more
             flexible and general.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12686},
   Key = {fds320781}
}

@article{fds327646,
   Author = {Kano, F and Krupenye, C and Hirata, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Submentalizing Cannot Explain Belief-Based Action
             Anticipation in Apes.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {633-634},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.011},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.011},
   Key = {fds327646}
}

@article{fds326494,
   Author = {Haux, L and Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Do young children preferentially trust gossip or firsthand
             observation in choosing a collaborative partner?},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {466-474},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sode.12225},
   Abstract = {From early on in ontogeny, young children hear things being
             said about particular individuals. Here we investigate the
             ways in which testimony with social content, that is,
             gossip, influences children's decision-making. We explored
             whether five-year-old (N = 72) and seven-year-old (N = 72)
             children trust gossip or firsthand observation in a partner
             choice setting. Seven-year-old children preferentially
             trusted what they had seen firsthand over gossip;
             five-year-old children, in contrast, did not differentiate
             between these two sources of information. However,
             five-year-old children (but not seven-year-olds) generally
             gave negative information more weight, that is, they showed
             a “negativity bias.” These results suggest that at
             around school age, young children become more
             “epistemically vigilant” about gossip.},
   Doi = {10.1111/sode.12225},
   Key = {fds326494}
}

@article{fds328849,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Duguid, S and Saur, H and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children, chimpanzees, and bonobos adjust the visibility of
             their actions for cooperators and competitors.},
   Journal = {Scientific reports},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {8504},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-08435-7},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees and bonobos are highly capable of tracking
             other's mental states. It has been proposed, however, that
             in contrast to humans, chimpanzees are only able to do this
             in competitive interactions but this has rarely been
             directly tested. Here, pairs of chimpanzees or bonobos
             (Study 1) and 4-year-old children (Study 2) were presented
             with two almost identical tasks differing only regarding the
             social context. In the cooperation condition, players'
             interests were matched: they had to make corresponding
             choices to be mutually rewarded. To facilitate coordination,
             subjects should thus make their actions visible to their
             partner whose view was partially occluded. In the
             competition condition, players' interests were directly
             opposed: the partner tried to match the subject's choice but
             subjects were only rewarded if they chose differently, so
             that they benefited from hiding their actions. The apes
             successfully adapted their decisions to the social context
             and their performance was markedly better in the cooperation
             condition. Children also distinguished between the two
             contexts, but somewhat surprisingly, performed better in the
             competitive condition. These findings demonstrate
             experimentally that chimpanzees and bonobos can take into
             account what others can see in cooperative interactions.
             Their social-cognitive skills are thus more flexible than
             previously assumed.},
   Doi = {10.1038/s41598-017-08435-7},
   Key = {fds328849}
}

@article{fds328850,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Clift, JB and Herrmann, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Social disappointment explains chimpanzees' behaviour in the
             inequity aversion task.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {284},
   Number = {1861},
   Pages = {20171502},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1502},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees' refusal of less-preferred food when an
             experimenter has previously provided preferred food to a
             conspecific has been taken as evidence for a sense of
             fairness. Here, we present a novel hypothesis-the social
             disappointment hypothesis-according to which food refusals
             express chimpanzees' disappointment in the human
             experimenter for not rewarding them as well as they could
             have. We tested this hypothesis using a two-by-two design in
             which food was either distributed by an experimenter or a
             machine and with a partner present or absent. We found that
             chimpanzees were more likely to reject food when it was
             distributed by an experimenter rather than by a machine and
             that they were not more likely to do so when a partner was
             present. These results suggest that chimpanzees' refusal of
             less-preferred food stems from social disappointment in the
             experimenter and not from a sense of fairness.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2017.1502},
   Key = {fds328850}
}

@article{fds320785,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's Intrinsic Motivation to Provide Help Themselves
             After Accidentally Harming Others.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {88},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1251-1264},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12646},
   Abstract = {Little is known about the flexibility of children's
             prosocial motivation. Here, 2- and 3-year-old children's
             (n = 128) internal arousal, as measured via changes in
             pupil dilation, was increased after they accidentally harmed
             a victim but were unable to repair the harm. If they were
             able to repair (or if they themselves did not cause the harm
             and the help was provided by someone else) their arousal
             subsided. This suggests that children are especially
             motivated to help those whom they have harmed, perhaps out
             of a sense of guilt and a desire to reconcile with them.
             Young children care not only about the well-being of others
             but also about the relationship they have with those who
             depend on their help.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12646},
   Key = {fds320785}
}

@article{fds325488,
   Author = {Kanngiesser, P and Köymen, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children mostly keep, and expect others to keep, their
             promises.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {159},
   Pages = {140-158},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.004},
   Abstract = {Promises are speech acts that create an obligation to do the
             promised action. In three studies, we investigated whether
             3- and 5-year-olds (N=278) understand the normative
             implications of promising in prosocial interactions. In
             Study 1, children helped a partner who promised to share
             stickers. When the partner failed to uphold the promise, 3-
             and 5-year-olds protested and referred to promise norms. In
             Study 2, when children in this same age range were asked to
             promise to continue a cleaning task-and they agreed-they
             persisted longer on the task and mentioned their obligation
             more frequently than without such a promise. They also
             persisted longer after a promise than after a cleaning
             reminder (Study 3). In prosocial interactions, thus, young
             children feel a normative obligation to keep their promises
             and expect others to keep their promises as
             well.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.004},
   Key = {fds325488}
}

@article{fds327020,
   Author = {Schmelz, M and Grueneisen, S and Kabalak, A and Jost, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees return favors at a personal cost.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {114},
   Number = {28},
   Pages = {7462-7467},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1700351114},
   Abstract = {Humans regularly provide others with resources at a personal
             cost to themselves. Chimpanzees engage in some cooperative
             behaviors in the wild as well, but their motivational
             underpinnings are unclear. In three experiments, chimpanzees
             (<i>Pan troglodytes</i>) always chose between an option
             delivering food both to themselves and a partner and one
             delivering food only to themselves. In one condition, a
             conspecific partner had just previously taken a personal
             risk to make this choice available. In another condition, no
             assistance from the partner preceded the subject's decision.
             Chimpanzees made significantly more prosocial choices after
             receiving their partner's assistance than when no assistance
             was given (experiment 1) and, crucially, this was the case
             even when choosing the prosocial option was materially
             costly for the subject (experiment 2). Moreover, subjects
             appeared sensitive to the risk of their partner's assistance
             and chose prosocially more often when their partner risked
             losing food by helping (experiment 3). These findings
             demonstrate experimentally that chimpanzees are willing to
             incur a material cost to deliver rewards to a conspecific,
             but only if that conspecific previously assisted them, and
             particularly when this assistance was risky. Some key
             motivations involved in human cooperation thus may have
             deeper phylogenetic roots than previously
             suspected.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1700351114},
   Key = {fds327020}
}

@article{fds326490,
   Author = {Sánchez-Amaro, A and Duguid, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees, bonobos, and children successfully coordinate
             in conflict situations.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
             Sciences},
   Volume = {284},
   Number = {1856},
   Publisher = {Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
             Sciences},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0259},
   Abstract = {Social animals need to coordinate with others to reap the
             benefits of group-living even when individuals’ interests
             are misaligned. We compare how chimpanzees, bonobos and
             children coordinate their actions with a conspecific in a
             Snowdrift game, which provides a model for understanding how
             organisms coordinate and make decisions under conflict. In
             study 1, we presented pairs of chimpanzees, bonobos and
             children with an unequal reward distribution. In the
             critical condition, the preferred reward could only be
             obtained by waiting for the partner to act, with the risk
             that if no one acted, both would lose the rewards. Apes and
             children successfully coordinated to obtain the rewards.
             Children used a ‘both-partner-pull’ strategy and
             communicated during the task, while some apes relied on an
             ‘only-one-partner-pulls’ strategy to solve the task,
             although there were also signs of strategic behaviour as
             they waited for their partner to pull when that strategy led
             to the preferred reward. In study 2, we presented pairs of
             chimpanzees and bonobos with the same set-up as in study 1
             with the addition of a non-social option that provided them
             with a secure reward. In this situation, apes had to
             actively decide between the unequal distribution and the
             alternative. In this set-up, apes maximized their rewards by
             taking their partners’ potential actions into account. In
             conclusion, children and apes showed clear instances of
             strategic decision-making to maximize their own rewards
             while maintaining successful coordination.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2017.0259},
   Key = {fds326490}
}

@article{fds326491,
   Author = {Hardecker, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {From imitation to implementation: How two- and
             three-year-old children learn to enforce social
             norms.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {237-248},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12159},
   Abstract = {Young children enforce social norms from early on, but
             little research has examined how this enforcement behaviour
             emerges. This study investigated whether observing an
             adult's norm enforcement influences children's own
             enforcement of that norm compared with observing an action
             demonstration without enforcement. Additionally, children
             experienced enforcement either following their own
             (second-party) or a third-party's transgression (N = 120).
             Results revealed that observing enforcement increased two-
             and three-year-old children's protest against the sanctioned
             action regardless of second- or third-party context.
             However, only three-year-olds generalized their enforcement
             to a novel action not matching the norm, whereas
             two-year-olds only protested against the previously
             sanctioned action. Importantly, without any enforcement
             demonstration, two-year-olds rarely protested at all while
             three-year-olds did so quite frequently. Thus, providing an
             opportunity to imitate enforcement seems to give rise to
             enforcement behaviour in two-year-olds while three-year-olds
             already understand normative implications following a
             variety of cues and even apply norm enforcement without any
             demonstration of how to do it. Statement of contribution
             What is already known on this subject? Children conform to
             social norms from early in development. Young children from
             2 to 3 years of age also enforce social norms on third
             parties. What does this study add? Observing enforcement by
             an adult increases two- and three-year-olds' protest against
             the sanctioned action. It does not matter whether children
             experienced enforcement on their own or a third party's
             action. Three-, but not two-year-olds, generalize their
             enforcement to novel actions that do not match the
             norm.},
   Doi = {10.1111/bjdp.12159},
   Key = {fds326491}
}

@article{fds326492,
   Author = {Rapp, DJ and Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The impact of choice on young children's prosocial
             motivation.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {158},
   Pages = {112-121},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.004},
   Abstract = {The current study explored how freedom of choice affects
             preschoolers' prosocial motivation. Children (3- and
             5-year-olds) participated in either a choice condition
             (where they could decide for themselves whether to help or
             not) or a no-choice condition (where they were instructed to
             help). Prosocial motivation was subsequently assessed by
             measuring the amount children helped an absent peer in the
             face of an attractive alternative game. The 5-year-olds
             provided with choice helped more than the children not
             provided with choice, and this effect was stronger for girls
             than for boys. There was no difference between conditions
             for the 3-year-olds. These results highlight the importance
             of choice in young children's prosocial development.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.004},
   Key = {fds326492}
}

@article{fds320783,
   Author = {Hardecker, S and Schmidt, MFH and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children’s Developing Understanding of the Conventionality
             of Rules},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {163-188},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2016.1255624},
   Abstract = {Much research has investigated how children relate to norms
             taught to them by adult authorities. Very few studies have
             investigated norms that arise out of children’s own peer
             interactions. In two studies, we investigated how 5- and
             7-year-old children teach, enforce, and understand rules
             that they either created themselves or were taught by an
             adult. Children (N = 240) were asked to either invent game
             rules on their own or were taught these exact same rules by
             an adult (yoked design). Children of both ages enforced and
             transmitted the rules in a normative way, regardless of
             whether they had invented them or were taught the rules by
             an adult, suggesting that they viewed even their own
             self-made rules as normatively binding. However, creating
             the rules led 5-year-old children to understand them as much
             more changeable as compared with adult-taught rules.
             Seven-year-olds, in contrast, regarded both kinds of rules
             as equally changeable, indeed allowing fewer changes to
             their self-created rules than 5-year-olds. While the process
             of creating rules seemed to enlighten preschoolers’
             understanding of the conventionality of the rules,
             school-aged children regarded both self-created rules and
             adult-taught rules in a similar manner, suggesting a deeper
             understanding of rule normativity as arising from social
             agreement and commitment.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15248372.2016.1255624},
   Key = {fds320783}
}

@article{fds320782,
   Author = {Ulber, J and Hamann, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children, but not chimpanzees, are averse to
             disadvantageous and advantageous inequities.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {155},
   Pages = {48-66},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.013},
   Abstract = {The age at which young children show an aversion to
             inequitable resource distributions, especially those
             favoring themselves, is unclear. It is also unclear whether
             great apes, as humans' nearest evolutionary relatives, have
             an aversion to inequitable resource distributions at all.
             Using a common methodology across species and child ages,
             the current two studies found that 3- and 4-year-old
             children (N=64) not only objected when they received less
             than a collaborative partner but also sacrificed to equalize
             when they received more. They did neither of these things in
             a nonsocial situation, demonstrating the fundamental role of
             social comparison. In contrast, chimpanzees (N=9) showed no
             aversion to inequitable distributions, only a concern for
             maximizing their own resources, with no differences between
             social and nonsocial conditions. These results underscore
             the unique importance for humans, even early in ontogeny,
             for treating others fairly, presumably as a way of becoming
             a cooperative member of one's cultural group.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.013},
   Key = {fds320782}
}

@article{fds325489,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children coordinate in a recurrent social dilemma by taking
             turns and along dominance asymmetries.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {265-273},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000236},
   Abstract = {Humans constantly have to coordinate their decisions with
             others even when their interests are conflicting (e.g., when
             2 drivers have to decide who yields at an intersection). So
             far, however, little is known about the development of these
             abilities. Here, we present dyads of 5-year-olds (N = 40)
             with a repeated chicken game using a novel methodology: Two
             children each steered an automated toy train carrying a
             reward. The trains simultaneously moved toward each other so
             that in order to avoid a crash-which left both children
             empty-handed-1 train had to swerve. By swerving, however,
             the trains lost a portion of the rewards so that it was in
             each child's interest to go straight. Children coordinated
             their decisions successfully over multiple rounds, and they
             mostly did so by taking turns at swerving. In dyads in which
             turn-taking was rare, dominant children obtained
             significantly higher payoffs than their partners. Moreover,
             the coordination process was more efficient in turn-taking
             dyads as indicated by a significant reduction in conflicts
             and verbal protest. These findings indicate that already by
             the late preschool years children can independently
             coordinate decisions with peers in recurrent conflicts of
             interest. (PsycINFO Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000236},
   Key = {fds325489}
}

@article{fds329018,
   Author = {Krupenye, C and Kano, F and Hirata, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes' performance
             in a false belief task inanimate control.},
   Journal = {Communicative & integrative biology},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {e1343771},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771},
   Abstract = {Much debate concerns whether any nonhuman animals share with
             humans the ability to infer others' mental states, such as
             desires and beliefs. In a recent eye-tracking false-belief
             task, we showed that great apes correctly anticipated that a
             human actor would search for a goal object where he had last
             seen it, even though the apes themselves knew that it was no
             longer there. In response, Heyes proposed that apes' looking
             behavior was guided not by social cognitive mechanisms but
             rather domain-general cueing effects, and suggested the use
             of inanimate controls to test this alternative
             submentalizing hypothesis. In the present study, we
             implemented the suggested inanimate control of our previous
             false-belief task. Apes attended well to key events but
             showed markedly fewer anticipatory looks and no significant
             tendency to look to the correct location. We thus found no
             evidence that submentalizing was responsible for apes'
             anticipatory looks in our false-belief task.},
   Doi = {10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771},
   Key = {fds329018}
}

@article{fds322245,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The fulfillment of others' needs elevates children's body
             posture.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {100-113},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000173},
   Abstract = {Much is known about young children's helping behavior, but
             little is known about the underlying motivations and
             emotions involved. In 2 studies we found that 2-year-old
             children showed positive emotions of similar magnitude-as
             measured by changes in their postural elevation using depth
             sensor imaging technology-after they achieved a goal for
             themselves and after they helped another person achieve her
             goal. Conversely, children's posture decreased in elevation
             when their actions did not result in a positive outcome.
             These results suggest that for young children, working for
             themselves and helping others are similarly rewarding.
             (PsycINFO Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000173},
   Key = {fds322245}
}

@article{fds326212,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Buttelmann, F and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an
             interactive helping task.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {e0173793},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173793},
   Abstract = {Understanding the behavior of others in a wide variety of
             circumstances requires an understanding of their
             psychological states. Humans' nearest primate relatives, the
             great apes, understand many psychological states of others,
             for example, perceptions, goals, and desires. However, so
             far there is little evidence that they possess the key
             marker of advanced human social cognition: an understanding
             of false beliefs. Here we demonstrate that in a nonverbal
             (implicit) false-belief test which is passed by human
             1-year-old infants, great apes as a group, including
             chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), and
             orangutans (Pongo abelii), distinguish between true and
             false beliefs in their helping behavior. Great apes thus may
             possess at least some basic understanding that an agent's
             actions are based on her beliefs about reality. Hence, such
             understanding might not be the exclusive province of the
             human species.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0173793},
   Key = {fds326212}
}

@article{fds351576,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Haberl, K and Lambert, S and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Toddlers Help Anonymously},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {130-145},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/infa.12143},
   Abstract = {Young children are extremely motivated to help others, but
             it is not clear whether they do so in anonymous situations
             without social recognition. In two studies, we found that
             18-month-old toddlers provided help equally in situations
             where an adult recipient was present and in situations
             where an adult recipient was not present. We included
             several control conditions to rule out that toddlers were
             simply unaware of their anonymity or were merely motivated
             to restore the physical order of things. Together, these
             findings suggest that early in ontogeny children are
             motivated to help others in need regardless of whether they
             can immediately be recognized for their prosocial
             intentions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/infa.12143},
   Key = {fds351576}
}

@article{fds351577,
   Author = {Schmid, B and Karg, K and Perner, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Great apes are sensitive to prior reliability of an
             informant in a gaze following task.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {e0187451},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187451},
   Abstract = {Social animals frequently rely on information from other
             individuals. This can be costly in case the other individual
             is mistaken or even deceptive. Human infants below 4 years
             of age show proficiency in their reliance on differently
             reliable informants. They can infer the reliability of an
             informant from few interactions and use that assessment in
             later interactions with the same informant in a different
             context. To explore whether great apes share that ability,
             in our study we confronted great apes with a reliable or
             unreliable informant in an object choice task, to see
             whether that would in a subsequent task affect their gaze
             following behaviour in response to the same informant. In
             our study, prior reliability of the informant and
             habituation during the gaze following task affected both
             great apes' automatic gaze following response and their more
             deliberate response of gaze following behind barriers. As
             habituation is very context specific, it is unlikely that
             habituation in the reliability task affected the gaze
             following task. Rather it seems that apes employ a
             reliability tracking strategy that results in a general
             avoidance of additional information from an unreliable
             informant.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0187451},
   Key = {fds351577}
}

@article{fds351578,
   Author = {Haun, DBM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How to Compare Across Species.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {1670-1672},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616671336},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797616671336},
   Key = {fds351578}
}

@article{fds320786,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Early Emergence of Guilt-Motivated Prosocial
             Behavior.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1772-1782},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12628},
   Abstract = {Guilt serves vital prosocial functions: It motivates
             transgressors to make amends, thus restoring damaged
             relationships. Previous developmental research on guilt has
             not clearly distinguished it from sympathy for a victim or a
             tendency to repair damage in general. The authors tested 2-
             and 3-year-old children (N = 62 and 64, respectively) in a
             2 × 2 design, varying whether or not a mishap caused harm
             to someone and whether children themselves caused that
             mishap. Three-year-olds showed greatest reparative behavior
             when they had caused the mishap and it caused harm, thus
             showing a specific effect of guilt. Two-year-olds repaired
             more whenever harm was caused, no matter by whom, thus
             showing only an effect of sympathy. Guilt as a distinct
             motivator of prosocial behavior thus emerges by at least
             3 years.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12628},
   Key = {fds320786}
}

@article{fds320787,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Grossmann, T and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young Children Want to See Others Get the Help They
             Need.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1703-1714},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12633},
   Abstract = {Children's instrumental helping has sometimes been
             interpreted as a desire to complete action sequences or to
             restore the physical order of things. Two-year-old children
             (n = 51) selectively retrieved for an adult the object he
             needed rather than one he did not (but which equally served
             to restore the previous order of things), and those with
             greater internal arousal (i.e., pupil dilation) were faster
             to help. In a second experiment (n = 64), children's
             arousal increased when they witnessed an adult respond
             inappropriately to another adult's need. This was not the
             case in a nonsocial control condition. These findings
             suggest that children's helping is not aimed at restoring
             the order of things but rather at seeing another person's
             need fulfilled.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12633},
   Key = {fds320787}
}

@article{fds320784,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How chimpanzees cooperate: If dominance is artificially
             constrained.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {113},
   Number = {44},
   Pages = {E6728-E6729},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1614378113},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1614378113},
   Key = {fds320784}
}

@article{fds351579,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The role of past interactions in great apes' communication
             about absent entities.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {130},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {351-357},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000042},
   Abstract = {Recent evidence suggests that great apes can use the former
             location of an entity to communicate about it. In this study
             we built on these findings to investigate the
             social-cognitive foundations of great apes' communicative
             abilities. We tested whether great apes (n = 35) would
             adjust their requests for absent entities to previous
             interactions they had with their interlocutor. We
             manipulated the apes' experience with respect to the
             interlocutor's knowledge about the previous content of the
             now-empty location as well as their experience with the
             interlocutor's competence to provide additional food items.
             We found that apes adjusted their requests to both of these
             aspects but failed to integrate them with one another. These
             results demonstrate a surprising amount of flexibility in
             great apes' communicative abilities while at the same time
             suggesting some important limitations in their social
             communicative skills. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/com0000042},
   Key = {fds351579}
}

@article{fds351580,
   Author = {Brandt, S and Buttelmann, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children's understanding of first- and third-person
             perspectives in complement clauses and false-belief
             tasks.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {151},
   Pages = {131-143},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.004},
   Abstract = {De Villiers (Lingua, 2007, Vol. 117, pp. 1858-1878) and
             others have claimed that children come to understand false
             belief as they acquire linguistic constructions for
             representing a proposition and the speaker's epistemic
             attitude toward that proposition. In the current study,
             English-speaking children of 3 and 4years of age (N=64) were
             asked to interpret propositional attitude constructions with
             a first- or third-person subject of the propositional
             attitude (e.g., "I think the sticker is in the red box" or
             "The cow thinks the sticker is in the red box",
             respectively). They were also assessed for an understanding
             of their own and others' false beliefs. We found that
             4-year-olds showed a better understanding of both
             third-person propositional attitude constructions and false
             belief than their younger peers. No significant
             developmental differences were found for first-person
             propositional attitude constructions. The older children
             also showed a better understanding of their own false
             beliefs than of others' false beliefs. In addition,
             regression analyses suggest that the older children's
             comprehension of their own false beliefs was mainly related
             to their understanding of third-person propositional
             attitude constructions. These results indicate that we need
             to take a closer look at the propositional attitude
             constructions that are supposed to support children's
             false-belief reasoning. Children may come to understand
             their own and others' beliefs in different ways, and this
             may affect both their use and understanding of propositional
             attitude constructions and their performance in various
             types of false-belief tasks.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.004},
   Key = {fds351580}
}

@article{fds320788,
   Author = {Krupenye, C and Kano, F and Hirata, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act
             according to false beliefs.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {354},
   Number = {6308},
   Pages = {110-114},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8110},
   Abstract = {Humans operate with a "theory of mind" with which they are
             able to understand that others' actions are driven not by
             reality but by beliefs about reality, even when those
             beliefs are false. Although great apes share with humans
             many social-cognitive skills, they have repeatedly failed
             experimental tests of such false-belief understanding. We
             use an anticipatory looking test (originally developed for
             human infants) to show that three species of great apes
             reliably look in anticipation of an agent acting on a
             location where he falsely believes an object to be, even
             though the apes themselves know that the object is no longer
             there. Our results suggest that great apes also operate, at
             least on an implicit level, with an understanding of false
             beliefs.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.aaf8110},
   Key = {fds320788}
}

@article{fds320789,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {In Memoriam: Jerome Seymour Bruner [1915–2016]},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {155},
   Pages = {iii-iv},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.013},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.013},
   Key = {fds320789}
}

@article{fds321683,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Butler, LP and Heinz, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young Children See a Single Action and Infer a Social
             Norm.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {1360-1370},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616661182},
   Abstract = {Human social life depends heavily on social norms that
             prescribe and proscribe specific actions. Typically, young
             children learn social norms from adult instruction. In the
             work reported here, we showed that this is not the whole
             story: Three-year-old children are promiscuous normativists.
             In other words, they spontaneously inferred the presence of
             social norms even when an adult had done nothing to indicate
             such a norm in either language or behavior. And children of
             this age even went so far as to enforce these self-inferred
             norms when third parties "broke" them. These results suggest
             that children do not just passively acquire social norms
             from adult behavior and instruction; rather, they have a
             natural and proactive tendency to go from "is" to "ought."
             That is, children go from observed actions to prescribed
             actions and do not perceive them simply as guidelines for
             their own behavior but rather as objective normative rules
             applying to everyone equally.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797616661182},
   Key = {fds321683}
}

@article{fds351581,
   Author = {Over, H and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do young children accept responsibility for the negative
             actions of ingroup members?},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {40},
   Pages = {24-32},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.004},
   Abstract = {This study investigated whether young children accept
             responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members.
             Five-year-old children watched a transgressor break someone
             else's valued possession. Depending on condition, this
             transgressor either belonged to the same group as the child
             or a different group from the child. Coding of children's
             nonverbal behaviour indicated that they displayed more signs
             of guilt (but not other negative emotions) when the
             transgressor belonged to their own group than the other
             group. Furthermore, when the transgressor belonged to their
             own group, children were more likely to say that their own
             group should apologise for the damage and that they
             themselves should try to repair the broken object.
             Children's connections to their groups are thus so profound
             that they appear to feel responsible for the negative
             actions of their group members even when they had no
             personal involvement in the harm those actions
             caused.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.004},
   Key = {fds351581}
}

@article{fds351582,
   Author = {Ibbotson, P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Language in a New Key.},
   Journal = {Scientific American},
   Volume = {315},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {70-75},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1116-70},
   Doi = {10.1038/scientificamerican1116-70},
   Key = {fds351582}
}

@article{fds351583,
   Author = {Hardecker, S and Schmidt, MFH and Roden, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's behavioral and emotional responses to
             different social norm violations.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {150},
   Pages = {364-379},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.012},
   Abstract = {From an early age, children can talk meaningfully about
             differences between moral and conventional norms. But does
             their understanding of these differences manifest itself in
             their actual behavioral and emotional reactions to norm
             violations? And do children discriminate between norm
             violations that affect either themselves or a third party?
             Two studies (N=224) were conducted in which children
             observed conventional game rule violations and moral
             transgressions that either disadvantaged themselves directly
             or disadvantaged an absent third party. Results revealed
             that 3- and 5-year-olds evaluated both conventional and
             moral transgressions as normative breaches and protested
             against them. However, 5-year-olds also clearly
             discriminated these types of transgressions along further
             dimensions in that (a) they tattled largely on the moral
             violation and less on the conventional violation and (b)
             they showed stronger emotional reactions to moral violations
             compared to conventional violations. The 3-year-olds'
             responses to moral and conventional transgressions, however,
             were less discriminatory, and these younger children
             responded rather similarly to both kinds of violations.
             Importantly, most children intervened both as victims of the
             transgression and as unaffected third parties alike,
             providing strong evidence for their agent-neutral
             understanding of social norms.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.012},
   Key = {fds351583}
}

@article{fds320790,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Jerome Seymour Bruner [1915-2016].},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {967-968},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000916000374},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000916000374},
   Key = {fds320790}
}

@article{fds351584,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Preschoolers affect others' reputations through prosocial
             gossip.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {447-460},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12143},
   Abstract = {Providing evaluative information to others about absent
             third parties helps them to identify cooperators and avoid
             cheaters. Here, we show that 5-year-olds, but not
             3-year-olds, reliably engage in such prosocial gossip. In an
             experimental setting, 5-year-old children spontaneously
             offered relevant reputational information to guide a peer
             towards a cooperative partner. Three-year-old children
             offered such evaluative information only rarely, although
             they still showed a willingness to inform in a
             non-evaluative manner. A follow-up study revealed that one
             component involved in this age difference is children's
             developing ability to provide justifications. The current
             results extend previous work on young children's tendency to
             manage their own reputation by showing that preschoolers
             also influence others' reputations via gossip.},
   Doi = {10.1111/bjdp.12143},
   Key = {fds351584}
}

@article{fds323256,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Herrmann, E and Markmann, C and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Preschoolers value those who sanction non-cooperators.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {153},
   Pages = {43-51},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.011},
   Abstract = {Large-scale human cooperation among unrelated individuals
             requires the enforcement of social norms. However, such
             enforcement poses a problem because non-enforcers can free
             ride on others' costly and risky enforcement. One solution
             is that enforcers receive benefits relative to
             non-enforcers. Here we show that this solution becomes
             functional during the preschool years: 5-year-old (but not
             4-year-old) children judged enforcers of norms more
             positively, preferred enforcers, and distributed more
             resources to enforcers than to non-enforcers. The ability to
             sustain not only first-order but also second-order
             cooperation thus emerges quite early in human ontogeny,
             providing a viable solution to the problem of higher-order
             cooperation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.011},
   Key = {fds323256}
}

@article{fds340136,
   Author = {Melis, A and Grocke, P and Kalbitz, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {One for you, one for me: Humans' unique turn-taking
             skills},
   Journal = {Psychological Science},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {987-996},
   Publisher = {Association for Psychological Science},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616644070},
   Abstract = {Long-term collaborative relationships require that any
             jointly produced resources be shared in mutually
             satisfactory ways. Prototypically, this sharing involves
             partners dividing up simultaneously available resources, but
             sometimes the collaboration makes a resource available to
             only one individual, and any sharing of resources must take
             place across repeated instances over time. Here, we show
             that beginning at 5 years of age, human children stabilize
             cooperation in such cases by taking turns across instances
             of obtaining a resource. In contrast, chimpanzees do not
             take turns in this way, and so their collaboration tends to
             disintegrate over time. Alternating turns in obtaining a
             collaboratively produced resource does not necessarily
             require a prosocial concern for the other, but rather
             requires only a strategic judgment that partners need
             incentives to continue collaborating. These results suggest
             that human beings are adapted for thinking strategically in
             ways that sustain long-term cooperative relationships and
             that are absent in their nearest primate
             relatives.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797616644070},
   Key = {fds340136}
}

@article{fds323257,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Rapp, DJ and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children (sometimes) do the right thing even when
             their peers do not},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {39},
   Pages = {86-92},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.04.004},
   Abstract = {Children must sometimes decide between conforming to peer
             behavior and doing what is right. While research shows that
             children have a strong inclination to act prosocially and to
             help conspecifics in need, many studies also demonstrate
             that children tend to adopt peer behavior. In two studies (N
             = 96), we investigated whether children would conform to an
             antisocial majority or, whether they would do the right
             thing even under peer pressure. Results show that if a
             recipient is in need, 5-year-old children act prosocially in
             two different contexts even when there is a strong selfish
             incentive not to. However, once the severity of the
             recipient's need is reduced, children conform to the
             antisocial group. The current studies suggest that
             children's prosocial motivation sometimes wins out against
             more selfish drives.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.04.004},
   Key = {fds323257}
}

@article{fds351585,
   Author = {Ulber, J and Hamann, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Extrinsic Rewards Diminish Costly Sharing in
             3-Year-Olds.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1192-1203},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12534},
   Abstract = {Two studies investigated the influence of external rewards
             and social praise in young children's fairness-related
             behavior. The motivation of ninety-six 3-year-olds' to
             equalize unfair resource allocations was measured in three
             scenarios (collaboration, windfall, and dictator game)
             following three different treatments (material reward,
             verbal praise, and neutral response). In all scenarios,
             children's willingness to engage in costly sharing was
             negatively influenced when they had received a reward for
             equal sharing during treatment than when they had received
             praise or no reward. The negative effect of material rewards
             was not due to subjects responding in kind to their
             partner's termination of rewards. These results provide new
             evidence for the intrinsic motivation of prosociality-in
             this case, costly sharing behavior-in preschool
             children.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12534},
   Key = {fds351585}
}

@article{fds351586,
   Author = {Sánchez-Amaro, A and Duguid, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees coordinate in a snowdrift game},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {116},
   Pages = {61-74},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.030},
   Abstract = {The snowdrift game is a model for studying social
             coordination in the context of competing interests. We
             presented pairs of chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, with a
             situation in which they could either pull a weighted tray
             together or pull alone to obtain food. Ultimately
             chimpanzees should coordinate their actions because if no
             one pulled, they would both lose the reward. There were two
             experimental manipulations: the tray's weight (low or high
             weight condition) and the time to solve the dilemma before
             the rewards became inaccessible (40 s or 10 s). When the
             costs were high (i.e. high weight condition), chimpanzees
             waited longer to act. Cooperation tended to increase in
             frequency across sessions. The pulling effort invested in
             the task also became more skewed between subjects. The
             subjects also adjusted their behaviour by changing their
             pulling effort for different partners. These results
             demonstrate that chimpanzees can coordinate their actions in
             situations where there is a conflict of interest.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.030},
   Key = {fds351586}
}

@article{fds351587,
   Author = {Butler, LP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two- and 3-year-olds integrate linguistic and pedagogical
             cues in guiding inductive generalization and
             exploration.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {145},
   Pages = {64-78},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.001},
   Abstract = {Young children can in principle make generic inferences
             (e.g., "doffels are magnetic") on the basis of their own
             individual experience. Recent evidence, however, shows that
             by 4 years of age children make strong generic inferences on
             the basis of a single pedagogical demonstration with an
             individual (e.g., an adult demonstrates for the child that a
             single "doffel" is magnetic). In the current experiments, we
             extended this to look at younger children, investigating how
             the mechanisms underlying this phenomenon are integrated
             with other aspects of inductive inference during early
             development. We found that both 2- and 3-year-olds used
             pedagogical cues to guide such generic inferences, but only
             so long as the "doffel" was linguistically labeled. In a
             follow-up study, 3-year-olds, but not 2-year-olds, continued
             to make this generic inference even if the word "doffel" was
             uttered incidentally and non-referentially in a context
             preceding the pedagogical demonstration, thereby simply
             marking the opportunity to learn about a culturally
             important category. By 3 years of age, then, young children
             show a remarkable ability to flexibly combine different
             sources of culturally relevant information (e.g., linguistic
             labeling, pedagogy) to make the kinds of generic inferences
             so central in human cultural learning.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.001},
   Key = {fds351587}
}

@article{fds351588,
   Author = {Zeidler, H and Herrmann, E and B M Haun and D and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Taking Turns or Not? Children's Approach to Limited Resource
             Problems in Three Different Cultures.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {677-688},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12505},
   Abstract = {Some problems of resource distribution can be solved on
             equal terms only by taking turns. We presented such a
             problem to 168 pairs of 5- to 10-year-old children from one
             Western and two non-Western societies (German, Samburu,
             Kikuyu). Almost all German pairs solved the problem by
             taking turns immediately, resulting in an equal distribution
             of resources throughout the game. In the other groups, one
             child usually monopolized the resource in Trial 1 and
             sometimes let the partner monopolize it in Trial 2,
             resulting in an equal distribution in only half the dyads.
             These results suggest that turn-taking is not a natural
             strategy uniformly across human cultures, but rather that
             different cultures use it to different degrees and in
             different contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12505},
   Key = {fds351588}
}

@article{fds351589,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cultural Learning Redux.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {643-653},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12499},
   Abstract = {M. Tomasello, A. Kruger, and H. Ratner (1993) proposed a
             theory of cultural learning comprising imitative learning,
             instructed learning, and collaborative learning. Empirical
             and theoretical advances in the past 20 years suggest
             modifications to the theory; for example, children do not
             just imitate but overimitate in order to identify and
             affiliate with others in their cultural group, children
             learn from pedagogy not just episodic facts but the generic
             structure of their cultural worlds, and children
             collaboratively co-construct with those in their culture
             normative rules for doing things. In all, human children do
             not just culturally learn useful instrumental activities and
             information, they conform to the normative expectations of
             the cultural group and even contribute themselves to the
             creation of such normative expectations.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12499},
   Key = {fds351589}
}

@article{fds351590,
   Author = {Karg, K and Schmelz, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Differing views: Can chimpanzees do Level 2
             perspective-taking?},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {555-564},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0956-7},
   Abstract = {Although chimpanzees understand what others may see, it is
             unclear whether they understand how others see things (Level
             2 perspective-taking). We investigated whether chimpanzees
             can predict the behavior of a conspecific which is holding a
             mistaken perspective that differs from their own. The
             subject competed with a conspecific over two food sticks.
             While the subject could see that both were the same size, to
             the competitor one appeared bigger than the other. In a
             previously established game, the competitor chose one stick
             in private first and the subject chose thereafter, without
             knowing which of the sticks was gone. Chimpanzees and
             6-year-old children chose the 'riskier' stick (that looked
             bigger to the competitor) significantly less in the game
             than in a nonsocial control. Children chose randomly in the
             control, thus showing Level 2 perspective-taking skills; in
             contrast, chimpanzees had a preference for the 'riskier'
             stick here, rendering it possible that they attributed their
             own preference to the competitor to predict her choice. We
             thus run a follow-up in which chimpanzees did not have a
             preference in the control. Now, they also chose randomly in
             the game. We conclude that chimpanzees solved the task by
             attributing their own preference to the other, while
             children truly understood the other's mistaken
             perspective.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-016-0956-7},
   Key = {fds351590}
}

@article{fds351591,
   Author = {Brandt, S and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {German Children’s Use of Word Order and Case Marking to
             Interpret Simple and Complex Sentences: Testing Differences
             Between Constructions and Lexical Items},
   Journal = {Language Learning and Development},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {156-182},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2015.1052448},
   Abstract = {ABSTRACT: Children and adults follow cues such as case
             marking and word order in their assignment of semantic roles
             in simple transitives (e.g., the dog chased the cat). It has
             been suggested that the same cues are used for the
             interpretation of complex sentences, such as transitive
             relative clauses (RCs) (e.g., that’s the dog that chased
             the cat) (Bates, Devescovi, & D’Amico, 1999). We used a
             pointing paradigm to test German-speaking 3-, 4-, and
             6-year-old children’s sensitivity to case marking and word
             order in their interpretation of simple transitives and
             transitive RCs. In Experiment 1, case marking was ambiguous.
             The only cue available was word order. In Experiment 2, case
             was marked on lexical NPs or demonstrative pronouns. In
             Experiment 3, case was marked on lexical NPs or personal
             pronouns. Whereas the younger children mainly followed word
             order, the older children were more likely to base their
             interpretations on the more reliable case-marking cue. In
             most cases, children from both age groups were more likely
             to use these cues in their interpretation of simple
             transitives than in their interpretation of transitive RCs.
             Finally, children paid more attention to nominative case
             when it was marked on first-person personal pronouns than
             when it was marked on third-person lexical NPs or
             demonstrative pronouns, such as der Löwe ‘the-NOM lion’
             or der ‘he-NOM.’ They were able to successfully
             integrate this case-marking cue in their sentence processing
             even when it appeared late in the sentence. We discuss four
             potential reasons for these differences across development,
             constructions, and lexical items. (1) Older children are
             relatively more sensitive to cue reliability. (2) Word order
             is more reliable in simple transitives than in transitive
             RCs. (3) The processing of case marking might initially be
             item-specific. (4) The processing of case marking might
             depend on its saliency and position in the
             sentence.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15475441.2015.1052448},
   Key = {fds351591}
}

@article{fds323843,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The ontogeny of cultural learning},
   Journal = {Current Opinion in Psychology},
   Volume = {8},
   Pages = {1-4},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.008},
   Abstract = {All primates engage in one or another form of social
             learning. Humans engage in cultural learning. From very
             early in ontogeny human infants and young children do not
             just learn useful things from others, they conform to others
             in order to affiliate with them and to identify with the
             cultural group. The cultural group normatively expects such
             conformity, and adults actively instruct children so as to
             ensure it. Young children learn from this instruction how
             the world is viewed and how it works in their culture. These
             special forms of cultural learning enable powerful and
             species-unique processes of cumulative cultural
             evolution.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.008},
   Key = {fds323843}
}

@article{fds323258,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Hardecker, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Preschoolers understand the normativity of cooperatively
             structured competition.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {143},
   Pages = {34-47},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.014},
   Abstract = {Human institutional practices often involve competition
             within a cooperative structure of mutually accepted rules.
             In a competitive game, for instance, we not only expect
             adherence to the rules of the game but also expect an
             opponent who tries to win and, thus, follows a rational
             game-playing strategy. We had 3- and 5-year-olds (N=48) play
             for a prize against an opponent (a puppet) who played either
             rationally (trying to win) or irrationally (helping the
             children to win) while either following or breaking the
             rules of the game. Both age groups performed costly protest
             against an opponent who followed the rules but played
             irrationally by helping the children to win. When facing a
             rule-breaking opponent, 3-year-olds protested only the rule
             breaches of an irrational opponent but not irrational play.
             Five-year-olds also protested the rule breaches of a
             rational opponent, but in contrast to the 3-year-olds, they
             protested irrational behavior even in the context of rule
             breaches. Moreover, many children, in particular
             3-year-olds, refrained from protesting. These findings
             suggest that 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, fully
             understand the dual-level normative structure of
             cooperatively regulated competition.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.014},
   Key = {fds323258}
}

@article{fds323259,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Rakoczy, H and Mietzsch, T and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young Children Understand the Role of Agreement in
             Establishing Arbitrary Norms-But Unanimity Is
             Key.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {612-626},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12510},
   Abstract = {Human cultural groups value conformity to arbitrary norms
             (e.g., rituals, games) that are the result of collective
             "agreement." Ninety-six 3-year-olds had the opportunity to
             agree upon arbitrary norms with puppets. Results revealed
             that children normatively enforced these novel norms only on
             a deviator who had actually entered into the agreement (not
             on dissenting or ignorant individuals). Interestingly, any
             dissent during the norm-setting process (even if a majority
             of 90% preferred one course of action) prevented children
             from seeing a norm as established for anyone at all. These
             findings suggest that even young children understand
             something of the role of agreement in establishing mutually
             binding social norms, but that their notion of norm
             formation may be confined to conditions of
             unanimity.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12510},
   Key = {fds323259}
}

@article{fds351592,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Mammen, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Preschoolers use common ground in their justificatory
             reasoning with peers.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {52},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {423-429},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000089},
   Abstract = {In the context of joint decision-making, we investigated
             whether preschoolers alter the informativeness of their
             justifications depending on the common ground that they
             share with their partner. Pairs of 3- and 5-year-olds (N =
             146) were introduced to a novel animal with unique
             characteristics (e.g., eating rocks). In the common ground
             condition, the children learned about the animal together.
             In the one-expert condition, one learned about it, the other
             was naïve. In the two-experts condition, children learned
             about it separately. Later, the pairs had to decide together
             on 3 items that the novel animal might need. Both age groups
             referred to the unique characteristics of the animal in
             their justifications more in the 2 conditions without common
             ground than in the common ground condition. Thus,
             preschoolers begin to use common ground flexibly in their
             justifications and reason-giving in peer
             interactions.},
   Doi = {10.1037/dev0000089},
   Key = {fds351592}
}

@article{fds351593,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Comprehension of iconic gestures by chimpanzees and human
             children.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {142},
   Pages = {1-17},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.001},
   Abstract = {Iconic gestures-communicative acts using hand or body
             movements that resemble their referent-figure prominently in
             theories of language evolution and development. This study
             contrasted the abilities of chimpanzees (N=11) and
             4-year-old human children (N=24) to comprehend novel iconic
             gestures. Participants learned to retrieve rewards from
             apparatuses in two distinct locations, each requiring a
             different action. In the test, a human adult informed the
             participant where to go by miming the action needed to
             obtain the reward. Children used the iconic gestures (more
             than arbitrary gestures) to locate the reward, whereas
             chimpanzees did not. Some children also used arbitrary
             gestures in the same way, but only after they had previously
             shown comprehension for iconic gestures. Over time,
             chimpanzees learned to associate iconic gestures with the
             appropriate location faster than arbitrary gestures,
             suggesting at least some recognition of the iconicity
             involved. These results demonstrate the importance of
             iconicity in referential communication.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.001},
   Key = {fds351593}
}

@article{fds322246,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Svetlova, M and Johe, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons
             for allocating resources unequally},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {37},
   Pages = {42-52},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.11.001},
   Abstract = {Recent research on distributive justice suggests that young
             children prefer equal distributions. But sometimes unequal
             distributions are justified, such as when some individuals
             deserve more than others based on merit, need, or
             agreed-upon rules. When and how do children start
             incorporating such factors in their distributive decisions?
             Three-, 5-, and 8-year-old children (N= 72) had the
             opportunity to allocate several items to two individuals.
             One individual was neutral and the other provided a reason
             why she should be favored. Three of these reasons were
             legitimate (based on merit, need, or agreed-upon rules)
             whereas a fourth was idiosyncratic ("I just want more."). We
             found that with age, children's equality preference
             diminished and their acceptance of various reasons for
             privileged treatment increased. It was not until 8 years,
             however, that they differentiated between legitimate and
             idiosyncratic reasons for inequality. These findings suggest
             that children's sense of distributive justice develops from
             an early equality preference to a more flexible
             understanding of the basic normative reasons that inequality
             may, in some cases, be just.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.11.001},
   Key = {fds322246}
}

@article{fds323260,
   Author = {Vogelsang, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Giving Is Nicer than Taking: Preschoolers Reciprocate Based
             on the Social Intentions of the Distributor.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e0147539},
   Publisher = {Public Library of Science (PLoS)},
   Editor = {di Pellegrino, G},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147539},
   Abstract = {Recent research has found that even preschoolers give more
             resources to others who have previously given resources to
             them, but the psychological bases of this reciprocity are
             unknown. In our study, a puppet distributed resources
             between herself and a child by taking some from a pile in
             front of the child or else by giving some from a pile in
             front of herself. Although the resulting distributions were
             identical, three- and five-year-olds reciprocated less
             generously when the puppet had taken rather than given
             resources. This suggests that children's judgments about
             resource distribution are more about the social intentions
             of the distributor and the social framing of the
             distributional act than about the amount of resources
             obtained. In order to rule out that the differences in the
             children's reciprocal behavior were merely due to
             experiencing gains and losses, we conducted a follow-up
             study. Here, three- and-five year olds won or lost resources
             in a lottery draw and could then freely give or take
             resources to/from a puppet, respectively. In this study,
             they did not respond differently after winning vs. losing
             resources.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0147539},
   Key = {fds323260}
}

@article{fds351594,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Precís of a natural history of human thinking},
   Journal = {Journal of Social Ontology},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {59-64},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/jso-2015-0041},
   Abstract = {A précis of Michael Tomasello, A Natural History of Human
             Thinking (Harvard University Press, 2014).},
   Doi = {10.1515/jso-2015-0041},
   Key = {fds351594}
}

@article{fds351596,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Response to commentators},
   Journal = {Journal of Social Ontology},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {117-123},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/jso-2015-0042},
   Abstract = {This paper is a reply to the comments by Henrike Moll,
             Glenda Satne, Ladislav Koreň and Michael Schmitz on Michael
             Tomasello, A Natural History of Human Thinking (Harvard
             University Press, 2014).},
   Doi = {10.1515/jso-2015-0042},
   Key = {fds351596}
}

@article{fds351597,
   Author = {Plötner, M and Over, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {What Is a Group? Young Children's Perceptions of Different
             Types of Groups and Group Entitativity.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e0152001},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0152001},
   Abstract = {To date, developmental research on groups has focused mainly
             on in-group biases and intergroup relations. However, little
             is known about children's general understanding of social
             groups and their perceptions of different forms of group. In
             this study, 5- to 6-year-old children were asked to evaluate
             prototypes of four key types of groups: an intimacy group
             (friends), a task group (people who are collaborating), a
             social category (people who look alike), and a loose
             association (people who coincidently meet at a tram stop).
             In line with previous work with adults, the vast majority of
             children perceived the intimacy group, task group, and
             social category, but not the loose association, to possess
             entitativity, that is, to be a 'real group.' In addition,
             children evaluated group member properties, social
             relations, and social obligations differently in each type
             of group, demonstrating that young children are able to
             distinguish between different types of in-group relations.
             The origins of the general group typology used by adults
             thus appear early in development. These findings contribute
             to our knowledge about children's intuitive understanding of
             groups and group members' behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0152001},
   Key = {fds351597}
}

@article{fds351598,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The effects of being watched on resource acquisition in
             chimpanzees and human children.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {147-151},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0920-y},
   Abstract = {Animals react in many different ways to being watched by
             others. In the context of cooperation, many theories
             emphasize reputational effects: Individuals should cooperate
             more if other potential cooperators are watching. In the
             context of competition, individuals might want to show off
             their strength and prowess if other potential competitors
             are watching. In the current study, we observed chimpanzees
             and human children in three experimental conditions
             involving resource acquisition: Participants were either in
             the presence of a passive observer (observed condition), an
             active observer who engaged in the same task as the
             participant (competition condition), or in the presence of
             but not directly observed by a conspecific (mere presence
             condition). While both species worked to acquire more
             resources in the competition condition, children but not
             chimpanzees also worked to acquire more resources in the
             observer condition (compared to the mere presence
             condition). These results suggest evolutionary continuity
             with regard to competition-based observer effects, but an
             additional observer effect in young children, potentially
             arising from an evolutionary-based concern for cooperative
             reputation.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-015-0920-y},
   Key = {fds351598}
}

@article{fds351599,
   Author = {Bohn, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Communication about absent entities in great apes and human
             infants.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {145},
   Pages = {63-72},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.009},
   Abstract = {There is currently debate about the extent to which
             non-linguistic beings such as human infants and great apes
             are capable of absent reference. In a series of experiments
             we investigated the flexibility and specificity of great
             apes' (N=36) and 12 month-old infants' (N=40) requests for
             absent entities. Subjects had the choice between requesting
             visible objects directly and using the former location of a
             depleted option to request more of these now-absent
             entities. Importantly, we systematically varied the quality
             of the present and absent options. We found that great apes
             as well as human infants flexibly adjusted their requests
             for absent entities to these contextual variations and only
             requested absent entities when the visible option was of
             lower quality than the absent option. These results suggest
             that the most basic cognitive capacities for absent
             reference do not depend on language and are shared by humans
             and their closest living relatives.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.009},
   Key = {fds351599}
}

@article{fds351600,
   Author = {Ulber, J and Hamann, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How 18- and 24-month-old peers divide resources among
             themselves.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {140},
   Pages = {228-244},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.009},
   Abstract = {Young children are often considered "selfish" with resources
             because they are reluctant to give up things already in
             their possession (e.g., as in dictator games). In the
             current two studies, we presented pairs of 18- and
             24-month-old toddlers with various situations involving
             resources that no one possessed ahead of time. We observed
             very few instances of individuals attempting to monopolize
             the resources; rather, the pair peaceably divided them such
             that each child got something. Equal divisions--even
             involving one child sacrificing his or her own resources to
             establish equality-were especially pronounced when children
             were acting together jointly even in the absence of active
             collaboration. Children's divisions were also influenced by
             cues to ownership such as a spatial pre-division of
             resources and resources marked by color (and originally
             spatially associated with one individual). These results
             suggest that young children are not selfish, but instead
             rather generous, with resources when they are dividing them
             among themselves.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.009},
   Key = {fds351600}
}

@article{fds351601,
   Author = {Grocke, P and Rossano, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal
             resource distributions if the procedure provides equal
             opportunities.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {140},
   Pages = {197-210},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.008},
   Abstract = {When it is not possible to distribute resources equitably to
             everyone, people look for an equitable or just procedure. In
             the current study, we investigated young children's sense of
             procedural justice. We tested 32 triads of 5-year-olds in a
             new resource allocation game. Triads were confronted with
             three unequal reward packages and then agreed on a procedure
             to allocate them among themselves. To allocate the rewards,
             they needed to use a "wheel of fortune." Half of the groups
             played with a fair wheel (where each child had an equal
             chance of obtaining each reward package), and the other half
             played with an unfair wheel. We analyzed children's
             interactions when using the wheel and conducted an interview
             with each child after the game was over. Children using the
             unfair wheel often decided to change the rules of the game,
             and they also rated it as an unfair procedure in the
             interview. In contrast, children who played with the fair
             wheel were mostly accepting of both the outcome and the
             procedure. Overall, we found that children as young as
             preschool age are already sensitive not only to distributive
             justice but to procedural justice as well.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.008},
   Key = {fds351601}
}

@article{fds323261,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Misch, A and Hernandez-Lloreda, V and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Uniquely human self-control begins at school
             age.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {979-993},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12272},
   Abstract = {Human beings have remarkable skills of self-control, but the
             evolutionary origins of these skills are unknown. Here we
             compare children at 3 and 6 years of age with one of humans'
             two nearest relatives, chimpanzees, on a battery of
             reactivity and self-control tasks. Three-year-old children
             and chimpanzees were very similar in their abilities to
             resist an impulse for immediate gratification, repeat a
             previously successful action, attend to a distracting noise,
             and quit in the face of repeated failure. Six-year-old
             children were more skillful than either 3-year-olds or
             chimpanzees at controlling their impulses. These results
             suggest that humans' most fundamental skills of self-control
             - as part of the overall decision-making process - are a
             part of their general great ape heritage, and that their
             species-unique skills of self-control begin at around the
             age at which many children begin formal schooling.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12272},
   Key = {fds323261}
}

@article{fds351602,
   Author = {Butler, LP and Schmidt, MFH and Bürgel, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children use pedagogical cues to modulate the strength
             of normative inferences.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {476-488},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12108},
   Abstract = {Young children understand pedagogical demonstrations as
             conveying generic, kind-relevant information. But, in some
             contexts, they also see almost any confident, intentional
             action on a novel artefact as normative and thus generic,
             regardless of whether this action was pedagogically
             demonstrated for them. Thus, although pedagogy may not be
             necessary for inferences to the generic, it may nevertheless
             be sufficient to produce inductive inferences on which the
             child relies more strongly. This study addresses this
             tension by bridging the literature on normative reasoning
             with that on social learning and inductive inference.
             Three-year-old children learned about a novel artefact from
             either a pedagogical or non-pedagogical demonstration, and
             then, a series of new actors acted on that artefact in novel
             ways. Although children protested normatively in both
             conditions (e.g., 'No, not like that'), they persisted
             longer in enforcing the learned norms in the face of
             repeated non-conformity by the new actors. This finding
             suggests that not all generic, normative inferences are
             created equal, but rather they depend - at least for their
             strength - on the nature of the acquisition
             process.},
   Doi = {10.1111/bjdp.12108},
   Key = {fds351602}
}

@article{fds351603,
   Author = {Plötner, M and Over, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on
             children's prosocial behavior, liking, affiliation, and
             trust.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {139},
   Pages = {161-173},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.05.008},
   Abstract = {Recent theoretical work has highlighted potential links
             between interpersonal collaboration and group membership in
             the evolution of human sociality. Here we compared the
             effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on
             young children's prosocial behavior (i.e., helping and
             resource allocation), liking, affiliation, and trust. In a
             design that matched as closely as possible these two ways of
             connecting with others, we showed that 5-year-olds' behavior
             was affected similarly by collaboration and minimal-group
             membership; both increased children's preference for their
             partners on multiple dimensions and produced overall effects
             of a similar magnitude. In contrast, 3.5-year-olds did not
             have a strong preference for either collaborators or minimal
             in-group members. Thus, both collaboration and minimal-group
             membership are similarly effective in their influence on
             children's prosocial behavior and social
             preferences.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.05.008},
   Key = {fds351603}
}

@article{fds361386,
   Title = {Michael Tomasello: Award for Distinguished Scientific
             Contributions.},
   Journal = {The American psychologist},
   Volume = {70},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {680-682},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039789},
   Abstract = {The APA Awards for Distinguished Scientific Contributions
             are presented to persons who, in the opinion of the
             Committee on Scientific Awards, have made distinguished
             theoretical or empirical contributions to basic research in
             psychology. One of the 2015 award winners is Michael
             Tomasello, who received this award for "outstanding
             empirical and theoretical contributions to understanding
             what makes the human mind unique. Michael Tomasello's
             pioneering research on the origins of social cognition has
             led to revolutionary insights in both developmental
             psychology and primate cognition." Tomasello's award
             citation, biography, and a selected bibliography are
             presented here.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0039789},
   Key = {fds361386}
}

@article{fds351604,
   Author = {Grosse, K and Call, J and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Differences in the Ability of Apes and Children to Instruct
             Others Using Gestures},
   Journal = {Language Learning and Development},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {310-330},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2014.955246},
   Abstract = {In all human cultures, people gesture iconically. However,
             the evolutionary basis of iconic gestures is unknown. In
             this study, chimpanzees and bonobos, and 2- and 3-year-old
             children, learned how to operate two apparatuses to get
             rewards. Then, at test, only a human adult had access to the
             apparatuses, and participants could instruct her about how
             to obtain the rewards. Children frequently produced
             appropriate iconic gestures, but with the exception of one
             human-raised chimpanzee, great apes did not gesture
             iconically. However, chimpanzees pointed to a reward outside
             the apparatus in another experimental condition, showing
             their motivation and ability to communicate with the human
             to request it. They also manipulated a duplicate apparatus
             in appropriate ways, though it was unclear if they did this
             to communicate with the human. Although great apes may have
             some of the prerequisite skills involved, iconic gestures
             come naturally to humans in a way that they do not for great
             apes.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15475441.2014.955246},
   Key = {fds351604}
}

@article{fds351605,
   Author = {Cameron-Faulkner, T and Theakston, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The relationship between infant holdout and gives, and
             pointing},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {576-586},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/infa.12085},
   Abstract = {We provide an analysis of holdout and giving (Ho&G)
             behaviours in prelinguistic infants and investigate their
             relationship with index finger pointing. The frequency of
             Ho&Gs at 10 and 11 months along with the length of the
             following social interaction correlated with index finger
             pointing at 12 months. We conclude that Ho&Gs are a
             precursor to index finger pointing and that this provides
             support for social-pragmatic approaches to communicative
             development.},
   Doi = {10.1111/infa.12085},
   Key = {fds351605}
}

@article{fds351606,
   Author = {Karg, K and Schmelz, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees strategically manipulate what others can
             see.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1069-1076},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0875-z},
   Abstract = {Humans often strategically manipulate the informational
             access of others to their own advantage. Although
             chimpanzees know what others can and cannot see, it is
             unclear whether they can strategically manipulate others'
             visual access. In this study, chimpanzees were given the
             opportunity to save food for themselves by concealing it
             from a human competitor and also to get more food for
             themselves by revealing it to a human cooperator. When
             knowing that a competitor was approaching, chimpanzees kept
             more food hidden (left it covered) than when expecting a
             cooperator to approach. When the experimenter was already at
             the location of the hidden food, they actively revealed less
             food to the competitor than to the cooperator. They did not
             actively hide food (cover up food in the open) from the
             competitor, however. Chimpanzees thus strategically
             manipulated what another could see in order to maximize
             their payoffs and showed their ability to plan for future
             situations.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-015-0875-z},
   Key = {fds351606}
}

@article{fds351607,
   Author = {Schmerse, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children use shared experience to interpret definite
             reference.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1146-1157},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000914000555},
   Abstract = {We investigated whether children at the ages of two and
             three years understand that a speaker's use of the definite
             article specifies a referent that is in common ground
             between speaker and listener. An experimenter and a child
             engaged in joint actions in which the experimenter chose one
             of three similar objects of the same category to perform an
             action. In subsequent interactions children were asked to
             get 'the X' or 'a X'. When children were instructed with the
             definite article they chose the shared object significantly
             more often than when they were instructed with the
             indefinite article in which case children's choice was at
             chance. The findings show that in their third year children
             use shared experiences to interpret the speaker's
             communicative intention underlying her referential choice.
             The results are discussed with respect to children's
             representation of linguistic categories and the role of
             joint action for establishing common ground.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000914000555},
   Key = {fds351607}
}

@article{fds359906,
   Author = {Grassmann, S and Schulze, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their
             exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel
             words},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {6},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01200},
   Abstract = {When children are learning a novel object label, they tend
             to exclude as possible referents familiar objects for which
             they already have a name. In the current study, we wanted to
             know if children would behave in this same way regardless of
             how well they knew the name of potential referent objects,
             specifically, whether they could only comprehend it or they
             could both comprehend and produce it. Sixty-six monolingual
             German-speaking 2-, 3-, and 4-year-old children participated
             in two experimental sessions. In one session the familiar
             objects were chosen such that their labels were in the
             children’s productive vocabularies, and in the other
             session the familiar objects were chosen such that their
             labels were only in the children’s receptive vocabularies.
             Results indicated that children at all three ages were more
             likely to exclude a familiar object as the potential
             referent of the novel word if they could comprehend and
             produce its name rather than comprehend its name only.
             Indeed, level of word knowledge as operationalized in this
             way was a better predictor than was age. These results are
             discussed in the context of current theories of word
             learning by exclusion.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01200},
   Key = {fds359906}
}

@article{fds323262,
   Author = {Schäfer, M and Haun, DBM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Fair Is Not Fair Everywhere.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1252-1260},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797615586188},
   Abstract = {Distributing the spoils of a joint enterprise on the basis
             of work contribution or relative productivity seems natural
             to the modern Western mind. But such notions of merit-based
             distributive justice may be culturally constructed norms
             that vary with the social and economic structure of a group.
             In the present research, we showed that children from three
             different cultures have very different ideas about
             distributive justice. Whereas children from a modern Western
             society distributed the spoils of a joint enterprise
             precisely in proportion to productivity, children from a
             gerontocratic pastoralist society in Africa did not take
             merit into account at all. Children from a partially
             hunter-gatherer, egalitarian African culture distributed the
             spoils more equally than did the other two cultures, with
             merit playing only a limited role. This pattern of results
             suggests that some basic notions of distributive justice are
             not universal intuitions of the human species but rather
             culturally constructed behavioral norms.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797615586188},
   Key = {fds323262}
}

@article{fds351608,
   Author = {Theakston, AL and Ibbotson, P and Freudenthal, D and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Productivity of Noun Slots in Verb Frames.},
   Journal = {Cognitive science},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1369-1395},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12216},
   Abstract = {Productivity is a central concept in the study of language
             and language acquisition. As a test case for exploring the
             notion of productivity, we focus on the noun slots of verb
             frames, such as __want__, __see__, and __get__. We develop a
             novel combination of measures designed to assess both the
             flexibility and creativity of use in these slots. We do so
             using a rigorously controlled sample of child speech and
             child directed speech from three English-speaking children
             between the ages of 2-3 years and their caregivers. We find
             different levels of creativity and flexibility between the
             adult and child samples for some measures, for some slots,
             and for some developmental periods. We discuss these
             differences in the context of verb frame semantics,
             conventionality versus creativity and child errors, and draw
             some tentative conclusions regarding developmental changes
             in children's early grammatical representations.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cogs.12216},
   Key = {fds351608}
}

@article{fds351609,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Focusing and shifting attention in human children (Homo
             sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {129},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {268-274},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039384},
   Abstract = {Humans often must coordinate co-occurring activities, and
             their flexible skills for doing so would seem to be uniquely
             powerful. In 2 studies, we compared 4- and 5-year-old
             children and one of humans' nearest relatives, chimpanzees,
             in their ability to focus and shift their attention when
             necessary. The results of Study 1 showed that 4-year-old
             children and chimpanzees were very similar in their ability
             to monitor two identical devices and to sequentially switch
             between the two to collect a reward, and that they were less
             successful at doing so than 5-year-old children. In Study 2,
             which required subjects to alternate between two different
             tasks, one of which had rewards continuously available
             whereas the other one only occasionally released rewards, no
             species differences were found. These results suggest that
             chimpanzees and human children share some fundamental
             attentional control skills, but that such abilities continue
             to develop during human ontogeny, resulting in the uniquely
             human capacity to succeed at complex multitasking.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0039384},
   Key = {fds351609}
}

@article{fds359907,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Novel paradigms to measure variability of behavior in early
             childhood: posture, gaze, and pupil dilation},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {6},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00858},
   Abstract = {A central challenge of investigating the underlying
             mechanisms of and the individual differences in young
             children’s behavior is the measurement of the internal
             physiological mechanism and the involved expressive
             emotions. Here, we illustrate two paradigms that assess
             concurrent indicators of both children’s social perception
             as well as their emotional expression. In one set of
             studies, children view situations while their eye movements
             are mapped onto a live scene. In these studies, children’s
             internal arousal is measured via changes in their pupil
             dilation by using eye tracking technology. In another set of
             studies, we measured children’s emotional expression via
             changes in their upper-body posture by using depth sensor
             imaging technology. Together, these paradigms can provide
             new insights into the internal mechanism and outward
             emotional expression involved in young children’s
             behavior.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00858},
   Key = {fds359907}
}

@article{fds323263,
   Author = {Karg, K and Schmelz, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The goggles experiment: Can chimpanzees use self-experience
             to infer what a competitor can see?},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {105},
   Pages = {211-221},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.028},
   Abstract = {In two experiments, we investigated whether chimpanzees, Pan
             troglodytes, can use self-experience to infer what another
             sees. Subjects first gained self-experience with the visual
             properties of an object (either opaque or see-through). In a
             subsequent test phase, a human experimenter interacted with
             the object and we tested whether chimpanzees understood that
             the experimenter experienced the object as opaque or as
             see-through. Crucially, in the test phase, the object seemed
             opaque to the subject in all cases (while the experimenter
             could see through the one that they had experienced as
             see-through before), such that she had to use her previous
             self-experience with the object to correctly infer whether
             the experimenter could or could not see when looking at the
             object. Chimpanzees did not attribute their previous
             self-experience with the object to the experimenter in a
             gaze-following task (experiment 1); however, they did so
             successfully in a competitive context (experiment 2). We
             conclude that chimpanzees successfully used their
             self-experience to infer what the competitor sees. We
             discuss our results in relation to the well-known 'goggles
             experiment' and address alternative explanations.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.028},
   Key = {fds323263}
}

@article{fds351610,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Schmidt, MFH and Rost, L and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Teaching versus enforcing game rules in preschoolers' peer
             interactions.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {135},
   Pages = {93-101},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.005},
   Abstract = {Children use normative language in two key contexts: when
             teaching others and when enforcing social norms. We
             presented pairs of 3- and 5-year-old peers (N=192) with a
             sorting game in two experimental conditions (in addition to
             a third baseline condition). In the teaching condition, one
             child was knowledgeable, whereas the other child was
             ignorant and so in need of instruction. In the enforcement
             condition, children learned conflicting rules so that each
             child was making mistakes from the other's point of view.
             When teaching rules to an ignorant partner, both age groups
             used generic normative language ("Bunnies go here"). When
             enforcing rules on a rule-breaking partner, 3-year-olds used
             normative utterances that were not generic and aimed at
             correcting individual behavior ("No, this goes there"),
             whereas 5-year-olds again used generic normative language,
             perhaps because they discerned that instruction was needed
             in this case as well. Young children normatively correct
             peers differently depending on their assessment of what
             their wayward partners need to bring them back into
             line.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.005},
   Key = {fds351610}
}

@article{fds351618,
   Author = {Kanngiesser, P and Rossano, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Late Emergence of the First Possession Heuristic: Evidence
             From a Small-Scale Culture},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {86},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1282-1289},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12365},
   Abstract = {Western preschool children often assign ownership based on
             first possession and some theorists have proposed that this
             judgment might be an early emerging, innate bias. Five- to
             9-year-olds (n = 112) from a small-scale group in Kenya
             (Kikuyu) watched videotaped interactions of two women
             passing an object. The object's starting position and the
             women's gestures were varied. Use of the first possession
             heuristic increased with age, and 8- to 9-year-olds
             performed similarly to German 5-year-olds (n = 24). Starting
             position and gestures had no effect. A control study
             confirmed that 5-year-old Kikuyus (n = 20) understood the
             video material. The findings reveal that the first
             possession heuristic follows different developmental
             trajectories cross-culturally and stress the role of
             children's sociocultural environment.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12365},
   Key = {fds351618}
}

@article{fds351611,
   Author = {Riedl, K and Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Restorative Justice in Children.},
   Journal = {Current biology : CB},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {13},
   Pages = {1731-1735},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.014},
   Abstract = {An important, and perhaps uniquely human, mechanism for
             maintaining cooperation against free riders is third-party
             punishment. Our closest living relatives, chimpanzees, will
             not punish third parties even though they will do so when
             personally affected. Until recently, little attention has
             been paid to how punishment and a sense of justice develop
             in children. Children respond to norm violations. They are
             more likely to share with a puppet that helped another
             individual as opposed to one who behaved harmfully, and they
             show a preference for seeing a harmful doll rather than a
             victim punished. By 6 years of age, children will pay a cost
             to punish fictional and real peers, and the threat of
             punishment will lead preschoolers to behave more generously.
             However, little is known about what motivates a sense of
             justice in children. We gave 3- and 5-year-old children--the
             youngest ages yet tested--the opportunity to remove items
             and prevent a puppet from gaining a reward for second- and
             third-party violations (experiment 1), and we gave
             3-year-olds the opportunity to restore items (experiment 2).
             Children were as likely to engage in third-party
             interventions as they were when personally affected, yet
             they did not discriminate among the different sources of
             harm for the victim. When given a range of options,
             3-year-olds chose restoration over removal. It appears that
             a sense of justice centered on harm caused to victims
             emerges early in childhood and highlights the value of
             third-party interventions for human cooperation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.014},
   Key = {fds351611}
}

@article{fds323264,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Wyman, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children use salience to solve coordination
             problems.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {495-501},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12224},
   Abstract = {Humans are routinely required to coordinate with others.
             When communication is not possible, adults often achieve
             this by using salient cues in the environment (e.g. going to
             the Eiffel Tower, as an obvious meeting point). To explore
             the development of this capacity, we presented dyads of 3-,
             5-, and 8-year-olds (N = 144) with a coordination problem:
             Two balls had to be inserted into the same of four boxes to
             obtain a reward. Identical pictures were attached to three
             boxes whereas a unique--and thus salient--picture was
             attached to the fourth. Children either received one ball
             each, and so had to choose the same box (experimental
             condition), or they received both balls and could get the
             reward independently (control condition). In all cases,
             children could neither communicate nor see each other's
             choices. Children were significantly more likely to choose
             the salient option in the experimental condition than in the
             control condition. However, only the two older age groups
             chose the salient box above chance levels. This study is the
             first to show that children from at least age 5 can solve
             coordination problems by converging on a salient
             solution.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12224},
   Key = {fds323264}
}

@article{fds351612,
   Author = {Grünloh, T and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young Children’s Intonational Marking of New, Given and
             Contrastive Referents},
   Journal = {Language Learning and Development},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {95-127},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2014.889530},
   Abstract = {In the current study we investigate whether 2- and
             3-year-old German children use intonation productively to
             mark the informational status of referents. Using a
             story-telling task, we compared children’s and adults’
             intonational realization via pitch accent (H*, L* and
             de-accentuation) of New, Given, and Contrastive referents.
             Both children and adults distinguished these elements with
             different pitch accents. Adults, however, de-accented Given
             information much more often than the children, especially
             the younger children. Since a failure to de-accent Given
             information may be a characteristic of caregiver speech, in
             a second study we tested how caregivers talking to their
             young children realize Given and New referents. In this
             discourse situation, the caregivers quite often failed to
             de-accent Given information, raising the possibility that
             the younger children were simply reproducing the pitch
             accents they had heard adults using.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15475441.2014.889530},
   Key = {fds351612}
}

@article{fds351613,
   Author = {Plötner, M and Over, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children show the bystander effect in helping
             situations.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {499-506},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797615569579},
   Abstract = {Much research in social psychology has shown that otherwise
             helpful people often fail to help when bystanders are
             present. Research in developmental psychology has shown that
             even very young children help and that the presence of
             others can actually increase helping in some cases. In the
             current study, in contrast, 5-year-old children helped an
             experimenter at very high levels when they were alone but
             helped significantly less often in the presence of
             bystanders who were potentially available to help. In
             another condition designed to elucidate the mechanism
             underlying the effect, children's helping was not reduced
             when bystanders were present but confined behind a barrier
             and thus unable to help (a condition that has not been run
             in previous studies with adults). Young children thus show
             the bystander effect, and it is due not to social
             referencing or shyness to act in front of others but,
             rather, to a sense of a diffusion of responsibility.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797615569579},
   Key = {fds351613}
}

@article{fds351614,
   Author = {Moore, R and Mueller, B and Kaminski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Two-year-old children but not domestic dogs understand
             communicative intentions without language, gestures, or
             gaze.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {232-242},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12206},
   Abstract = {Infants can see someone pointing to one of two buckets and
             infer that the toy they are seeking is hidden inside. Great
             apes do not succeed in this task, but, surprisingly,
             domestic dogs do. However, whether children and dogs
             understand these communicative acts in the same way is not
             yet known. To test this possibility, an experimenter did not
             point, look, or extend any part of her body towards either
             bucket, but instead lifted and shook one via a centrally
             pulled rope. She did this either intentionally or
             accidentally, and did or did not address her act to the
             subject using ostensive cues. Young 2-year-old children but
             not dogs understood the experimenter's act in intentional
             conditions. While ostensive pulling of the rope made no
             difference to children's success, it actually hindered dogs'
             performance. We conclude that while human children may be
             capable of inferring communicative intent from a wide
             variety actions, so long as these actions are performed
             intentionally, dogs are likely to be less flexible in this
             respect. Their understanding of communicative intention may
             be more dependent upon bodily markers of communicative
             intent, including gaze, orientation, extended limbs, and
             vocalizations. This may be because humans have come under
             selective pressure to develop skills for communicating with
             absent interlocutors - where bodily co-presence is not
             possible.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12206},
   Key = {fds351614}
}

@article{fds351615,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Wyman, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Conforming to coordinate: children use majority information
             for peer coordination.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {136-147},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12078},
   Abstract = {Humans are constantly required to coordinate their behaviour
             with others. As this often relies on everyone's convergence
             on the same strategy (e.g., driving on the left side of the
             road), a common solution is to conform to majority
             behaviour. In this study, we presented 5-year-old children
             with a coordination problem: To retrieve some rewards, they
             had to choose the same of four options as a peer partner--in
             reality a stooge--whose decision they were unable to see.
             Before making a choice, they watched a video showing how
             other children from their partner's peer group had behaved;
             a majority chose the same option and a minority chose a
             different one. In a control condition, children watched the
             same video but could then retrieve the reward irrespective
             of their partner's choice (i.e., no coordination was
             necessary). Children followed the majority more often when
             coordination was required. Moreover, conformers mostly
             justified their choices by referring to the majority from
             the video demonstration. This study is the first to show
             that young children are able to strategically coordinate
             decisions with peers by conforming to the
             majority.},
   Doi = {10.1111/bjdp.12078},
   Key = {fds351615}
}

@article{fds351616,
   Author = {Schulze, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {18-month-olds comprehend indirect communicative
             acts.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {136},
   Pages = {91-98},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.036},
   Abstract = {From soon after their first birthdays young children are
             able to make inferences from a communicator's referential
             act (e.g., pointing to a container) to her overall social
             goal for communication (e.g., to inform that a searched-for
             toy is inside; see Behne, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2005;
             Behne, Liszkowski, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2012). But in
             such cases the inferential distance between referential act
             and communicative intention is still fairly close, as both
             container and searched-for toy lie in the direction of the
             pointing gesture. In the current study we tested 18- and
             26-month-old children in a situation in which referential
             act and communicative goal were more distant: In the midst
             of a game, the child needed a certain toy. The experimenter
             then held up a key (that they knew in common ground could be
             used to open a container) to the child ostensively. In two
             control conditions the experimenter either inadvertently
             moved the key and so drew the child's attention to it
             non-ostensively or else held up the key for her own
             inspection intentionally but non-communicatively. Children
             of both ages took only the ostensive showing of the key, not
             the accidental moving or the non-ostensive but intentional
             inspection of the key, as an indirect request to take the
             key and open the container to retrieve the toy inside. From
             soon after they start acquiring language young children thus
             are able to infer a communicator's social goal for
             communication not only from directly-referential acts, but
             from more indirect communicative acts as
             well.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.036},
   Key = {fds351616}
}

@article{fds323266,
   Author = {Rossano, F and Fiedler, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Preschoolers' understanding of the role of communication and
             cooperation in establishing property rights.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {51},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {176-184},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038493},
   Abstract = {Property as a social "agreement" comprises both a
             communicative component, in which someone makes a claim that
             she is entitled to some piece of property, and a cooperative
             component, in which others in the community respect that
             claim as legitimate. In the current study, preschool
             children were (a) given the opportunity to mark some objects
             as "theirs" (to claim them in the face of other fictitious
             children who would supposedly enter the room later); and (b)
             confronted with stickers in various spatial arrangements
             (e.g., piled up neatly vs. scattered), told that a
             fictitious child had previously chosen some for herself but
             had to suddenly leave the room, and then invited first to
             choose some stickers for themselves and second to identify
             which stickers had already been claimed by the fictitious
             child. Five-year-olds but not 3-year-olds were skillful in
             both of these tasks, demonstrating an understanding of the
             crucial role of communication in asserting property claims
             and the crucial role of cooperation in respecting
             them.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0038493},
   Key = {fds323266}
}

@article{fds351617,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees trust conspecifics to engage in low-cost
             reciprocity.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {282},
   Number = {1801},
   Pages = {20142803},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2803},
   Abstract = {Many of humans' most important social interactions rely on
             trust, including most notably among strangers. But little is
             known about the evolutionary roots of human trust. We
             presented chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with a modified
             version of the human trust game--trust in reciprocity--in
             which subjects could opt either to obtain a small but safe
             reward on their own or else to send a larger reward to a
             partner and trust her to reciprocate a part of the reward
             that she could not access herself. In a series of three
             studies, we found strong evidence that in interacting with a
             conspecific, chimpanzees show spontaneous trust in a novel
             context; flexibly adjust their level of trust to the
             trustworthiness of their partner and develop patterns of
             trusting reciprocity over time. At least in some contexts
             then, trust in reciprocity is not unique to humans, but
             rather has its evolutionary roots in the social interactions
             of humans' closest primate relatives.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2014.2803},
   Key = {fds351617}
}

@article{fds323265,
   Author = {Grueneisen, S and Wyman, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {"I know you don't know I know…" children use second-order
             false-belief reasoning for peer coordination.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {86},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {287-293},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12264},
   Abstract = {Numerous studies have investigated children's abilities to
             attribute mental states, but few have examined their ability
             to recruit these abilities in social interactions. Here,
             6-year-olds (N = 104) were tested on whether they can use
             first- and second-order false-belief understanding to
             coordinate with peers. Children adjusted their decisions in
             a coordination game in response to either their partner's
             erroneous belief or their partner's erroneous belief about
             their own belief-a result that contrasts with previous
             findings on the use of higher order "theory of mind" (TOM)
             reasoning at this age. Six-year-olds are thus able to use
             their higher order TOM capacities for peer coordination,
             which marks an important achievement in becoming competent
             social collaborators.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12264},
   Key = {fds323265}
}

@article{fds351619,
   Author = {Moore, R and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Production and Comprehension of Gestures between Orang-Utans
             (Pongo pygmaeus) in a Referential Communication
             Game.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {e0129726},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129726},
   Abstract = {Orang-utans played a communication game in two studies
             testing their ability to produce and comprehend requestive
             pointing. While the 'communicator' could see but not obtain
             hidden food, the 'donor' could release the food to the
             communicator, but could not see its location for herself.
             They could coordinate successfully if the communicator
             pointed to the food, and if the donor comprehended his
             communicative goal and responded pro-socially. In Study 1,
             one orang-utan pointed regularly and accurately for peers.
             However, they responded only rarely. In Study 2, a human
             experimenter played the communicator's role in three
             conditions, testing the apes' comprehension of points of
             different heights and different degrees of ostension. There
             was no effect of condition. However, across conditions one
             donor performed well individually, and as a group
             orang-utans' comprehension performance tended towards
             significance. We explain this on the grounds that
             comprehension required inferences that they found difficult
             - but not impossible. The finding has valuable implications
             for our thinking about the development of pointing in
             phylogeny.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0129726},
   Key = {fds351619}
}

@article{fds351620,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Floedl, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Non-egalitarian allocations among preschool peers in a
             face-to-face bargaining task.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e0120494},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120494},
   Abstract = {In face-to-face bargaining tasks human adults almost always
             agree on an equal split of resources. This is due to
             mutually recognized fairness and equality norms. Early
             developmental studies on sharing and equality norms found
             that egalitarian allocations of resources are not common
             before children are 5 or 6 years old. However, recent
             studies have shown that in some face-to face collaborative
             situations, or when recipients express their desires,
             children at much younger ages choose equal allocations. We
             investigated the ability of 3.5 and 5-year-olds to negotiate
             face-to-face, whether to collaborate to obtain an equal or
             an unequal distribution of rewards. We hypothesized that the
             face-to-face interaction and interdependency between
             partners would facilitate egalitarian outcomes at both ages.
             In the first experiment we found that 5-year-olds were more
             egalitarian than 3.5-year-olds, but neither of the age
             classes shared equally. In the second experiment, in which
             we increased the magnitude of the inequality, we found that
             children at both ages mostly agreed on the unequal
             distribution. These results show that communication and
             face-to-face interactions are not sufficient to guarantee
             equal allocations at 3-5 years of age. These results add to
             previous findings suggesting that in the context of
             non-collaboratively produced resources it is only after 5
             years of age that children use equality norms to allocate
             resources.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0120494},
   Key = {fds351620}
}

@article{fds321684,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Riedl, K and Jensen, K and Call,,
             J},
   Title = {Restorative justice in young children},
   Journal = {Current Biology},
   Volume = {25},
   Pages = {1-5},
   Year = {2015},
   Key = {fds321684}
}

@article{fds351621,
   Author = {Haun, DBM and Rekers, Y and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children conform to the behavior of peers; other great apes
             stick with what they know.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {2160-2167},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797614553235},
   Abstract = {All primates learn things from conspecifics socially, but it
             is not clear whether they conform to the behavior of these
             conspecifics--if conformity is defined as overriding
             individually acquired behavioral tendencies in order to copy
             peers' behavior. In the current study, chimpanzees,
             orangutans, and 2-year-old human children individually
             acquired a problem-solving strategy. They then watched
             several conspecific peers demonstrate an alternative
             strategy. The children switched to this new, socially
             demonstrated strategy in roughly half of all instances,
             whereas the other two great-ape species almost never
             adjusted their behavior to the majority's. In a follow-up
             study, children switched much more when the peer
             demonstrators were still present than when they were absent,
             which suggests that their conformity arose at least in part
             from social motivations. These results demonstrate an
             important difference between the social learning of humans
             and great apes, a difference that might help to account for
             differences in human and nonhuman cultures.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797614553235},
   Key = {fds351621}
}

@article{fds351622,
   Author = {Duguid, S and Wyman, E and Bullinger, AF and Herfurth-Majstorovic, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and human children in
             a Stag Hunt game.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {281},
   Number = {1796},
   Pages = {20141973},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1973},
   Abstract = {Much of human cooperation takes place in mutualistic
             contexts in which the main challenge for individuals is how
             to coordinate decisions. In the current studies, we compared
             the abilities of chimpanzees and young children to
             coordinate with a partner in two versions of a Stag Hunt
             game. When risks were low (the hare was of low value) and
             information was cheap (the partner's behaviour was readily
             observable), partners of both species were able to
             successfully coordinate on the higher value stag more than
             90% of the time. By contrast, when the risks were raised and
             observing the partner was more difficult, the chimpanzees
             became less successful, whereas the children compensated,
             and so remained highly successful, by communicating more
             often and more specifically. This pattern of results is
             consistent with the hypothesis that humans evolved unique
             skills of coordination and communication in the context of
             especially risky coordination problems.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2014.1973},
   Key = {fds351622}
}

@article{fds351623,
   Author = {Karg, K and Schmelz, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {All great ape species (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan
             troglodytes, Pongo abelii) and two-and-a-half-year-old
             children (Homo sapiens) discriminate appearance from
             reality.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {128},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {431-439},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037385},
   Abstract = {Nonhuman great apes and human children were tested for an
             understanding that appearance does not always correspond to
             reality. Subjects were 29 great apes (bonobos [Pan
             paniscus], chimpanzees [Pan troglodytes], gorillas [Gorilla
             gorilla], and orangutans [Pongo abelii]) and 24 2½-year-old
             children. In our task, we occluded portions of 1 large and 1
             small food stick such that the size relations seemed
             reversed. Subjects could then choose which one they wanted.
             There was 1 control condition and 2 experimental conditions
             (administered within subjects). In the control condition
             subjects saw only the apparent stick sizes, whereas in the 2
             experimental conditions they saw the true stick sizes as
             well (the difference between them being what the subjects
             saw first: the apparent or the real stick sizes). All great
             ape species and children successfully identified the bigger
             stick, despite its smaller appearance, in the experimental
             conditions, but not in the control. We discuss these results
             in relation to the understanding of object permanence and
             conservation, and exclude reversed reward contingency
             learning as an explanation.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0037385},
   Key = {fds351623}
}

@article{fds351624,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Rosenbaum, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Reasoning during joint decision-making by preschool
             peers},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {32},
   Pages = {74-85},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.09.001},
   Abstract = {Reasoning with a peer to make a joint decision involves
             making a proposal (e.g., "Polar bears go here") and
             justifying it with relevant facts (e.g., "This is ice")
             based on common ground assumptions or warrants (e.g., polar
             bears need ice). Twenty-four dyads of 3- and 5-year-olds
             built a zoo with toy items that were either conventional
             (e.g., animals, cages) or unconventional (e.g., piano). For
             conventional items, both participants in both age groups
             used justifications that relied on implicit warrants (e.g.,
             stating only the fact "This is ice", assuming that both
             partners know that polar bears need ice). For unconventional
             items, they more often articulated the warrant explicitly,
             arguably to create the necessary common ground.
             Five-year-olds made warrants explicit more often, produced
             more justifications, and reached mutual agreement more often
             than did 3-year-olds. These results suggest that
             preschoolers can reason with one another appropriately,
             specifically in justifying their proposals based on
             appropriate common ground assumptions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.09.001},
   Key = {fds351624}
}

@article{fds351625,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Schmerse, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children create partner-specific referential pacts
             with peers.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {2334-2342},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037837},
   Abstract = {In 2 studies, we investigated how peers establish a
             referential pact to call something, for example, a cushion
             versus a pillow (both equally felicitous). In Study 1, pairs
             of 4- and 6-year-old German-speaking peers established a
             referential pact for an artifact, for example, a woman's
             shoe, in a referential communication task. Six-year-olds,
             but not 4-year-olds, continued to use these same expressions
             with the same partner (even when they were overinformative)
             but shifted to simpler expressions, for example, shoe, with
             a new partner. In Study 2, both age groups were successful
             in establishing such partner-specific referential pacts with
             a peer when using a proper name. These results suggest that
             even preschool children appreciate something of the
             conventional nature of linguistic expressions, with
             significant flexibility emerging between ages 4 and
             6.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0037837},
   Key = {fds351625}
}

@article{fds351626,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Walter, V and Gampe, A and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Limitations to the cultural ratchet effect in young
             children.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {126},
   Pages = {152-160},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.04.006},
   Abstract = {Although many animal species show at least some evidence of
             cultural transmission, broadly defined, only humans show
             clear evidence of cumulative culture. In the current study,
             we investigated whether young children show the "ratchet
             effect," an important component of cumulative culture--the
             ability to accumulate efficient modifications across
             generations. We tested 16 diffusion chains--altogether
             consisting of 80 children--to see how they solved an
             instrumental task (i.e., carrying something from one
             location to another). We found that when the chain was
             seeded with an inefficient way of solving the task,
             4-year-olds were able to innovate and transmit these
             innovations so as to reach a more efficient solution.
             However, when it started out with relatively efficient
             solutions already (i.e., the ones that children in a control
             condition discovered for themselves), there were no further
             techniques invented and/or transmitted beyond that. Thus,
             young children showed the ratchet effect to a limited
             extent, accumulating efficient modifications but not going
             beyond the inventive level of the individual.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2014.04.006},
   Key = {fds351626}
}

@article{fds351627,
   Author = {Bullinger, AF and Melis, AP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) instrumentally help but do not
             communicate in a mutualistic cooperative
             task.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {128},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {251-260},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035645},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees cooperate in a variety of contexts, but
             communicating to influence and regulate cooperative
             activities is rare. It is unclear whether this reflects
             chimpanzees' general inability or whether they have found
             other means to coordinate cooperative activities. In the
             present study chimpanzees could help a partner play her role
             in a mutually beneficial food-retrieval task either by
             transferring a needed tool (transfer condition) or by
             visually or acoustically communicating the hiding-location
             of the needed tool (communication condition). Overall,
             chimpanzees readily helped their partner by delivering the
             needed tool, but none of them communicated the hiding
             location of the tool to their partner reliably across
             trials. These results demonstrate that although chimpanzees
             can coordinate their cooperative activities by
             instrumentally helping their partner in her role, they do
             not readily use communication with their partner for this
             same end.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0035645},
   Key = {fds351627}
}

@article{fds351628,
   Author = {Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children create iconic gestures to inform
             others.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {2049-2060},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037224},
   Abstract = {Much is known about young children's use of deictic gestures
             such as pointing. Much less is known about their use of
             other types of communicative gestures, especially iconic or
             symbolic gestures. In particular, it is unknown whether
             children can create iconic gestures on the spot to inform
             others. Study 1 provided 27-month-olds with the opportunity
             to inform a novice how to perform a task. The majority of
             children created appropriate iconic gestures, and they did
             so significantly more than in a control condition in which
             the need to inform someone was removed. In Study 2, some of
             the 21-month-olds tested also created novel iconic gestures
             but to a lesser extent. Results are discussed in relation to
             children's symbolic, linguistic, and social-cognitive
             development.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0037224},
   Key = {fds351628}
}

@article{fds351629,
   Author = {Austin, K and Theakston, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of denial.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {2061-2070},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037179},
   Abstract = {Although a fair amount is known about young children's
             production of negation, little is known about their
             comprehension. Here, we focus on arguably the most complex
             basic form, denial, and how young children understand
             denial, when it is expressed in response to a question with
             gesture, single word, or sentence. One hundred twenty-six
             children in 3 age groups (Ms = 1 year 9 months, 2 years 0
             months, and 2 years 4 months) witnessed an adult look into 1
             of 2 buckets and then, in response to a question about
             whether the toy was in there, communicate either something
             positive (positive head nod, "yes," "it is in this bucket")
             or negative (negative head shake, "No," "It's not in this
             bucket"). The youngest children did not search differently
             in response to any of the communicative cues (nor in
             response to an additional cue using both gesture and single
             word). Children at 2 years 0 months searched at above-chance
             levels only in response to the negative word and negative
             sentence. Children at 2 years 4 months were successful with
             all 3 types of cues in both positive and negative
             modalities, with the exception of the positive sentence.
             Young children thus seem to understand the denial of a
             statement before they understand its affirmation, and they
             understand linguistic means of expressing denial before they
             understand gestural means.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0037179},
   Key = {fds351629}
}

@article{fds351630,
   Author = {Moné, Y and Monnin, D and Kremer, N},
   Title = {The oxidative environment: a mediator of interspecies
             communication that drives symbiosis evolution.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {281},
   Number = {1785},
   Pages = {20133112},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3112},
   Abstract = {Symbiotic interactions are ubiquitous in nature and play a
             major role in driving the evolution of life. Interactions
             between partners are often mediated by shared signalling
             pathways, which strongly influence both partners' biology
             and the evolution of the association in various
             environments. As an example of 'common language', the
             regulation of the oxidative environment plays an important
             role in driving the evolution of symbiotic associations.
             Such processes have been occurring for billions of years,
             including the increase in Earth's atmospheric oxygen and the
             subsequent evolution of mitochondria. The effect of reactive
             oxygen species and reactive nitrogen species (RONS) has been
             characterized functionally, but the molecular dialogue
             between partners has not been integrated within a broader
             evolutionary context yet. Given the pleiotropic role of RONS
             in cell-cell communication, development and immunity, but
             also their associated physiological costs, we discuss here
             how their regulation can influence the establishment, the
             maintenance and the breakdown of various symbiotic
             associations. By synthesizing recent developments in redox
             biology, we aim to provide an interdisciplinary
             understanding of the influence of such mediators of
             interspecies communication on the evolution and stability of
             symbioses, which in turn can shape ecosystems and play a
             role in health and disease.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2013.3112},
   Key = {fds351630}
}

@article{fds351631,
   Author = {Köymen, B and Lieven, E and Engemann, DA and Rakoczy, H and Warneken,
             F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's norm enforcement in their interactions with
             peers.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {1108-1122},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12178},
   Abstract = {This study investigates how children negotiate social norms
             with peers. In Study 1, 48 pairs of 3- and 5-year-olds (N =
             96) and in Study 2, 48 pairs of 5- and 7-year-olds (N = 96)
             were presented with sorting tasks with conflicting
             instructions (one child by color, the other by shape) or
             identical instructions. Three-year-olds differed from older
             children: They were less selective for the contexts in which
             they enforced norms, and they (as well as the older children
             to a lesser extent) used grammatical constructions
             objectifying the norms ("It works like this" rather than
             "You must do it like this"). These results suggested that
             children's understanding of social norms becomes more
             flexible during the preschool years.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12178},
   Key = {fds351631}
}

@article{fds351632,
   Author = {Ibbotson, P and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The communicative contexts of grammatical aspect use in
             English.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {41},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {705-723},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000913000135},
   Abstract = {In many of the world's languages grammatical aspect is used
             to indicate how events unfold over time. In English,
             activities that are ongoing can be distinguished from those
             that are completed using the morphological marker -ing.
             Using naturalistic observations of two children in their
             third year of life, we quantify the availability and
             reliability of the imperfective form in the communicative
             context of the child performing actions. On average, 30% of
             verbal descriptions refer to child actions that are grounded
             in the here-and-now. Of these utterances, there are two
             features of the communicative context that reliably map onto
             the functions of the imperfective, namely, that events are
             construed as ongoing and from within. The findings are
             discussed with reference to how the context in which a child
             hears aspectual language may limit the degrees of freedom on
             what these constructions mean.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000913000135},
   Key = {fds351632}
}

@article{fds320792,
   Author = {Wobber, V and Herrmann, E and Hare, B and Wrangham, R and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Differences in the early cognitive development of children
             and great apes.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychobiology},
   Volume = {56},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {547-573},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.21125},
   Abstract = {There is very little research comparing great ape and human
             cognition developmentally. In the current studies we
             compared a cross-sectional sample of 2- to 4-year-old human
             children (n=48) with a large sample of chimpanzees and
             bonobos in the same age range (n=42, hereafter: apes) on a
             broad array of cognitive tasks. We then followed a group of
             juvenile apes (n=44) longitudinally over 3 years to track
             their cognitive development in greater detail. In skills of
             physical cognition (space, causality, quantities), children
             and apes performed comparably at 2 years of age, but by 4
             years of age children were more advanced (whereas apes
             stayed at their 2-year-old performance levels). In skills of
             social cognition (communication, social learning, theory of
             mind), children out-performed apes already at 2 years, and
             increased this difference even more by 4 years. Patterns of
             development differed more between children and apes in the
             social domain than the physical domain, with support for
             these patterns present in both the cross-sectional and
             longitudinal ape data sets. These results indicate key
             differences in the pattern and pace of cognitive development
             between humans and other apes, particularly in the early
             emergence of specific social cognitive capacities in
             humans.},
   Doi = {10.1002/dev.21125},
   Key = {fds320792}
}

@article{fds351633,
   Author = {van der Goot, MH and Tomasello, M and Liszkowski,
             U},
   Title = {Differences in the nonverbal requests of great apes and
             human infants.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {444-455},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12141},
   Abstract = {This study investigated how great apes and human infants use
             imperative pointing to request objects. In a series of three
             experiments (infants, N = 44; apes, N = 12), subjects were
             given the opportunity to either point to a desired object
             from a distance or else to approach closer and request it
             proximally. The apes always approached close to the object,
             signaling their request through instrumental actions. In
             contrast, the infants quite often stayed at a distance,
             directing the experimenters' attention to the desired object
             through index-finger pointing, even when the object was in
             the open and they could obtain it by themselves. Findings
             distinguish 12-month-olds' imperative pointing from
             ontogenetic and phylogenetic earlier forms of ritualized
             reaching.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12141},
   Key = {fds351633}
}

@article{fds351634,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Over, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an
             unexpected-contents task.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {119},
   Pages = {120-126},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002},
   Abstract = {Recent studies suggest that infants understand that others
             can have false beliefs. However, most of these studies have
             used looking time measures, and the few that have used
             behavioral measures are all based on the change-of-location
             paradigm, leading to claims that infants might use
             behavioral rules instead of mental state understanding to
             pass these tests. We investigated infants' false-belief
             reasoning using a different paradigm. In this
             unexpected-contents helping task, 18-month-olds were
             familiarized with boxes for blocks that contained blocks.
             When an experimenter subsequently reached for a box for
             blocks that now contained a spoon, infants based their
             choice of whether to give her a spoon or a block on her true
             or false belief about which object the block box contained.
             These results help to demonstrate the flexibility of
             infants' false-belief understanding.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002},
   Key = {fds351634}
}

@article{fds351635,
   Author = {Schmerse, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Discourse particles and belief reasoning: The case of German
             doch},
   Journal = {Journal of Semantics},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {115-133},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jos/fft001},
   Abstract = {Discourse particles typically express the attitudes of
             interlocutors with respect to the propositional content of
             an utterance - for example, marking whether or not a speaker
             believes the content of the proposition that she uttered. In
             German, the particle doch - which has no direct English
             translation - is commonly used to correct a belief that is
             thought to be common ground among those present. We asked
             whether German adults and 5-year-olds are able to infer that
             a speaker who utters doch intends to be understood in this
             way. Sixty-four children (4;9-5;3 years) and twenty-four
             adults participated in a comprehension task in which a
             speaker explicitly expressed either a positive belief or a
             negative belief. Subsequently, in both conditions, the
             speaker checked the truth of her previous belief and
             corrected her belief with doch. In both the group of adults
             and the group of children, polarity of the speaker's belief
             affected hearers' interpretations of the speaker's
             utterance. In a third condition we investigated whether
             participants could also perform the more difficult task of
             interpreting the speaker's utterance with doch while
             inferring the speaker's belief. Whereas adults showed a
             similar performance as in the explicit belief conditions,
             children showed limited abilities in keeping track of the
             speaker's belief. © The Author 2013. Published by Oxford
             University Press. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1093/jos/fft001},
   Key = {fds351635}
}

@article{fds351636,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Vaish, A and Haun, D and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Correction: Does sympathy motivate prosocial behaviour in
             great apes? (PLoS ONE)},
   Journal = {PLoS ONE},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/annotation/1fe9c2b8-84dd-44c4-a4ba-b62e0460b513},
   Doi = {10.1371/annotation/1fe9c2b8-84dd-44c4-a4ba-b62e0460b513},
   Key = {fds351636}
}

@article{fds321685,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The ultra-social animal},
   Journal = {European Journal of Social Psychology},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {187-194},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2015},
   Abstract = {In evolutionary perspective, what is most remarkable about
             human sociality is its many and diverse forms of
             cooperation. Here, I provide an overview of some recent
             research, mostly from our laboratory, comparing human
             children with their nearest living relatives, the great
             apes, in various tests of collaboration, prosocial behavior,
             conformity, and group-mindedness (e.g., following and
             enforcing social norms). This is done in the context of a
             hypothetical evolutionary scenario comprising two ordered
             steps: a first step in which early humans began
             collaborating with others in unique ways in their everyday
             foraging and a second step in which modern humans began
             forming cultural groups. Humans' unique forms of sociality
             help to explain their unique forms of cognition and
             morality. © 2014.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ejsp.2015},
   Key = {fds321685}
}

@article{fds351637,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Vaish, A and Haun, D and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Does sympathy motivate prosocial behaviour in great
             apes?},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {e84299},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084299},
   Abstract = {Prosocial behaviours such as helping, comforting, or sharing
             are central to human social life. Because they emerge early
             in ontogeny, it has been proposed that humans are prosocial
             by nature and that from early on empathy and sympathy
             motivate such behaviours. The emerging question is whether
             humans share these abilities to feel with and for someone
             with our closest relatives, the great apes. Although several
             studies demonstrated that great apes help others, little is
             known about their underlying motivations. This study
             addresses this issue and investigates whether four species
             of great apes (Pongo pygmaeus, Gorilla gorilla, Pan
             troglodytes, Pan paniscus) help a conspecific more after
             observing the conspecific being harmed (a human experimenter
             steals the conspecific's food) compared to a condition where
             no harming occurred. Results showed that in regard to the
             occurrence of prosocial behaviours, only orangutans, but not
             the African great apes, help others when help is needed,
             contrasting prior findings on chimpanzees. However, with the
             exception of one population of orangutans that helped
             significantly more after a conspecific was harmed than when
             no harm occurred, prosocial behaviour in great apes was not
             motivated by concern for others.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0084299},
   Key = {fds351637}
}

@article{fds351644,
   Author = {Nitzschner, M and Kaminski, J and Melis, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Side matters: Potential mechanisms underlying dogs'
             performance in a social eavesdropping paradigm},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {90},
   Pages = {263-271},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.035},
   Abstract = {Social eavesdropping is the gathering of information by
             observing interactions between other individuals. Previous
             studies have claimed that dogs, Canis familiaris, are able
             to use information obtained via social eavesdropping, that
             is, preferring a generous over a selfish human donor.
             However, in these studies the side was constant between the
             demonstrations and the dogs' choices, not controlling for
             potential location biases. In the crucial control condition
             of our experiments, the donors swapped places in half of the
             trials before the dogs chose. We found that first choice
             behaviour as well as the time dogs interacted with the
             generous donor were influenced by location (side). In a
             second experiment the subject's owner interacted with the
             two donors. Again, the result of the side control revealed
             that the critical factor was location (side) not person. The
             results of these experiments provide no evidence for social
             eavesdropping in dogs and show the importance of critical
             control conditions. © 2014 The Association for the Study of
             Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.035},
   Key = {fds351644}
}

@article{fds351645,
   Author = {Vogelsang, M and Jensen, K and Kirschner, S and Tennie, C and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Preschoolers are sensitive to free riding in a public goods
             game},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {JUL},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00729},
   Abstract = {Despite the benefits of cooperation, selfish individuals
             often produce outcomes where everyone is worse off. This
             "tragedy of the commons" has been demonstrated
             experimentally in adults with the public goods game.
             Contributions to a public good decline over time due to
             free-riders who keep their endowments. Little is known about
             how children behave when confronted with this social
             dilemma. Forty-eight preschoolers were tested using a novel
             non-verbal procedure and simplified choices more appropriate
             to their age than standard economic approaches. The rate of
             cooperation was initially very low and rose in the second
             round for the girls only. Children were affected by their
             previous outcome, as they free rode more after experiencing
             a lower outcome compared to the other group members. © 2014
             Vogelsang, Jensen, Kirschner, Tennie and
             Tomasello.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00729},
   Key = {fds351645}
}

@article{fds351646,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Steinwender, J and Hamann, K and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's planning in a collaborative problem-solving
             task},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {48-58},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.02.003},
   Abstract = {One important component of collaborative problem solving is
             the ability to plan one's own action in relation to that of
             a partner. We presented 3- and 5-year-old peer pairs with
             two different tool choice situations in which they had to
             choose complementary tools with which to subsequently work
             on a collaborative problem-solving apparatus. In the
             bidirectional condition, exemplars of the two necessary
             tools appeared in front of each child. In the unidirectional
             condition, one child had to choose between two different
             tools first, while the other child had only one tool
             available. Thus, both conditions required close attention to
             the actions of the partner, but the unidirectional condition
             additionally required the anticipation of the partner's
             constrained tool choice. Five-year-olds were proficient
             planners in both conditions, whereas 3-year-olds did not
             consistently make the correct choice. However, 3-year-olds
             who had first experienced the unidirectional condition chose
             the correct tool at an above-chance level. Moreover,
             communication during the tool choice led to greater success
             among 3-year-olds, but not among 5-year-olds. These results
             provide the first experimental evidence that between 3 and 5
             years of age children develop the ability to plan the
             division of labor in a collaborative task. We discuss our
             findings regarding planning for a collaborative task in
             relation to prior research on planning abilities for
             individual problem-solving that appear to undergo
             developmental change between 3 and 5 years of age. © 2014
             Elsevier Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.02.003},
   Key = {fds351646}
}

@article{fds351647,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M},
   Title = {Dueling dualists : Commentary on carpendale, atwood, and
             kettner},
   Journal = {Human Development},
   Volume = {56},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {401-405},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000357237},
   Doi = {10.1159/000357237},
   Key = {fds351647}
}

@article{fds351648,
   Author = {Göckeritz, S and Schmidt, MFH and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's creation and transmission of social
             norms},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {81-95},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.003},
   Abstract = {Children's lives are governed by social norms. Since Piaget,
             however, it has been assumed that they understand very
             little about how norms work. Recent studies in which
             children enforce social norms indicate a richer
             understanding, but children are still relating to
             pre-existing adult norms. In this study, triads of
             5-year-olds worked on an instrumental task without adult
             guidance. Children spontaneously created social norms
             regarding how the game "should" be played. They transmitted
             these with special force (using more generic and objective
             language) to novices, suggesting that young children
             understand to some degree, the conventional nature and
             special force of social norms in binding all who would
             participate. © 2014 Elsevier Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.003},
   Key = {fds351648}
}

@article{fds351649,
   Author = {Moll, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two- and 3-Year-Olds Know What Others Have and Have Not
             Heard},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {12-21},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.710865},
   Abstract = {Recent studies have established that even infants can
             determine what others know based on previous visual
             experience. In the current study, we investigated whether 2-
             and 3-year-olds know what others know based on previous
             auditory experience. A child and an adult heard the sound of
             one object together, but only the child heard the sound of
             another (target) object. When later the sounds of both
             objects were played simultaneously, the adult reacted with
             surprise and excitement ("Oh, listen, what is that?"). In
             response, both 24- and 36-month-olds directed the adult's
             attention to the target more often than chance and more
             often than in a control condition in which the adult had
             heard neither sound. These results indicate that by 24
             months of age, children's understanding of others' knowledge
             and ignorance is not limited to the visual domain but
             extends across perceptual domains. © 2014 Taylor and
             Francis Group, LLC.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15248372.2012.710865},
   Key = {fds351649}
}

@article{fds351650,
   Author = {Hertel, A and Kaminski, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Generalize or personalize--do dogs transfer an acquired rule
             to novel situations and persons?},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {e102666},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102666},
   Abstract = {Recent studies have raised the question of whether dogs,
             like human infants, comprehend an established rule as
             generalizable, normative knowledge or rather as episodic
             information, existing only in the immediate situation. In
             the current study we tested whether dogs disobeyed a
             prohibition to take a treat (i) in the presence of the
             communicator of the ban, (ii) after a temporary absence of
             the communicator, and (iii) in the presence of a novel
             person. Dogs disobeyed the rule significantly more often
             when the communicator left the room for a moment or when
             they were faced with a new person, than when she stayed
             present in the room. These results indicate that dogs
             "forget" a rule as soon as the immediate human context
             becomes disrupted.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0102666},
   Key = {fds351650}
}

@article{fds351651,
   Author = {Tempelmann, S and Kaminski, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do domestic dogs learn words based on humans' referential
             behaviour?},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e91014},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091014},
   Abstract = {Some domestic dogs learn to comprehend human words, although
             the nature and basis of this learning is unknown. In the
             studies presented here we investigated whether dogs learn
             words through an understanding of referential actions by
             humans rather than simple association. In three studies,
             each modelled on a study conducted with human infants, we
             confronted four word-experienced dogs with situations
             involving no spatial-temporal contiguity between the word
             and the referent; the only available cues were referential
             actions displaced in time from exposure to their referents.
             We found that no dogs were able to reliably link an object
             with a label based on social-pragmatic cues alone in all the
             tests. However, one dog did show skills in some tests,
             possibly indicating an ability to learn based on
             social-pragmatic cues.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0091014},
   Key = {fds351651}
}

@article{fds351652,
   Author = {Dittmar, M and Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Familiar verbs are not always easier than novel verbs: how
             German pre-school children comprehend active and passive
             sentences.},
   Journal = {Cognitive science},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {128-151},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12066},
   Abstract = {Many studies show a developmental advantage for transitive
             sentences with familiar verbs over those with novel verbs.
             It might be that once familiar verbs become entrenched in
             particular constructions, they would be more difficult to
             understand (than would novel verbs) in non-prototypical
             constructions. We provide support for this hypothesis
             investigating German children using a forced-choice pointing
             paradigm with reversed agent-patient roles. We tested active
             transitive verbs in study 1. The 2-year olds were better
             with familiar than novel verbs, while the 2½-year olds
             pointed correctly for both. In study 2, we tested passives:
             2½-year olds were significantly below chance for familiar
             verbs and at chance for novel verbs, supporting the
             hypothesis that the entrenchment of the familiar verbs in
             the active transitive voice was interfering with
             interpreting them in the passive voice construction. The
             3½-year olds were also at chance for novel verbs but above
             chance with familiar verbs. We interpret this as reflecting
             a lessening of the verb-in-construction entrenchment as the
             child develops knowledge that particular verbs can occur in
             a range of constructions. The 4½-year olds were above
             chance for both familiar and novel verbs. We discuss our
             findings in terms of the relative entrenchment of lexical
             and syntactic information and to interference between
             them.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cogs.12066},
   Key = {fds351652}
}

@article{fds351653,
   Author = {Hamann, K and Bender, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Meritocratic sharing is based on collaboration in
             3-year-olds.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {121-128},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032965},
   Abstract = {The present study investigated young preschoolers'
             proportional allocation of rewards in 2 different work
             contexts. We presented 32 pairs of 3.5-year-old peers with a
             collaborative task to obtain rewards by pulling ropes. In
             order to establish differences in work input, 1 child's rope
             was not immediately accessible but had to be retrieved from
             the apparatus by means of a specific tool, while the other
             child had no such additional work to do. The result of the
             game was that 1 individual received 1 toy and the other
             received 3 toys. In the Deserving condition, the working
             child received the 3 toys (thus work and reward matched),
             whereas in the Undeserving condition, the other child
             received the 3 toys (he or she was overpaid, and the working
             child was underpaid). Another 32 dyads participated in a
             noncollaborative, parallel work task, again in a Deserving
             condition and an Undeserving condition. On average, children
             with 3 toys shared with their partner more in the
             Undeserving condition than in the Deserving condition after
             collaboration but not in a parallel work setup. These
             results suggest that young children take merit into account
             in distributing resources at a much younger age than
             previously believed and that peer collaboration is an
             especially facilitative context for children's attention to
             norms of fairness.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0032965},
   Key = {fds351653}
}

@article{fds351654,
   Author = {Bannard, C and Klinger, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic
             material?},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {2344-2356},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032062},
   Abstract = {In 3 studies we explored when 3-year-olds would imitate
             novel words in utterances produced by adult speakers. Child
             and experimenter took turns in requesting objects from a
             game master. The experimenter always went first and always
             preceded the object's familiar name with a novel adjective
             (e.g., "the dilsige duck"). In the first 2 experiments, we
             found that children were more likely to reproduce the
             adjective when there were 2 different instances of the same
             object present in the situation than when there was only 1
             or when there were 2 objects of different types present.
             Thus, children seemed to be sensitive to the descriptive and
             contrastive function of the adjectives in determining which
             parts of the utterances to reproduce. Nonetheless,
             replication of even redundant material was over 50%,
             suggesting a strategy of somewhat blind copying. In the 3rd
             experiment, we found that children were less likely to
             reproduce a redundant adjective when the speaker indicated
             gesturally that he did not intend to produce it than when he
             clearly produced it intentionally. We distinguish insightful
             imitation (the copying of a speaker's goal and means when
             motivated by insight into why those particular means were
             chosen) and blind imitation (the copying of a speaker's goal
             and means with no awareness of why those specific means were
             chosen) from mimicry. We explore the roles that these modes
             of imitation might play in language development.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0032062},
   Key = {fds351654}
}

@article{fds351655,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children understand and defend the entitlements of
             others.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {116},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {930-944},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.013},
   Abstract = {Human social life is structured by social norms creating
             both obligations and entitlements. Recent research has found
             that young children enforce simple obligations against norm
             violators by protesting. It is not known, however, whether
             they understand entitlements in the sense that they will
             actively object to a second party attempting to interfere in
             something that a third party is entitled to do-what we call
             counter-protest. In two studies, we found that 3-year-old
             children understand when a person is entitled to do
             something, and so they actively defend this person's
             entitlement against unjustified interference from second
             parties. In some cases, they even enforce second-order
             entitlements, for example, in the case of ownership where an
             owner is entitled to entitle others to use the owner's
             property.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.013},
   Key = {fds351655}
}

@article{fds320793,
   Author = {Grosse, G and Scott-Phillips, TC and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Three-year-olds hide their communicative intentions in
             appropriate contexts.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {2095-2101},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032017},
   Abstract = {Human cooperative communication involves both an informative
             intention that the recipient understands the content of the
             signal and also a (Gricean) communicative intention that the
             recipient recognizes that the speaker has an informative
             intention. The degree to which children understand this
             2-layered nature of communication is the subject of some
             debate. One phenomenon that would seem to constitute clear
             evidence of such understanding is hidden authorship, in
             which informative acts are produced but with the
             communicative intent behind them intentionally hidden. In
             this study, 3- and 5-year-old children were told that an
             adult was seeking a toy but wanted to find it on her own.
             Children of both ages often did something to make the toy
             easier for the adult to see while at the same time
             concealing their actions in some way. This suggests that by
             the age of 3, children are able to separate the multiple
             layers of intentionality involved in human cooperative
             communication.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0032017},
   Key = {fds320793}
}

@article{fds351656,
   Author = {Schulze, C and Grassmann, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect
             verbal communication.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {84},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {2079-2093},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12093},
   Abstract = {Three studies investigated 3-year-old children's ability to
             determine a speaker's communicative intent when the
             speaker's overt utterance related to that intent only
             indirectly. Studies 1 and 2 examined children's
             comprehension of indirectly stated requests (e.g., "I find
             Xs good" can imply, in context, a request for X; N = 32).
             Study 3 investigated 3- and 4-year-old children's and
             adults' (N = 52) comprehension of the implications of a
             speaker responding to an offer by mentioning an action's
             fulfilled or unfulfilled precondition (e.g., responding to
             an offer of cereal by stating that we have no milk implies
             rejection of the cereal). In all studies, 3-year-old
             children were able to make the relevance inference necessary
             to integrate utterances meaningfully into the ongoing
             context.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12093},
   Key = {fds351656}
}

@article{fds351657,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Over, H and Herrmann, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup
             members and potential reciprocators.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {952-958},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12086},
   Abstract = {Human cooperation depends on individuals caring about their
             reputation, and so they sometimes attempt to manage them
             strategically. Here we show that even 5-year-old children
             strategically manage their reputation. In an experimental
             setting, children shared significantly more resources with
             an anonymous recipient when (1) the child watching them
             could reciprocate later, and (2) the child watching them was
             an ingroup rather than an outgroup member (as established by
             minimal group markers). This study is not only the first to
             show that young children selectively invest in their
             reputation with specific individuals, but also the first to
             show that we care more about our reputation with ingroup
             than with outgroup members.},
   Doi = {10.1111/desc.12086},
   Key = {fds351657}
}

@article{fds351658,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, recognize successful actions,
             but fail toimitate them},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {86},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {755-761},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.015},
   Abstract = {Cultural transmission, by definition, involves some form of
             social learning. Chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates
             clearly engage in some forms of social learning enabling
             some types of cultural transmission, but there is
             controversy about whether they copy the actual bodily
             actions of demonstrators. In this study chimpanzees
             recognized when a human actor was using particular bodily
             actions that had led to successful problem solving in the
             past. But then when it was their turn to solve the problem,
             they did not reproduce the human actor's bodily actions
             themselves, even though they were clearly capable of
             producing the movements. These results help us identify more
             precisely key reasons for the differences in the social
             learning and cultural transmission of humans and other
             primates. © 2013 The Association for the Study of Animal
             Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.015},
   Key = {fds351658}
}

@article{fds351659,
   Author = {Wittig, M and Jensen, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Five-year-olds understand fair as equal in a mini-ultimatum
             game.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {116},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {324-337},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.004},
   Abstract = {In studies of children's resource distribution, it is almost
             always the case that "fair" means an equal amount for all.
             In the mini-ultimatum game, players are confronted with
             situations in which fair does not always mean equal, and so
             the recipient of an offer needs to take into account the
             alternatives the proposer had available to her or him.
             Because of its forced-choice design, the mini-ultimatum game
             measures sensitivity to unfair intentions in addition to
             unfair outcomes. In the current study, we gave a
             mini-ultimatum game to 5-year-old children, allowing us to
             determine the nature of fairness sensitivity at a period
             after false belief awareness is typically passed and before
             formal schooling begins. The only situation in which
             responders rejected offers was when the proposer could have
             made an equal offer. But unlike adults, they did not employ
             more sophisticated notions of fairness that take into
             account the choices facing the proposer. Proposers, in their
             turn, were also not adult-like in that they had a very poor
             understanding that responders would reject unequal offers
             when an equal one was available. Thus, preschool children
             seem to understand "fair=equal" in this task, but not much
             more, and they are not yet skillful at anticipating what
             others will find fair beyond 50/50 splits.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.004},
   Key = {fds351659}
}

@article{fds351660,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young
             children.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {116},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {338-350},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002},
   Abstract = {Contingent reciprocity is important in theories of the
             evolution of human cooperation, but it has been very little
             studied in ontogeny. We gave 2- and 3-year-old children the
             opportunity to either help or share with a partner after
             that partner either had or had not previously helped or
             shared with the children. Previous helping did not influence
             children's helping. In contrast, previous sharing by the
             partner led to greater sharing in 3-year-olds but not in
             2-year-olds. These results do not support theories claiming
             either that reciprocity is fundamental to the origins of
             children's prosocial behavior or that it is irrelevant.
             Instead, they support an account in which children's
             prosocial behavior emerges spontaneously but is later
             mediated by reciprocity.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002},
   Key = {fds351660}
}

@article{fds351661,
   Author = {Wyman, E and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Non-verbal communication enables children's coordination in
             a "Stag Hunt" game},
   Journal = {European Journal of Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {597-610},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.726469},
   Abstract = {This study assessed the role of non-verbal communication in
             4-year-old children's decisions to coordinate with others.
             During a "Stag Hunt" game, the child and an adult
             individually and continually collected low-value prizes
             (hares). Occasionally, an alternative option of collecting a
             high-value prize (stag) cooperatively with the adult arose,
             but entailed a risk: a lone attempt on this prize by either
             player would leave that player empty handed. Children
             coordinated with the adult to obtain the high-value prize
             more often when that adult made mutual eye contact and
             smiled at them than when she attended to the prizes only.
             This suggests that neither verbal nor gestural communication
             are necessary for coordination: Minimal, non-verbal
             communication enables children's coordination with others
             towards joint goals. © 2013 Copyright Taylor and Francis
             Group, LLC.},
   Doi = {10.1080/17405629.2012.726469},
   Key = {fds351661}
}

@article{fds351662,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Uebel, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old
             infants.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {84},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1511-1518},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12083},
   Abstract = {Most previous research on imitation in infancy has focused
             on infants' learning of instrumental actions on objects.
             This study focused instead on the more social side of
             imitation, testing whether being mimicked increases
             prosocial behavior in infants, as it does in adults (van
             Baaren, Holland, Kawakami, & van Knippenberg, 2004).
             Eighteen-month-old infants (N = 48) were either mimicked
             or not by an experimenter; then either that experimenter or
             a different adult needed help. Infants who had previously
             been mimicked were significantly more likely to help both
             adults than infants who had not been mimicked. Thus, even in
             infancy, mimicry has positive social consequences: It
             promotes a general prosocial orientation toward
             others.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdev.12083},
   Key = {fds351662}
}

@article{fds351663,
   Author = {Graf, E and Theakston, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Subject and object omission in children's early transitive
             constructions: A discourse-pragmatic approach},
   Journal = {Applied Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {701-727},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000477},
   Abstract = {This paper investigates discourse effects on the provision
             of both subjects and objects and investigates whether
             pragmatic discourse features govern the realization/omission
             of both constituents alike. In an elicitation study, we
             examined how the discourse-pragmatic feature contrast, as
             applied to the subject, verb, or object of a transitive
             utterance affected the provision of elements in the
             remainder of the sentence when all elements were previously
             introduced. The results showed that 3.5-year-old children
             were more likely to realize a contrasted argument with a
             lexical noun but more likely to omit the argument when it
             was not part of a contrast, regardless of its subject or
             object status. This suggests that contrast presents a
             unifying discourse feature for argument omission in language
             development.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0142716413000477},
   Key = {fds351663}
}

@article{fds351664,
   Author = {Ibbotson, P and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The attention-grammar interface: Eye-gaze cues structural
             choice in children and adults},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {457-481},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cog-2013-0020},
   Abstract = {We investigated whether children (3- and 4-year-olds) and
             adults can use the active passive alternation - essentially
             a choice of subject - in a way that is consistent with the
             eye-gaze of the speaker. Previous work suggests the function
             of the subject position can be grounded in attentional
             mechanisms (Tomlin 1995, 1997). Eye-gaze is one powerful
             source of directing attention that we know adults and young
             children are sensitive to; furthermore, we know adults are
             more likely to look at the subject of their sentence than
             any other character (Gleitman et al. 2007; Griffin and Bock
             2000). We demonstrate that older children and adults are
             able to use speaker-gaze to choose a felicitous subject when
             describing a scene with both agent-focused and patient
             focused cues. Integrating attentional and grammatical
             information in this way allows children to limit the degrees
             of freedom on what the function of certain linguistic
             constructions might be.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cog-2013-0020},
   Key = {fds351664}
}

@article{fds351665,
   Author = {Halina, M and Rossano, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {653-666},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0601-7},
   Abstract = {Great apes communicate with gestures in flexible ways. Based
             on several lines of evidence, Tomasello and colleagues have
             posited that many of these gestures are learned via
             ontogenetic ritualization-a process of mutual anticipation
             in which particular social behaviors come to function as
             intentional communicative signals. Recently, Byrne and
             colleagues have argued that all great ape gestures are
             basically innate. In the current study, for the first time,
             we attempted to observe the process of ontogenetic
             ritualization as it unfolds over time. We focused on one
             communicative function between bonobo mothers and infants:
             initiation of "carries" for joint travel. We observed 1,173
             carries in ten mother-infant dyads. These were initiated by
             nine different gesture types, with mothers and infants using
             many different gestures in ways that reflected their
             different roles in the carry interaction. There was also a
             fair amount of variability among the different dyads,
             including one idiosyncratic gesture used by one infant. This
             gestural variation could not be attributed to sampling
             effects alone. These findings suggest that ontogenetic
             ritualization plays an important role in the origin of at
             least some great ape gestures.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-013-0601-7},
   Key = {fds351665}
}

@article{fds351666,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children sympathize less in response to unjustified
             emotional distress.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1132-1138},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029501},
   Abstract = {Three-year-old children saw an adult displaying the exact
             same distress in 3 different conditions: (a) the adult's
             distress was appropriate to a genuine harm, (b) the adult's
             distress was an overreaction to a minor inconvenience, and
             (c) there was no apparent cause for the adult's distress.
             Children who witnessed the adult being appropriately upset
             showed concern for him, intervened on his behalf, and
             checked on him when he later expressed distress out of their
             view. Children who did not know the cause for the adult's
             distress responded similarly. In contrast, children who
             witnessed the adult overreacting to an inconvenience showed
             lower rates of intervening and checking. The degree of
             children's concern across conditions was correlated with the
             latency of their helping behavior toward the adult later.
             These results suggest that from an early age, young
             children's sympathy and prosocial behavior are not automatic
             responses to emotional displays but, rather, involve taking
             into account whether the displayed distress is
             justified.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0029501},
   Key = {fds351666}
}

@article{fds351667,
   Author = {Schmerse, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Error patterns in young German children's
             wh-questions.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {656-671},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000912000104},
   Abstract = {In this article we report two studies: a detailed
             longitudinal analysis of errors in wh-questions from six
             German-learning children (age 2 ; 0-3 ; 0) and an analysis
             of the prosodic characteristics of wh-questions in German
             child-directed speech. The results of the first study
             demonstrate that German-learning children frequently omit
             the initial wh-word. A lexical analysis of wh-less questions
             revealed that children are more likely to omit the wh-word
             was ('what') than other wh-words (e.g. wo 'where'). In the
             second study, we performed an acoustic analysis of sixty
             wh-questions that one mother produced during her child's
             third year of life. The results show that the wh-word was is
             much less likely to be accented than the wh-word wo,
             indicating a relationship between children's omission of
             wh-words and the stress patterns associated with
             wh-questions. The findings are discussed in the light of
             discourse-pragmatic and metrical accounts of omission
             errors.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000912000104},
   Key = {fds351667}
}

@article{fds351668,
   Author = {Moore, R and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Three-year-olds understand communicative intentions without
             language, gestures, or gaze},
   Journal = {Interaction Studies},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {62-80},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/is.14.1.05moo},
   Abstract = {The communicative interactions of very young children almost
             always involve language (based on conventions), gesture
             (based on bodily deixis or iconicity) and directed gaze. In
             this study, ninety-six children (3;0 years) were asked to
             determine the location of a hidden toy by understanding a
             communicative act that contained none of these familiar
             means. A light-and-sound mechanism placed behind the hiding
             place and illuminated by a centrally placed switch was used
             to indicate the location of the toy. After a communicative
             training session, an experimenter pressed the switch either
             deliberately or accidentally, and with or without ostension
             (in the form of eye contact and child-directed speech). In
             no condition did she orient towards the hiding place. When
             the switch was pressed intentionally, children used the
             light-and-sound cue to find the toy - and tended to do so
             even in the absence of ostensive eye contact. When the
             experimenter pressed the switch accidentally, children
             searched randomly - demonstrating that they were tracking
             her communicative intent, and not merely choosing on the
             basis of salience. The absence of an effect of ostension
             contradicts research that ostension helps children to
             interpret the communicative intentions underlying unfamiliar
             signs. We explain this by concluding that while it may play
             a role in establishing a communicative interaction, it is
             not necessary for sustaining one; and that even with a
             highly novel communicative act - involving none of the means
             of communication on which children typically rely -
             three-year-olds can comprehend the communicative intentions
             behind an intentionally produced act. © John Benjamins
             Publishing Company.},
   Doi = {10.1075/is.14.1.05moo},
   Key = {fds351668}
}

@article{fds351669,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Parental Presence and Encouragement Do Not Influence Helping
             in Young Children},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {345-368},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00120.x},
   Abstract = {Young children begin helping others with simple instrumental
             problems from soon after their first birthdays. In previous
             observations of this phenomenon, both naturalistic and
             experimental, children's parents were in the room and could
             potentially have influenced their behavior. In the two
             current studies, we gave 24-month-old children the
             opportunity to help an unfamiliar adult obtain an
             out-of-reach object when the parent (or a friendly female
             adult) (i) was present but passive, (ii) was present and
             highlighted the problem for the child, (iii) was present and
             actively encouraged the child to help, (iv) was present and
             ordered the child to help, or (v) was absent from the room.
             The children helped at relatively high levels and equally
             under all these treatment conditions. There was also no
             differential effect of treatment condition on children's
             helping in a subsequent test phase in which no parent was
             present, and children had to disengage from a fun activity
             to help. Young children's helping behavior is not
             potentiated or facilitated by parental behavior in the
             immediate situation, suggesting that it is spontaneous and
             intrinsically motivated. Copyright © International Society
             on Infant Studies (ISIS).},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00120.x},
   Key = {fds351669}
}

@article{fds351670,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Keckeisen, M and Pitsch, A and Kaminski, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs conceal auditory but not visual information
             from others.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {351-359},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0576-9},
   Abstract = {A number of studies have shown that dogs are sensitive to a
             human's perspective, but it remains unclear whether they use
             an egocentric strategy to assess what humans perceive. We
             investigated whether dogs know what a human can see and
             hear, even when the dogs themselves are unable to see the
             human. Dogs faced a task in which forbidden food was placed
             in a tunnel that they could retrieve by using their paw.
             Whereas the dogs could not see the experimenter during their
             food retrieval attempts, the experimenter could potentially
             see the dog's paw. In the first experiment, dogs could
             choose between an opaque and a transparent side of the
             tunnel, and in the second experiment, they could choose
             between a silent and a noisy approach to the tunnel. The
             results showed that dogs preferred a silent approach to
             forbidden food but they did not hide their approach when
             they could not see a human present. We conclude that dogs
             probably rely on what they themselves can perceive when they
             assess what the human can see and hear.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0576-9},
   Key = {fds351670}
}

@article{fds351671,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzee responders still behave like rational
             maximizers.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {110},
   Number = {20},
   Pages = {E1837},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1303627110},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1303627110},
   Key = {fds351671}
}

@article{fds351672,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Pitsch, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Dogs steal in the dark.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {385-394},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0579-6},
   Abstract = {All current evidence of visual perspective taking in dogs
             can possibly be explained by dogs reacting to certain
             stimuli rather than understanding what others see. In the
             current study, we set up a situation in which contextual
             information and social cues are in conflict. A human always
             forbade the dog from taking a piece of food. The part of the
             room being illuminated was then varied, for example, either
             the area where the human was seated or the area where the
             food was located was lit. Results show that dogs steal
             significantly more food when it is dark compared to when it
             is light. While stealing forbidden food the dog's behaviour
             also depends on the type of illumination in the room.
             Illumination around the food, but not the human, affected
             the dogs' behaviour. This indicates that dogs do not take
             the sight of the human as a signal to avoid the food. It
             also cannot be explained by a low-level associative rule of
             avoiding illuminated food which dogs actually approach
             faster when they are in private. The current finding
             therefore raises the possibility that dogs take into account
             the human's visual access to the food while making their
             decision to steal it.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0579-6},
   Key = {fds351672}
}

@article{fds351673,
   Author = {Scheider, L and Kaminski, J and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Do domestic dogs interpret pointing as a
             command?},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {361-372},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0577-8},
   Abstract = {Domestic dogs comprehend human gestural communication
             flexibly, particularly the pointing gesture. Here, we
             examine whether dogs interpret pointing informatively, that
             is, as simply providing information, or rather as a command,
             for example, ordering them to move to a particular location.
             In the first study a human pointed toward an empty cup. In
             one manipulation, the dog either knew or did not know that
             the designated cup was empty (and that the other cup
             actually contained the food). In another manipulation, the
             human (as authority) either did or did not remain in the
             room after pointing. Dogs ignored the human's gesture if
             they had better information, irrespective of the authority's
             presence. In the second study, we varied the level of
             authority of the person pointing. Sometimes this person was
             an adult, and sometimes a young child. Dogs followed
             children's pointing just as frequently as they followed
             adults' pointing (and ignored the dishonest pointing of
             both), suggesting that the level of authority did not affect
             their behavior. Taken together these studies suggest that
             dogs do not see pointing as an imperative command ordering
             them to a particular location. It is still not totally
             clear, however, if they interpret it as informative or in
             some other way.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0577-8},
   Key = {fds351673}
}

@article{fds351674,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a
             collaborative task.},
   Journal = {Biology letters},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {20130009},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0009},
   Abstract = {Many animal species cooperate, but the underlying proximate
             mechanisms are often unclear. We presented chimpanzees with
             a mutualistic collaborative food-retrieval task requiring
             complementary roles, and tested subjects' ability to help
             their partner perform her role. For each role, subjects
             required a different tool, and the tools were not
             interchangeable. We gave one individual in each dyad both
             tools, and measured subjects' willingness to transfer a tool
             to their partner as well as which tool (correct versus
             incorrect) they transferred. Most subjects helped their
             partner and transferred the tool the partner needed. Thus,
             chimpanzees not only coordinate different roles, but they
             also know which particular action the partner needs to
             perform. These results add to previous findings suggesting
             that many of chimpanzees' limitations in collaboration are,
             perhaps, more motivational than cognitive.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rsbl.2013.0009},
   Key = {fds351674}
}

@article{fds351675,
   Author = {Moll, H and Meltzoff, AN and Merzsch, K and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Taking versus confronting visual perspectives in preschool
             children.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {646-654},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028633},
   Abstract = {Recent evidence suggests that 3-year-olds can take other
             people's visual perspectives not only when they perceive
             different things (Level 1) but even when they see the same
             thing differently (Level 2). One hypothesis is that
             3-year-olds are good perspective takers but cannot confront
             different perspectives on the same object (Perner, Stummer,
             Sprung, & Doherty, 2002). In 2 studies using color filters,
             3-year-olds were unable to judge in what color they and an
             adult saw the same picture. This was the case irrespective
             of whether children replied verbally (pilot study) or by
             pointing to color samples (main study). However, 3-year-olds
             readily took an adult's perspective by determining which of
             2 objects an adult referred to as being a certain color,
             independently from how the children saw the objects (main
             study). Taken together, these results suggest that
             preschoolers' difficulty is not so much taking perspectives
             as it is directly confronting another's view with their
             own-an ability that seems to be acquired between 4 and 5
             years of age.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0028633},
   Key = {fds351675}
}

@article{fds351676,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Bös, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in
             a problem-solving task.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {273-285},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0571-1},
   Abstract = {Cooperative hunting is a cognitively challenging activity
             since individuals have to coordinate movements with a
             partner and at the same time react to the prey. Domestic
             dogs evolved from wolves, who engage in cooperative hunting
             regularly, but it is not clear whether dogs have kept their
             cooperative hunting skills. We presented pairs of dogs with
             a reward behind a fence with two openings in it. A sliding
             door operated by the experimenter could block one opening
             but not both simultaneously. The dogs needed to coordinate
             their actions, so that each was in front of a different
             opening, if one of them was to cross through and get food.
             All 24 dog pairs solved the problem. In study 1, we
             demonstrated that dogs understood how the apparatus worked.
             In study 2, we found that, although the performance of the
             pairs did not depend on the divisibility of the reward,
             pairs were quicker at coordinating their actions when both
             anticipated rewards. However, the dogs did not monitor one
             another, suggesting that their solutions were achieved by
             each individual attempting to maximize for
             itself.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0571-1},
   Key = {fds351676}
}

@article{fds351677,
   Author = {Salomo, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's ability to answer different types of
             questions.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {469-491},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000912000050},
   Abstract = {Young children answer many questions every day. The extent
             to which they do this in an adult-like way - following
             Grice's Maxim of Quantity by providing the requested
             information, no more no less - has been studied very little.
             In an experiment, we found that two-, three- and
             four-year-old children are quite skilled at answering
             argument-focus questions and predicate-focus questions with
             intransitives in which their response requires only a single
             element. But predicate-focus questions for transitives -
             requiring both the predicate and the direct object - are
             difficult for children below four years of age. Even more
             difficult for children this young are sentence-focus
             questions such as "What's happening?", which give the child
             no anchor in given information around which to structure
             their answer. In addition, in a corpus study, we found that
             parents ask their children predicate-focus and
             sentence-focus questions very infrequently, thus giving
             children little experience with them.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000912000050},
   Key = {fds351677}
}

@article{fds351678,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of cultural common
             ground.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {Pt 1},
   Pages = {88-96},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.2012.02080.x},
   Abstract = {Human social interaction depends on individuals identifying
             the common ground they have with others, based both on
             personally shared experiences and on cultural common ground
             that all members of the group share. We introduced 3- and
             5-year-old children to a culturally well-known object and a
             novel object. An experimenter then entered and asked, 'What
             is that?', either as a request for information or in a
             recognitory way. When she was requesting information, both
             3- and 5-year-olds assumed she was asking about the novel
             object. When she seemed to recognize an object, 5-year-olds
             assumed she was referring to the culturally well-known
             object. Thus, by 3 years of age, children are beginning to
             understand that they share cultural common ground with other
             members of their group.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.2044-835x.2012.02080.x},
   Key = {fds351678}
}

@article{fds320794,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Keupp, S and Hare, B and Vaish, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great
             apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo
             pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {127},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {63-75},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028929},
   Abstract = {Humans make decisions about when and with whom to cooperate
             based on their reputations. People either learn about others
             by direct interaction or by observing third-party
             interactions or gossip. An important question is whether
             other animal species, especially our closest living
             relatives, the nonhuman great apes, also form reputations of
             others. In Study 1, chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, and
             2.5-year-old human children experienced a nice experimenter
             who tried to give food/toys to the subject and a mean
             experimenter who interrupted the food/toy giving. In studies
             2 and 3, nonhuman great apes and human children could only
             passively observe a similar interaction, in which a nice
             experimenter and a mean experimenter interacted with a third
             party. Orangutans and 2.5-year-old human children preferred
             to approach the nice experimenter rather than the mean one
             after having directly experienced their respective
             behaviors. Orangutans, chimpanzees, and 2.5-year-old human
             children also took into account experimenter actions toward
             third parties in forming reputations. These studies show
             that the human ability to form direct and indirect
             reputation judgment is already present in young children and
             shared with at least some of the other great
             apes.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0028929},
   Key = {fds320794}
}

@article{fds351679,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Altrichter, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Allocation of resources to collaborators and free-riders in
             3-year-olds.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {114},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {364-370},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.08.006},
   Abstract = {Recent studies have shown that in situations where resources
             have been acquired collaboratively, children at around 3
             years of age share mostly equally. We investigated
             3-year-olds' sharing behavior with a collaborating partner
             and a free-riding partner who explicitly expressed her
             preference not to collaborate. Children shared more equally
             with the collaborating partner than with the free rider.
             These results suggest that young children are sensitive to
             the contributions made by others to a collaborative effort
             (and possibly their reasons for not collaborating) and
             distribute resources accordingly.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2012.08.006},
   Key = {fds351679}
}

@article{fds351680,
   Author = {Schmelz, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees predict that a competitor's preference will
             match their own.},
   Journal = {Biology letters},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {20120829},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2012.0829},
   Abstract = {The ability to predict how another individual will behave is
             useful in social competition. Chimpanzees can predict the
             behaviour of another based on what they observe her to see,
             hear, know and infer. Here we show that chimpanzees act on
             the assumption that others have preferences that match their
             own. All subjects began with a preference for a box with a
             picture of food over one with a picture of nothing, even
             though the pictures had no causal relation to the contents.
             In a back-and-forth food competition, chimpanzees then
             avoided the box with the picture of food when their
             competitor had chosen one of the boxes before
             them-presumably on the assumption that the competitor shared
             their own preference for it and had already chosen it.
             Chimpanzees predicted that their competitor's preference
             would match their own and adjusted their behavioural
             strategies accordingly.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rsbl.2012.0829},
   Key = {fds351680}
}

@article{fds321687,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Vaish, A},
   Title = {Origins of human cooperation and morality},
   Journal = {Annual Review of Psychology},
   Volume = {64},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {231-255},
   Publisher = {ANNUAL REVIEWS},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812},
   Abstract = {From an evolutionary perspective, morality is a form of
             cooperation. Cooperation requires individuals either to
             suppress their own self-interest or to equate it with that
             of others. We review recent research on the origins of human
             morality, both phylogenetic (research with apes) and
             ontogenetic (research with children). For both time frames
             we propose a two-step sequence: first a second-personal
             morality in which individuals are sympathetic or fair to
             particular others, and second an agent-neutral morality in
             which individuals follow and enforce group-wide social
             norms. Human morality arose evolutionarily as a set of
             skills and motives for cooperating with others, and the
             ontogeny of these skills and motives unfolds in part
             naturally and in part as a result of sociocultural contexts
             and interactions. © 2013 by Annual Reviews. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812},
   Key = {fds321687}
}

@article{fds351682,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A New Look at Children's Prosocial Motivation},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {67-90},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00130.x},
   Abstract = {Young children routinely behave prosocially, but what is
             their motivation for doing so? Here, we review three studies
             which show that young children (1) are intrinsically
             motivated rather than motivated by extrinsic rewards; (2)
             are more inclined to help those for whom they feel sympathy;
             and (3) are not so much motivated to provide help themselves
             as to see the person helped (as can be seen in changes of
             their sympathetic arousal, as measured by pupil dilation, in
             different circumstances). Young children's prosocial
             behavior is thus intrinsically motivated by a concern for
             others' welfare, which has its evolutionary roots in a
             concern for the well-being of those with whom one is
             interdependent. © International Society on Infant Studies
             (ISIS).},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00130.x},
   Key = {fds351682}
}

@article{fds351683,
   Author = {Bullinger, AF and Burkart, JM and Melis, AP and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Bonobos, Pan paniscus, chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and
             marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, prefer to feed
             alone},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {51-60},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.006},
   Abstract = {Many primates share food, but the motives behind this food
             sharing are mostly not known. We investigated individuals'
             preference to feed either alone or together with a tolerant
             partner. Subjects (chimpanzees who are highly competitive
             around food, bonobos who are more tolerant around food and
             common marmosets who are cooperative breeders and share food
             actively with some partners) were tested with a door-opening
             paradigm. In a 2 × 2 design with the factors 'food' and
             'partner', subjects had the opportunity to eat piles of
             sharable food, if present, and/or to open the door, thereby
             allowing the partner, if present, to join the subject. While
             food had a main effect on the subject's behaviour, the
             presence of the partner did not. Individuals of all species
             opened the door much more often if there was no food
             available. These results suggest that regardless of their
             differing social organizations, chimpanzees, bonobos and
             marmosets do not voluntarily co-feed, but do not mind having
             company if there is no food present. © 2012 The Association
             for the Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.006},
   Key = {fds351683}
}

@article{fds351684,
   Author = {Gräfenhain, M and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Three-year-olds' understanding of the consequences of joint
             commitments.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {e73039},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0073039},
   Abstract = {Here we investigate the extent of children's understanding
             of the joint commitments inherent in joint activities.
             Three-year-old children either made a joint commitment to
             assemble a puzzle with a puppet partner, or else the child
             and puppet each assembled their own puzzle. Afterwards,
             children who had made the joint commitment were more likely
             to stop and wait for their partner on their way to fetch
             something, more likely to spontaneously help their partner
             when needed, and more likely to take over their partner's
             role when necessary. There was no clear difference in
             children's tendency to tattle on their partner's cheating
             behavior or their tendency to distribute rewards equally at
             the end. It thus appears that by 3 years of age making a
             joint commitment to act together with others is beginning to
             engender in children a "we"-intentionality which holds
             across at least most of the process of the joint activity
             until the shared goal is achieved, and which withstands at
             least some of the perturbations to the joint activity
             children experience.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0073039},
   Key = {fds351684}
}

@article{fds351685,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential
             emotional expressions to locate hidden food?},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {137-145},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0560-4},
   Abstract = {Although many studies have investigated domestic dogs'
             (Canis familiaris) use of human communicative cues, little
             is known about their use of humans' emotional expressions.
             We conducted a study following the general paradigm of
             Repacholi in Dev Psychol 34:1017-1025, (1998) and tested
             four breeds of dogs in the laboratory and another breed in
             the open air. In our study, a human reacted emotionally
             (happy, neutral or disgust) to the hidden contents of two
             boxes, after which the dog was then allowed to choose one of
             the boxes. Dogs tested in the laboratory distinguished
             between the most distinct of the expressed emotions
             (Happy-Disgust condition) by choosing appropriately, but
             performed at chance level when the two emotions were less
             distinct (Happy-Neutral condition). The breed tested in the
             open air passed both conditions, but this breed's differing
             testing setup might have been responsible for their success.
             Although without meaningful emotional expressions, when
             given a choice, these subjects chose randomly, their
             performance did not differ from that in the experimental
             conditions. Based on the findings revealed in the
             laboratory, we suggest that some domestic dogs recognize
             both the directedness and the valence of some human
             emotional expressions.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0560-4},
   Key = {fds351685}
}

@article{fds351686,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Melis, AP and Tennie, C and Wyman, E and Herrmann,
             E},
   Title = {Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The
             interdependence Hypothesis},
   Journal = {Current Anthropology},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {673-692},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/668207},
   Abstract = {Modern theories of the evolution of human cooperation focus
             mainly on altruism. In contrast, we propose that humans'
             species-unique forms of cooperation-as well as their
             species-unique forms of cognition, communication, and social
             life-all derive from mutualistic collaboration (with social
             selection against cheaters). In a first step, humans became
             obligate collaborative foragers such that individuals were
             interdependent with one another and so had a direct interest
             in the well-being of their partners. In this context, they
             evolved new skills and motivations for collaboration not
             possessed by other great apes (joint intentionality), and
             they helped their potential partners (and avoided cheaters).
             In a second step, these new collaborative skills and
             motivations were scaled up to group life in general, as
             modern humans faced competition from other groups. As part
             of this new group-mindedness, they created cultural
             conventions, norms, and institutions (all characterized by
             collective intentionality), with knowledge of a specific set
             of these marking individuals as members of a particular
             cultural group. Human cognition and sociality thus became
             ever more collaborative and altruistic as human individuals
             became ever more interdependent. © 2012 by The Wenner-Gren
             Foundation for Anthropological Research. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1086/668207},
   Key = {fds351686}
}

@article{fds351687,
   Author = {Kaiser, I and Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Theft in an ultimatum game: chimpanzees and bonobos are
             insensitive to unfairness.},
   Journal = {Biology letters},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {942-945},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2012.0519},
   Abstract = {Humans, but not chimpanzees, punish unfair offers in
             ultimatum games, suggesting that fairness concerns evolved
             sometime after the split between the lineages that gave rise
             to Homo and Pan. However, nothing is known about fairness
             concerns in the other Pan species, bonobos. Furthermore,
             apes do not typically offer food to others, but they do
             react against theft. We presented a novel game, the
             ultimatum theft game, to both of our closest living
             relatives. Bonobos and chimpanzee 'proposers' consistently
             stole food from the responders' portions, but the responders
             did not reject any non-zero offer. These results support the
             interpretation that the human sense of fairness is a derived
             trait.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rsbl.2012.0519},
   Key = {fds351687}
}

@article{fds351688,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Why be nice? Better not think about it.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {580-581},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.006},
   Abstract = {Are people more likely to be cooperative if they must act
             quickly or if they have more time to mull it over? The
             results of a recent series of studies suggest that peoples'
             initial impulse is to cooperate, but that with more time and
             reflection they become more selfish.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.006},
   Key = {fds351688}
}

@article{fds351689,
   Author = {Schneider, A-C and Melis, AP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How chimpanzees solve collective action problems.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {279},
   Number = {1749},
   Pages = {4946-4954},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1948},
   Abstract = {We presented small groups of chimpanzees with two collective
             action situations, in which action was necessary for reward
             but there was a disincentive for individuals to act owing to
             the possibility of free-riding on the efforts of others. We
             found that in simpler scenarios (experiment 1) in which
             group size was small, there was a positive relationship
             between rank and action with more dominant individuals
             volunteering to act more often, particularly when the reward
             was less dispersed. Social tolerance also seemed to mediate
             action whereby higher tolerance levels within a group
             resulted in individuals of lower ranks sometimes acting and
             appropriating more of the reward. In more complex scenarios,
             when group size was larger and cooperation was necessary
             (experiment 2), overcoming the problem was more challenging.
             There was highly significant variability in the action rates
             of different individuals as well as between dyads,
             suggesting success was more greatly influenced by the
             individual personalities and personal relationships present
             in the group.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2012.1948},
   Key = {fds351689}
}

@article{fds351690,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Behne, T and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Origins of the human pointing gesture: a training
             study.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {817-829},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01181.x},
   Abstract = {Despite its importance in the development of children's
             skills of social cognition and communication, very little is
             known about the ontogenetic origins of the pointing gesture.
             We report a training study in which mothers gave children
             one month of extra daily experience with pointing as
             compared with a control group who had extra experience with
             musical activities. One hundred and two infants of 9, 10, or
             11 months of age were seen at the beginning, middle, and end
             of this one-month period and tested for declarative pointing
             and gaze following. Infants'ability to point with the index
             finger at the end of the study was not affected by the
             training but was instead predicted by infants' prior ability
             to follow the gaze direction of an adult. The frequency with
             which infants pointed indexically was also affected by
             infant gaze following ability and, in addition, by maternal
             pointing frequency in free play, but not by training. In
             contrast, infants' ability to monitor their partner's gaze
             when pointing, and the frequency with which they did so, was
             affected by both training and maternal pointing frequency in
             free play. These results suggest that prior social cognitive
             advances, rather than adult socialization of pointing per
             se, determine the developmental onset of indexical pointing,
             but socialization processes such as imitation and adult
             shaping subsequently affect both infants' ability to monitor
             their interlocutor's gaze while they point and how
             frequently infants choose to point.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01181.x},
   Key = {fds351690}
}

@article{fds351691,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Schütte, S and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Great apes infer others' goals based on context.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1037-1053},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0528-4},
   Abstract = {In previous studies claiming to demonstrate that great apes
             understand the goals of others, the apes could potentially
             have been using subtle behavioral cues present during the
             test to succeed. In the current studies, we ruled out the
             use of such cues by making the behavior of the experimenter
             identical in the test phase of both the experimental and
             control conditions; the only difference was the preceding
             "context." In the first study, apes interpreted a human's
             ambiguous action as having the underlying goal of opening a
             box, or not, based on that human's previous actions with
             similar boxes. In the second study, chimpanzees learned that
             when a human stood up she was going to go get food for them,
             but when a novel, unexpected event happened, they changed
             their expectation-presumably based on their understanding
             that this new event led the human to change her goal. These
             studies suggest that great apes do not need concurrent
             behavioral cues to infer others' goals, but can do so from a
             variety of different types of cues-even cues displaced in
             time.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0528-4},
   Key = {fds351691}
}

@article{fds351694,
   Author = {Hepach, R and Vaish, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others
             helped.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {967-972},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612440571},
   Abstract = {Young children help other people, but it is not clear why.
             In the current study, we found that 2-year-old children's
             sympathetic arousal, as measured by relative changes in
             pupil dilation, is similar when they themselves help a
             person and when they see that person being helped by a third
             party (and sympathetic arousal in both cases is different
             from that when the person is not being helped at all). These
             results demonstrate that the intrinsic motivation for young
             children's helping behavior does not require that they
             perform the behavior themselves and thus "get credit" for
             it, but rather requires only that the other person be
             helped. Thus, from an early age, humans seem to have genuine
             concern for the welfare of others.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797612440571},
   Key = {fds351694}
}

@article{fds351695,
   Author = {Riedl, K and Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {No third-party punishment in chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {109},
   Number = {37},
   Pages = {14824-14829},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1203179109},
   Abstract = {Punishment can help maintain cooperation by deterring
             free-riding and cheating. Of particular importance in
             large-scale human societies is third-party punishment in
             which individuals punish a transgressor or norm violator
             even when they themselves are not affected. Nonhuman
             primates and other animals aggress against conspecifics with
             some regularity, but it is unclear whether this is ever
             aimed at punishing others for noncooperation, and whether
             third-party punishment occurs at all. Here we report an
             experimental study in which one of humans' closest living
             relatives, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), could punish an
             individual who stole food. Dominants retaliated when their
             own food was stolen, but they did not punish when the food
             of third-parties was stolen, even when the victim was
             related to them. Third-party punishment as a means of
             enforcing cooperation, as humans do, might therefore be a
             derived trait in the human lineage.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1203179109},
   Key = {fds351695}
}

@article{fds351696,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on
             the violator's group affiliation.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {124},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {325-333},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.004},
   Abstract = {To become cooperative members of their cultural groups,
             developing children must follow their group's social norms.
             But young children are not just blind norm followers, they
             are also active norm enforcers, for example, protesting and
             correcting when someone plays a conventional game the
             "wrong" way. In two studies, we asked whether young children
             enforce social norms on all people equally, or only on
             ingroup members who presumably know and respect the norm. We
             looked at both moral norms involving harm and conventional
             game norms involving rule violations. Three-year-old
             children actively protested violation of moral norms equally
             for ingroup and outgroup individuals, but they enforced
             conventional game norms for ingroup members only. Despite
             their ingroup favoritism, young children nevertheless hold
             ingroup members to standards whose violation they tolerate
             from outsiders.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.004},
   Key = {fds351696}
}

@article{fds351697,
   Author = {Ibbotson, P and Theakston, AL and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Semantics of the transitive construction: prototype effects
             and developmental comparisons.},
   Journal = {Cognitive science},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1268-1288},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01249.x},
   Abstract = {This paper investigates whether an abstract linguistic
             construction shows the kind of prototype effects
             characteristic of non-linguistic categories, in both adults
             and young children. Adapting the prototype-plus-distortion
             methodology of Franks and Bransford (1971), we found that
             whereas adults were lured toward false-positive recognition
             of sentences with prototypical transitive semantics, young
             children showed no such effect. We examined two main
             implications of the results. First, it adds a novel data
             point to a growing body of research in cognitive linguistics
             and construction grammar that shows abstract linguistic
             categories can behave in similar ways to non-linguistic
             categories, for example, by showing graded membership of a
             category. Thus, the findings lend psychological validity to
             the existing cross-linguistic evidence for prototypical
             transitive semantics. Second, we discuss a possible
             explanation for the fact that prototypical sentences were
             processed differently in adults and children, namely, that
             children's transitive semantic network is not as
             interconnected or cognitively coherent as
             adults'.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01249.x},
   Key = {fds351697}
}

@article{fds351698,
   Author = {Behne, T and Liszkowski, U and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Twelve-month-olds' comprehension and production of
             pointing.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {Pt 3},
   Pages = {359-375},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.2011.02043.x},
   Abstract = {This study explored whether infants aged 12 months already
             recognize the communicative function of pointing gestures.
             Infants participated in a task requiring them to comprehend
             an adult's informative pointing gesture to the location of a
             hidden toy. They mostly succeeded in this task, which
             required them to infer that the adult was attempting to
             direct their attention to a location for a reason - because
             she wanted them to know that a toy was hidden there. Many of
             the infants also reversed roles and produced appropriate
             pointing gestures for the adult in this same game, and
             indeed there was a correlation such that comprehenders were
             for the most part producers. These findings indicate that by
             12 months of age infants are beginning to show a
             bidirectional understanding of communicative
             pointing.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.2044-835x.2011.02043.x},
   Key = {fds351698}
}

@article{fds351699,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young Children Enforce Social Norms},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {232-236},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721412448659},
   Abstract = {Social norms have played a key role in the evolution of
             human cooperation, serving to stabilize prosocial and
             egalitarian behavior despite the self-serving motives of
             individuals. Young children's behavior mostly conforms to
             social norms, as they follow adult behavioral directives and
             instructions. But it turns out that even preschool children
             also actively enforce social norms on others, often using
             generic normative language to do so. This behavior is not
             easily explained by individualistic motives; it is more
             likely a result of children's growing identification with
             their cultural group, which leads to prosocial motives for
             preserving its ways of doing things. © The Author(s)
             2012.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0963721412448659},
   Key = {fds351699}
}

@article{fds351700,
   Author = {Gampe, A and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through
             overhearing},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {385-397},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723711433584},
   Abstract = {The prototypical word learning situation in western,
             middle-class cultures is dyadic: an adult addresses a child
             directly, ideally in a manner sensitive to their current
             focus of attention. But young children also seem to learn
             many of their words in polyadic situations through
             overhearing. Extending the previous work of Akhtar and
             colleagues, in the current two studies we gave 18-month-old
             infants opportunities to acquire novel words through
             overhearing in situations that were a bit more complex: they
             did not socially interact with the adult who used the new
             word before the word learning situation began, and the way
             the adult used the new word was less transparent in that it
             was neither a naming nor a directive speech act. In both
             studies, infants learned words equally well (and above
             chance) whether they were directly addressed or had to
             eavesdrop on two adults. Almost from the beginning, young
             children employ diverse learning strategies for acquiring
             new words. © 2011 The Author(s).},
   Doi = {10.1177/0142723711433584},
   Key = {fds351700}
}

@article{fds351701,
   Author = {Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Three-year-olds understand appearance and reality--just not
             about the same object at the same time.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1124-1132},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025915},
   Abstract = {Young children struggle in the classic tests of appearance
             versus reality. In the current Study 1, 3-year-olds had to
             determine which of 2 objects (a deceptive or a nondeceptive
             one) an adult requested when asking for the "real X" versus
             "the one that looks like X." In Study 2, children of the
             same age had to indicate what a single deceptive object
             (e.g., a chocolate-eraser) looked like and what it really
             was by selecting one of two items that represented this
             object's appearance (a chocolate bar) or identity (a regular
             eraser). Children were mainly successful in Study 1 but not
             in Study 2. The findings are discussed with a focus on young
             children's difficulty with "confronting" perspectives, which
             may be involved in their struggles with a number of classic
             theory of mind tasks.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0025915},
   Key = {fds351701}
}

@article{fds351702,
   Author = {Grassmann, S and Kaminski, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How two word-trained dogs integrate pointing and
             naming.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {657-665},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0494-x},
   Abstract = {Two word-trained dogs were presented with acts of reference
             in which a human pointed, named objects, or simultaneously
             did both. The question was whether these dogs would assume
             co-reference of pointing and naming and thus pick the
             pointed-to object. Results show that the dogs did indeed
             assume co-reference of pointing and naming in order to
             determine the reference of a spoken word, but they did so
             only when pointing was not in conflict with their previous
             word knowledge. When pointing and a spoken word conflicted,
             the dogs preferentially fetched the object by name. This is
             not surprising since they are trained to fetch objects by
             name. However, interestingly, in these conflict conditions,
             the dogs fetched the named objects only after they had
             initially approached the pointed-to object. We suggest that
             this shows that the word-trained dogs interpret pointing as
             a spatial directive, which they integrate into the fetching
             game, presumably assuming that pointing is relevant to
             finding the requested object.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-012-0494-x},
   Key = {fds351702}
}

@article{fds351703,
   Author = {Haun, DBM and Rekers, Y and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human
             children, but not orangutans.},
   Journal = {Current biology : CB},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {727-731},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.006},
   Abstract = {Cultural transmission is a key component of human evolution.
             Two of humans' closest living relatives, chimpanzees and
             orangutans, have also been argued to transmit behavioral
             traditions across generations culturally [1-3], but how much
             the process might resemble the human process is still in
             large part unknown. One key phenomenon of human cultural
             transmission is majority-biased transmission: the increased
             likelihood for learners to end up not with the most frequent
             behavior but rather with the behavior demonstrated by most
             individuals. Here we show that chimpanzees and human
             children as young as 2 years of age, but not orangutans, are
             more likely to copy an action performed by three
             individuals, once each, than an action performed by one
             individual three times. The tendency to acquire the
             behaviors of the majority has been posited as key to the
             transmission of relatively safe, reliable, and productive
             behavioral strategies [4-7] but has not previously been
             demonstrated in primates.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.006},
   Key = {fds351703}
}

@article{fds351704,
   Author = {Fletcher, GE and Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Differences in cognitive processes underlying the
             collaborative activities of children and
             chimpanzees},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {136-153},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.02.003},
   Abstract = {We compared the performance of 3- and 5-year-old children
             with that of chimpanzees in two tasks requiring
             collaboration via complementary roles. In both tasks,
             children and chimpanzees were able to coordinate two
             complementary roles with peers and solve the problem
             cooperatively. This is the first experimental demonstration
             of the coordination of complementary roles in chimpanzees.
             In the second task, neither species was skillful at waiting
             for a partner to be positioned appropriately before
             beginning (although children did hesitate significantly
             longer when the partner was absent). The main difference
             between species in both tasks was in children's, but not
             chimpanzees', ability to profit from experience as a
             collaborator in one role when later reversing roles. This
             difference suggests that as they participate in a
             collaboration, young children integrate both roles into a
             single " birds-eye-view" representational format in a way
             that chimpanzees do not. © 2012 Elsevier
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.02.003},
   Key = {fds351704}
}

@article{fds351705,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Butcher, J and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions
             to context: effects of distracters and feedback on
             referential communication.},
   Journal = {Topics in cognitive science},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {184-210},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01181.x},
   Abstract = {Children often refer to things ambiguously but learn not to
             from responding to clarification requests. We review and
             explore this learning process here. In Study 1, eighty-four
             2- and 4-year-olds were tested for their ability to request
             stickers from either (a) a small array with one dissimilar
             distracter or (b) a large array containing similar
             distracters. When children made ambiguous requests, they
             received either general feedback or specific questions about
             which of two options they wanted. With training, children
             learned to produce more complex object descriptions and did
             so faster in the specific feedback condition. They also
             tended to provide more information when requesting stickers
             from large arrays. In Study 2, we varied only distracter
             similarity during training and then varied array size in a
             generalization test. Children found it harder to learn in
             this case. In the generalization test, 4-year-olds were more
             likely to provide information (a) when it was needed because
             distracters were similar to the target and (b) when the
             array size was greater (regardless of need for information).
             We discuss how clear cues to potential ambiguity are needed
             for children to learn to tailor their referring expression
             to context and how several cues of heuristic value (e.g.,
             more distracters > say more) can promote the efficiency of
             communication while language is developing. Finally, we
             consider whether it would be worthwhile drawing on the human
             learning process when developing algorithms for the
             production of referring expressions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01181.x},
   Key = {fds351705}
}

@article{fds351708,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Schulz, L and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How dogs know when communication is intended for
             them.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {222-232},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01120.x},
   Abstract = {Domestic dogs comprehend human gestural communication in a
             way that other animal species do not. But little is known
             about the specific cues they use to determine when human
             communication is intended for them. In a series of four
             studies, we confronted both adult dogs and young dog puppies
             with object choice tasks in which a human indicated one of
             two opaque cups by either pointing to it or gazing at it. We
             varied whether the communicator made eye contact with the
             dog in association with the gesture (or whether her back was
             turned or her eyes were directed at another recipient) and
             whether the communicator called the dog's name (or the name
             of another recipient). Results demonstrated the importance
             of eye contact in human-dog communication, and, to a lesser
             extent, the calling of the dog's name--with no difference
             between adult dogs and young puppies--which are precisely
             the communicative cues used by human infants for identifying
             communicative intent. Unlike human children, however, dogs
             did not seem to comprehend the human's communicative gesture
             when it was directed to another human, perhaps because dogs
             view all human communicative acts as directives for the
             recipient.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01120.x},
   Key = {fds351708}
}

@article{fds351709,
   Author = {Theakston, AL and Maslen, R and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The acquisition of the active transitive construction in
             English: A detailed case study},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {91-128},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cog-2012-0004},
   Abstract = {In this study, we test a number of predictions concerning
             children's knowledge of the transitive Subject-Verb-Object
             (SVO) construction between two and three years on one child
             (Thomas) for whom we have densely collected data. The data
             show that the earliest SVO utterances reflect earlier use of
             those same verbs, and that verbs acquired before 2;7 show an
             earlier move towards adultlike levels of use in the SVO
             construction and in object argument complexity than later
             acquired verbs. There is not a close relation with the input
             in the types of subject and object referents used, nor a
             close adherence to Preferred Argument Structure (PAS) before
             2;7, but both early and late acquired verbs show a
             simultaneous move towards PAS patterns in selection of
             referent type at 2;9. The event semantics underpinning early
             transitive utterances do not straightforwardly fit prototype
             (high or inalienable) notions of transitivity, but rather
             may reflect sensitivity to animacy and intentionality in a
             way that mirrors the input. We conclude that children's
             knowledge of the transitive construction continues to
             undergo significant development between 2;0 and 3;0,
             reflecting the gradual abstraction and integration of the
             SVO and VO constructions, verb semantics, discourse
             pragmatics, and the interactions between these factors.
             These factors are considered in the context of a prototype
             for the transitive construction. © Walter de
             Gruyter.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cog-2012-0004},
   Key = {fds351709}
}

@article{fds351711,
   Author = {Nitzschner, M and Melis, AP and Kaminski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Dogs (Canis familiaris) evaluate humans on the basis of
             direct experiences only.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {e46880},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046880},
   Abstract = {Reputation formation is a key component in the social
             interactions of many animal species. An evaluation of
             reputation is drawn from two principal sources: direct
             experience of an individual and indirect experience from
             observing that individual interacting with a third party. In
             the current study we investigated whether dogs use direct
             and/or indirect experience to choose between two human
             interactants. In the first experiment, subjects had direct
             interaction either with a "nice" human (who played with,
             talked to and stroked the dog) or with an "ignoring"
             experimenter who ignored the dog completely. Results showed
             that the dogs stayed longer close to the "nice" human. In a
             second experiment the dogs observed a "nice" or "ignoring"
             human interacting with another dog. This indirect
             experience, however, did not lead to a preference between
             the two humans. These results suggest that the dogs in our
             study evaluated humans solely on the basis of direct
             experience.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0046880},
   Key = {fds351711}
}

@article{fds351712,
   Author = {Bannard, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Can we dissociate contingency learning from social learning
             in word acquisition by 24-month-olds?},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {e49881},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049881},
   Abstract = {We compared 24-month-old children's learning when their
             exposure to words came either in an interactive (coupled)
             context or in a nonsocial (decoupled) context. We measured
             the children's learning with two different methods: one in
             which they were asked to point to the referent for the
             experimenter, and the other a preferential looking task in
             which they were encouraged to look to the referent. In the
             pointing test, children chose the correct referents for
             words encountered in the coupled condition but not in the
             decoupled condition. In the looking time test, however, they
             looked to the targets regardless of condition. We explore
             the explanations for this and propose that the different
             response measures are reflecting two different kinds of
             learning.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0049881},
   Key = {fds351712}
}

@article{fds351713,
   Author = {Rossano, F and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {One-year-old infants follow others' voice
             direction.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {1298-1302},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612450032},
   Abstract = {We investigated 1-year-old infants' ability to infer an
             adult's focus of attention solely on the basis of her voice
             direction. In Studies 1 and 2, 12- and 16-month-olds watched
             an adult go behind a barrier and then heard her verbally
             express excitement about a toy hidden in one of two boxes at
             either end of the barrier. Even though they could not see
             the adult, infants of both ages followed her voice direction
             to the box containing the toy. Study 2 showed that infants
             could do this even when the adult was positioned closer to
             the incorrect box while she vocalized toward the correct one
             (and thus ruled out the possibility that infants were merely
             approaching the source of the sound). In Study 3, using the
             same methods as in Study 2, we found that chimpanzees
             performed the task at chance level. Our results show that
             infants can determine the focus of another person's
             attention through auditory information alone-a useful skill
             for establishing joint attention.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797612450032},
   Key = {fds351713}
}

@article{fds351714,
   Author = {Engelmann, JM and Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their
             reputations.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {e48433},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048433},
   Abstract = {Virtually all theories of the evolution of cooperation
             require that cooperators find ways to interact with one
             another selectively, to the exclusion of cheaters. This
             means that individuals must make reputational judgments
             about others as cooperators, based on either direct or
             indirect evidence. Humans, and possibly other species, add
             another component to the process: they know that they are
             being judged by others, and so they adjust their behavior in
             order to affect those judgments - so-called impression
             management. Here, we show for the first time that already
             preschool children engage in such behavior. In an
             experimental study, 5-year-old human children share more and
             steal less when they are being watched by a peer than when
             they are alone. In contrast, chimpanzees behave the same
             whether they are being watched by a groupmate or not. This
             species difference suggests that humans' concern for their
             own self-reputation, and their tendency to manage the
             impression they are making on others, may be unique to
             humans among primates.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0048433},
   Key = {fds351714}
}

@article{fds351715,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail
             to imitate novel actions.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {e41548},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041548},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>Social learning research in apes has
             focused on social learning in the technical (problem
             solving) domain - an approach that confounds action and
             physical information. Successful subjects in such studies
             may have been able to perform target actions not as a result
             of imitation learning but because they had learnt some
             technical aspect, for example, copying the movements of an
             apparatus (i.e., different forms of emulation
             learning).<h4>Methods</h4>Here we present data on action
             copying by non-enculturated and untrained chimpanzees when
             physical information is removed from demonstrations. To
             date, only one such study (on gesture copying in a begging
             context) has been conducted--with negative results. Here we
             have improved this methodology and have also added
             non-begging test situations (a possible confound of the
             earlier study). Both familiar and novel actions were used as
             targets. Prior to testing, a trained conspecific
             demonstrator was rewarded for performing target actions in
             view of observers. All but one of the tested chimpanzees
             already failed to copy familiar actions. When retested with
             a novel target action, also the previously successful
             subject failed to copy--and he did so across several
             contexts.<h4>Conclusion</h4>Chimpanzees do not seem to copy
             novel actions, and only some ever copy familiar ones. Due to
             our having tested only non-enculturated and untrained
             chimpanzees, the performance of our test subjects speak more
             than most other studies of the general (dis-)ability of
             chimpanzees to copy actions, and especially novel
             actions.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0041548},
   Key = {fds351715}
}

@article{fds351716,
   Author = {Kirchhofer, KC and Zimmermann, F and Kaminski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan
             troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {e30913},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030913},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees routinely follow the gaze of humans to outside
             targets. However, in most studies using object choice they
             fail to use communicative gestures (e.g. pointing) to find
             hidden food. Chimpanzees' failure to do this may be due to
             several difficulties with this paradigm. They may, for
             example, misinterpret the gesture as referring to the opaque
             cup instead of the hidden food. Or perhaps they do not
             understand informative communicative intentions. In
             contrast, dogs seem to be skilful in using human
             communicative cues in the context of finding food, but as of
             yet there is not much data showing whether they also use
             pointing in the context of finding non-food objects. Here we
             directly compare chimpanzees' (N = 20) and dogs' (N = 32)
             skills in using a communicative gesture directed at a
             visible object out of reach of the human but within reach of
             the subject. Pairs of objects were placed in view of and
             behind the subjects. The task was to retrieve the object the
             experimenter wanted. To indicate which one she desired, the
             experimenter pointed imperatively to it and directly
             rewarded the subject for handing over the correct one. While
             dogs performed well on this task, chimpanzees failed to
             identify the referent. Implications for great apes' and
             dogs' understanding of human communicative intentions are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0030913},
   Key = {fds351716}
}

@article{fds351717,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Hamann, K},
   Title = {Collaboration in young children.},
   Journal = {Quarterly journal of experimental psychology
             (2006)},
   Volume = {65},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-12},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.608853},
   Abstract = {Humans accomplish much of what they do in collaboration with
             others. In ontogeny, children's earliest abilities to
             collaborate develop in two basic steps. First, 1- and
             2-year-olds learn to form with others joint goals and joint
             attention--which include an understanding of the individual
             roles and perspectives involved. Second, as they approach
             their third birthdays, children's collaborative interactions
             with others take on a more normative dimension involving
             obligations to the partner. In addition, their cognitive
             abilities to conceptualize simultaneously both their own
             role and perspective along with those of the other develop
             considerably as well. This form of collaborative interaction
             is underlain by species-unique skills and motivations for
             shared intentionality that make possible, ultimately, such
             things as complex cultural institutions.},
   Doi = {10.1080/17470218.2011.608853},
   Key = {fds351717}
}

@article{fds351718,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Gräfenhain, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young
             children's understanding of joint intentions in
             collaborative activities.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {54-61},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01107.x},
   Abstract = {Some children's social activities are structured by joint
             goals. In previous research, the criterion used to determine
             this was relatively weak: if the partner stopped
             interacting, did the child attempt to re-engage her? But
             re-engagement attempts could easily result from the child
             simply realizing that she needs the partner to reach her own
             goal in the activity (social tool explanation). In two
             experiments, 21- and 27-month-old children interacted with
             an adult in games in which they either did or did not
             physically need the partner to reach a concrete goal.
             Moreover, when the partner stopped interacting, she did so
             because she was either unwilling to continue (breaking off
             from the joint goal) or unable to continue (presumably still
             maintaining the joint goal). Children of both age groups
             encouraged the recalcitrant partner equally often whether
             she was or was not physically needed for goal attainment. In
             addition, they did so more often when the partner was unable
             to continue than when she was unwilling to continue. These
             findings suggest that young children do not just view their
             collaborative partners as mindless social tools, but rather
             as intentional, cooperative agents with whom they must
             coordinate intentional states.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01107.x},
   Key = {fds351718}
}

@article{fds351719,
   Author = {Hamann, K and Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's developing commitments to joint
             goals.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {137-145},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01695.x},
   Abstract = {This study investigated young children's commitment to a
             joint goal by assessing whether peers in collaborative
             activities continue to collaborate until all received their
             rewards. Forty-eight 2.5- and 3.5-year-old children worked
             on an apparatus dyadically. One child got access to her
             reward early. For the partner to benefit as well, this child
             had to continue to collaborate even though there was no
             further reward available to her. The study found that
             3.5-year-olds, but not 2.5-year-olds, eagerly assisted their
             unlucky partner. They did this less readily in a
             noncollaborative control condition. A second study confirmed
             that 2.5-year-old children understood the task structure.
             These results suggest that children begin to appreciate the
             normative dimensions of collaborative activities during the
             3rd year of life.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01695.x},
   Key = {fds351719}
}

@article{fds351720,
   Author = {Grosse, G and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-old children differentiate test questions from
             genuine questions.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {192-204},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000760},
   Abstract = {Children are frequently confronted with so-called 'test
             questions'. While genuine questions are requests for missing
             information, test questions ask for information obviously
             already known to the questioner. In this study we explored
             whether two-year-old children respond differentially to one
             and the same question used as either a genuine question or
             as a test question based on the situation (playful game
             versus serious task) and attitude (playful ostensive cues
             versus not). Results indicated that children responded to
             questions differently on the basis of the situation but not
             the expressed attitude of the questioner. Two-year-old
             children thus understand something of the very special
             communicative intentions behind test questions.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000910000760},
   Key = {fds351720}
}

@article{fds351721,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Methodological challenges in the study of primate
             cognition},
   Journal = {Science},
   Volume = {334},
   Number = {6060},
   Pages = {1227-1228},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1213443},
   Abstract = {Laboratory studies of primate cognition face the problem
             that captive populations of a species are not always
             comparable, and generalizations to natural populations are
             never certain. Studies of primate cognition in the field
             face the problem that replications are expensive and
             difficult, and again different populations are not always
             comparable. To help remedy these problems, we recommend the
             creation of data banks where primary data and videotapes may
             be deposited (perhaps as a requirement of publication) to
             facilitate cross-examination, replication, and, eventually,
             the pooling of data across investigators.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1213443},
   Key = {fds351721}
}

@article{fds351722,
   Author = {Bullinger, AF and Wyman, E and Melis, AP and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Coordination of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a Stag Hunt
             Game},
   Journal = {International Journal of Primatology},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1296-1310},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10764-011-9546-3},
   Abstract = {Group-living animals frequently face situations in which
             they must coordinate individual and sometimes conflicting
             goals. We assessed chimpanzees' ability to coordinate in a
             Stag Hunt game. Dyads were confronted with a situation in
             which each individual was already foraging on a low-value
             food (hare) when a high-value food (stag) appeared that
             required collaboration for retrieval, with a solo attempt to
             get the stag resulting in a loss of both options. In one
             condition visibility between partners was open whereas in
             the other it was blocked by a barrier. Regardless of
             condition, dyads almost always (91%) coordinated to choose
             the higher valued collaborative option. Intentional
             communication or monitoring of the partner's behavior before
             decision making-characteristic of much human
             coordination-were limited. Instead, all dyads adopted a
             leader-follower strategy in which one partner took the risk
             of going first, presumably predicting that this would induce
             the other to join in (sometimes communicating if she was
             slow to do so). These results show that humans' closest
             primate relatives do not use complex communication to
             coordinate but most often use a less cognitively complex
             strategy that achieves the same end. © 2011 Springer
             Science+Business Media, LLC.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10764-011-9546-3},
   Key = {fds351722}
}

@article{fds320795,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Hare, B and Cissewski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human
             infants.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1393-1405},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01082.x},
   Abstract = {The adaptive behavior of primates, including humans, is
             often mediated by temperament. Human behavior likely differs
             from that of other primates in part due to temperament. In
             the current study we compared the reaction of bonobos,
             chimpanzees, orangutans, and 2.5-year-old human infants to
             novel objects and people - as a measure of their
             shyness-boldness, a key temperamental trait. Human children
             at the age of 2.5 years avoided novelty of all kinds far
             more than the other ape species. This response was most
             similar to that seen in bonobos and least like that of
             chimpanzees and orangutans. This comparison represents a
             first step in characterizing the temperamental profiles of
             species in the hominoid clade, and these findings are
             consistent with the hypothesis that human temperament has
             evolved since our lineage diverged from the other apes in
             ways that likely have broad effects on behavior. These
             findings also provide new insights into how species
             differences in ecology may shape differences in
             temperament.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01082.x},
   Key = {fds320795}
}

@article{fds351723,
   Author = {Bullinger, AF and Melis, AP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, prefer individual over
             collaborative strategies towards goals},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {82},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1135-1141},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.08.008},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees engage in a number of group activities, but it
             is still unclear to what extent they prefer mutualistic
             collaborative strategies over individual strategies to
             achieve their goals. In one experiment, we gave chimpanzees
             the choice between pulling a platform to within reach either
             individually or collaboratively with a tolerant partner,
             both strategies having equivalent payoffs. Overall,
             chimpanzees preferred the individual option, and this
             preference was independent of the type of reward for which
             they were working (food or tool). In a second experiment,
             chimpanzees switched to the collaboration option as soon as
             the payoff was increased for this option. These results
             suggest that chimpanzees prefer to work alone in
             foraging-like situations and choose collaboration only if it
             maximizes their reward. These results thus make a strong
             case for the hypothesis that differences between humans' and
             chimpanzees' collaboration are to a great extent due to
             motivational differences. © 2011 The Association for the
             Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.08.008},
   Key = {fds351723}
}

@article{fds351724,
   Author = {Dittmar, M and Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Children aged 2 ; 1 use transitive syntax to make a
             semantic-role interpretation in a pointing
             task.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1109-1123},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000747},
   Abstract = {The current study used a forced choice pointing paradigm to
             examine whether English children aged 2 ; 1 can use abstract
             knowledge of the relationship between word order position
             and semantic roles to make an active behavioural decision
             when interpreting active transitive sentences with novel
             verbs, when the actions are identical in the target and foil
             video clips. The children pointed significantly above chance
             with novel verbs but only if the final trial was excluded.
             With familiar verbs the children pointed consistently above
             chance. Children aged 2 ; 7 did not show these tiring
             effects and their performance in the familiar and novel verb
             conditions was always equivalent.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000910000747},
   Key = {fds351724}
}

@article{fds351725,
   Author = {Haun, DBM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Conformity to peer pressure in preschool
             children.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {82},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1759-1767},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01666.x},
   Abstract = {Both adults and adolescents often conform their behavior and
             opinions to peer groups, even when they themselves know
             better. The current study investigated this phenomenon in 24
             groups of 4 children between 4;2 and 4;9 years of age.
             Children often made their judgments conform to those of 3
             peers, who had made obviously erroneous but unanimous public
             judgments right before them. A follow-up study with 18
             groups of 4 children between 4;0 and 4;6 years of age
             revealed that children did not change their "real" judgment
             of the situation, but only their public expression of it.
             Preschool children are subject to peer pressure, indicating
             sensitivity to peers as a primary social reference group
             already during the preschool years.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01666.x},
   Key = {fds351725}
}

@article{fds351726,
   Author = {Rossano, F and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of violations of property
             rights.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {121},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {219-227},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.007},
   Abstract = {The present work investigated young children's normative
             understanding of property rights using a novel methodology.
             Two- and 3-year-old children participated in situations in
             which an actor (1) took possession of an object for himself,
             and (2) attempted to throw it away. What varied was who
             owned the object: the actor himself, the child subject, or a
             third party. We found that while both 2- and 3-year-old
             children protested frequently when their own object was
             involved, only 3-year-old children protested more when a
             third party's object was involved than when the actor was
             acting on his own object. This suggests that at the latest
             around 3 years of age young children begin to understand the
             normative dimensions of property rights.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.007},
   Key = {fds351726}
}

@article{fds351727,
   Author = {Rekers, Y and Haun, DBM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to
             collaborate.},
   Journal = {Current biology : CB},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {20},
   Pages = {1756-1758},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066},
   Abstract = {Human societies are built on collaborative activities.
             Already from early childhood, human children are skillful
             and proficient collaborators. They recognize when they need
             help in solving a problem and actively recruit collaborators
             [1, 2]. The societies of other primates are also to some
             degree cooperative. Chimpanzees, for example, engage in a
             variety of cooperative activities such as border patrols,
             group hunting, and intra- and intergroup coalitionary
             behavior [3-5]. Recent studies have shown that chimpanzees
             possess many of the cognitive prerequisites necessary for
             human-like collaboration. Chimpanzees have been shown to
             recognize when they need help in solving a problem and to
             actively recruit good over bad collaborators [6, 7].
             However, cognitive abilities might not be all that differs
             between chimpanzees and humans when it comes to cooperation.
             Another factor might be the motivation to engage in a
             cooperative activity. Here, we hypothesized that a key
             difference between human and chimpanzee collaboration-and so
             potentially a key mechanism in the evolution of human
             cooperation-is a simple preference for collaborating (versus
             acting alone) to obtain food. Our results supported this
             hypothesis, finding that whereas children strongly prefer to
             work together with another to obtain food, chimpanzees show
             no such preference.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066},
   Key = {fds351727}
}

@article{fds351728,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Neumann, M and Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Dogs, Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request
             but not to inform},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {82},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {651-658},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.06.015},
   Abstract = {Dogs are especially skilful at comprehending human
             communicative signals. This raises the question of whether
             they are also able to produce such signals flexibly,
             specifically, whether they helpfully produce indicative
             ('showing') behaviours to inform an ignorant human. In
             experiment 1, dogs indicated the location of an object more
             frequently when it was something they wanted themselves than
             when it was something the human wanted. There was some
             suggestion that this might be different when the human was
             their owner. So in experiment 2 we investigated whether dogs
             could understand when the owner needed helpful information
             to find a particular object (out of two) that they needed.
             They did not. Our findings, therefore, do not support the
             hypothesis that dogs communicate with humans to inform them
             of things they do not know. © 2011 The Association for the
             Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.06.015},
   Key = {fds351728}
}

@article{fds351729,
   Author = {Pettersson, H and Kaminski, J and Herrmann, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Understanding of human communicative motives in domestic
             dogs},
   Journal = {Applied Animal Behaviour Science},
   Volume = {133},
   Number = {3-4},
   Pages = {235-245},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.05.008},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees find it easier to locate food when a human
             prohibits them from going to a certain location than when
             she indicates that location helpfully. Human children, in
             contrast, use the cooperative gesture more readily. The
             question here was whether domestic dogs are more like
             chimpanzees, in this regard, or more like human children. In
             our first study we presented 40 dogs with two communicative
             contexts. In the cooperative context the experimenter
             informed the subject where food was hidden by pointing
             helpfully (with a cooperative tone of voice). In the
             competitive context the experimenter extended her arm
             towards the correct location in a prohibitive manner, palm
             of hand out (uttering a forbidding command in a prohibitive
             tone of voice). Dogs were successful in the cooperative
             condition (P=0.005) but chose randomly in the competitive
             condition (P=0.221). The second study independently varied
             the two characteristics of the communicative gesture (the
             gesture itself and the tone of voice). In addition to
             replicating dogs' better performance with the cooperative
             gestures, this study suggests that tone of voice and context
             had more effect than type of gesture. In the context of food
             acquisition, domestic dogs, like human children, seem more
             prepared to use human gestures when they are given
             cooperatively. © 2011 Elsevier B.V.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.applanim.2011.05.008},
   Key = {fds351729}
}

@article{fds351730,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Schneider, AC and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, share food in the same way
             after collaborative and individual food acquisition},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {82},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {485-493},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.024},
   Abstract = {We investigated the hypothesis that patterns of chimpanzee
             food sharing are influenced by whether individuals
             contributed to its acquisition collaboratively. In two
             experiments we exposed pairs of captive chimpanzees to food
             acquisition/sharing situations in which we manipulated (1)
             whether or not the two individuals had worked together
             collaboratively to retrieve the food and (2) the proximity
             of the individuals to the food at the moment of retrieval.
             The first experiment resembled a scramble competition
             scenario, with nonmonopolizable food. Proximity of
             individuals to the food when it arrived was the major
             variable affecting amount obtained by subordinates. Whether
             or not the food was obtained via collaboration had no
             effect. The second experiment resembled a contest
             competition scenario, as the food was a single large piece
             of fruit that could be more readily monopolized. In this
             scenario, dominants obtained more food than subordinates,
             the amount of food obtained by 'noncaptors' was affected by
             their proximity to the food when it arrived, and again
             previous collaboration had no effect. These results suggest
             that in many food acquisition situations first-arriver and
             first-possessor chimpanzees, as well as dominants in
             general, have a significant advantage in food acquisition,
             but being a collaborator brings no extra benefits. © 2011
             The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.024},
   Key = {fds351730}
}

@article{fds351731,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of markedness in non-verbal
             communication.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {888-903},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000383},
   Abstract = {Speakers often anticipate how recipients will interpret
             their utterances. If they wish some other, less obvious
             interpretation, they may 'mark' their utterance (e.g. with
             special intonations or facial expressions). We investigated
             whether two- and three-year-olds recognize when adults mark
             a non-verbal communicative act--in this case a pointing
             gesture--as special, and so search for a not-so-obvious
             referent. We set up the context of cleaning up and then
             pointed to an object. Three-year-olds inferred that the
             adult intended the pointing gesture to indicate that object,
             and so cleaned it up. However, when the adult marked her
             pointing gesture (with exaggerated facial expression) they
             took the object's hidden contents or a hidden aspect of it
             as the intended referent. Two-year-olds' appreciation of
             such marking was less clear-cut. These results demonstrate
             that markedness is not just a linguistic phenomenon, but
             rather something concerning the pragmatics of intentional
             communication more generally.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000910000383},
   Key = {fds351731}
}

@article{fds351732,
   Author = {Salomo, D and Graf, E and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The role of perceptual availability and discourse context in
             young children's question answering.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {918-931},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000395},
   Abstract = {Three- and four-year-old children were asked predicate-focus
             questions ('What's X doing?') about a scene in which an
             agent performed an action on a patient. We varied: (i)
             whether (or not) the preceding discourse context, which
             established the patient as given information, was available
             for the questioner; and (ii) whether (or not) the patient
             was perceptually available to the questioner when she asked
             the question. The main finding in our study differs from
             those of previous studies since it suggests that children
             are sensitive to the perceptual context at an earlier age
             than they are to previous discourse context if they need to
             take the questioner's perspective into account. Our finding
             indicates that, while children are in principle sensitive to
             both factors, young children rely on perceptual availability
             when a conflict arises.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000910000395},
   Key = {fds351732}
}

@article{fds351733,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's responses to guilt displays.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1248-1262},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024462},
   Abstract = {Displaying guilt after a transgression serves to appease the
             victim and other group members, restore interpersonal
             relationships, and indicate the transgressors' awareness of
             and desire to conform to the group's norms. We investigated
             whether and when young children are sensitive to these
             functions of guilt displays. In Study 1, after 4- and
             5-year-old children watched videos of transgressors either
             displaying guilt (without explicitly apologizing) or not
             displaying guilt, 5-year-olds appropriately inferred that
             the victim would be madder at the unremorseful transgressor
             and would prefer the remorseful transgressor. They also said
             that they would prefer to interact with the remorseful
             transgressor, judged the unremorseful transgressor to be
             meaner, and, in a distribution of resources task, gave more
             resources to the remorseful transgressor. The 4-year-olds
             did not draw any of these inferences and distributed the
             resources equally. However, Study 2 showed that 4-year-olds
             were able to draw appropriate inferences about transgressors
             who explicitly apologized versus those who did not
             apologize. Thus, 4-year-olds seem to know the appeasement
             functions that explicit apologies serve but only when
             children have reached the age of 5 years do they seem to
             grasp the emotions that apologies stand for, namely, guilt
             and remorse, and the appeasement functions that displaying
             these emotions serve.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0024462},
   Key = {fds351733}
}

@article{fds351734,
   Author = {Mersmann, D and Tomasello, M and Call, J and Kaminski, J and Taborsky,
             M},
   Title = {Simple Mechanisms Can Explain Social Learning in Domestic
             Dogs (Canis familiaris)},
   Journal = {Ethology},
   Volume = {117},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {675-690},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01919.x},
   Abstract = {Recent studies have suggested that domestic dogs (Canis
             familiaris) engage in highly complex forms of social
             learning. Here, we critically assess the potential
             mechanisms underlying social learning in dogs using two
             problem-solving tasks. In a classical detour task, the test
             dogs benefited from observing a demonstrator walking around
             a fence to obtain a reward. However, even inexperienced dogs
             did not show a preference for passing the fence at the same
             end as the demonstrator. Furthermore, dogs did not need to
             observe a complete demonstration by a human demonstrator to
             pass the task. Instead, they were just as successful in
             solving the problem after seeing a partial demonstration by
             an object passing by at the end of the fence. In contrast to
             earlier findings, our results suggest that stimulus
             enhancement (or affordance learning) might be a powerful
             social learning mechanism used by dogs to solve such detour
             problems. In the second task, we examined whether naïve
             dogs copy actions to solve an instrumental problem. After
             controlling for stimulus enhancement and other forms of
             social influence (e.g. social facilitation and observational
             conditioning), we found that dogs' problem solving was not
             influenced by witnessing a skilful demonstrator (either an
             unknown human, a conspecific or the dog's owner). Together,
             these results add to evidence suggesting that social
             learning may often be explained by relatively simple (but
             powerful) mechanisms. © 2011 Blackwell Verlag
             GmbH.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01919.x},
   Key = {fds351734}
}

@article{fds351735,
   Author = {Callaghan, T and Moll, H and Rakoczy, H and Warneken, F and Liszkowski,
             U and Behne, T and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Early social cognition in three cultural
             contexts.},
   Journal = {Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
             Development},
   Volume = {76},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {vii-142},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00603.x},
   Abstract = {The influence of culture on cognitive development is well
             established for school age and older children. But almost
             nothing is known about how different parenting and
             socialization practices in different cultures affect
             infants' and young children's earliest emerging cognitive
             and social-cognitive skills. In the current monograph, we
             report a series of eight studies in which we systematically
             assessed the social-cognitive skills of 1- to 3-year-old
             children in three diverse cultural settings. One group of
             children was from a Western, middle-class cultural setting
             in rural Canada and the other two groups were from
             traditional, small-scale cultural settings in rural Peru and
             India.In a first group of studies, we assessed 1-year-old
             children's most basic social-cognitive skills for
             understanding the intentions and attention of others:
             imitation, helping, gaze following, and communicative
             pointing.Children's performance in these tasks was mostly
             similar across cultural settings. In a second group of
             studies, we assessed 1-year-old children's skills in
             participating in interactive episodes of collaboration and
             joint attention.Again in these studies the general finding
             was one of cross-cultural similarity. In a final pair of
             studies, we assessed 2- to 3-year-old children's skills
             within two symbolic systems (pretense and pictorial). Here
             we found that the Canadian children who had much more
             experience with such symbols showed skills at an earlier
             age.Our overall conclusion is that young children in all
             cultural settings get sufficient amounts of the right kinds
             of social experience to develop their most basic
             social-cognitive skills for interacting with others and
             participating in culture at around the same age. In
             contrast, children's acquisition of more culturally specific
             skills for use in practices involving artifacts and symbols
             is more dependent on specific learning experiences.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00603.x},
   Key = {fds351735}
}

@article{fds351737,
   Author = {Stumper, B and Bannard, C and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {"Frequent frames" in German child-directed speech: a limited
             cue to grammatical categories.},
   Journal = {Cognitive science},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1190-1205},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01187.x},
   Abstract = {Mintz (2003) found that in English child-directed speech,
             frequently occurring frames formed by linking the preceding
             (A) and succeeding (B) word (A_x_B) could accurately predict
             the syntactic category of the intervening word (x). This has
             been successfully extended to French (Chemla, Mintz, Bernal,
             & Christophe, 2009). In this paper, we show that, as for
             Dutch (Erkelens, 2009), frequent frames in German do not
             enable such accurate lexical categorization. This can be
             explained by the characteristics of German including a less
             restricted word order compared to English or French and the
             frequent use of some forms as both determiner and pronoun in
             colloquial German. Finally, we explore the relationship
             between the accuracy of frames and their potential utility
             and find that even some of those frames showing high
             token-based accuracy are of limited value because they are
             in fact set phrases with little or no variability in the
             slot position.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01187.x},
   Key = {fds351737}
}

@article{fds351736,
   Author = {Hamann, K and Warneken, F and Greenberg, JR and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not
             in chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {476},
   Number = {7360},
   Pages = {328-331},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature10278},
   Abstract = {Humans actively share resources with one another to a much
             greater degree than do other great apes, and much human
             sharing is governed by social norms of fairness and equity.
             When in receipt of a windfall of resources, human children
             begin showing tendencies towards equitable distribution with
             others at five to seven years of age. Arguably, however, the
             primordial situation for human sharing of resources is that
             which follows cooperative activities such as collaborative
             foraging, when several individuals must share the spoils of
             their joint efforts. Here we show that children of around
             three years of age share with others much more equitably in
             collaborative activities than they do in either windfall or
             parallel-work situations. By contrast, one of humans' two
             nearest primate relatives, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes),
             'share' (make food available to another individual) just as
             often whether they have collaborated with them or not. This
             species difference raises the possibility that humans'
             tendency to distribute resources equitably may have its
             evolutionary roots in the sharing of spoils after
             collaborative efforts.},
   Doi = {10.1038/nature10278},
   Key = {fds351736}
}

@article{fds351738,
   Author = {Krajewski, G and Theakston, AL and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {How polish children switch from one case to another when
             using novel nouns: Challenges for models of inflectional
             morphology},
   Journal = {Language and Cognitive Processes},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {4-6},
   Pages = {830-861},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.506062},
   Abstract = {The two main models of children's acquisition of
             inflectional morphology-the Dual-Mechanism approach and the
             usage-based (schema-based) approach-have both been applied
             mainly to languages with fairly simple morphological
             systems. Here we report two studies of 2-3-year-old Polish
             children's ability to generalise across case-inflectional
             endings on nouns. In the first study, we found that the
             morphological form in which children first encounter a noun
             in Polish has a strong effect on their ability to produce
             other forms of that same noun. In the second study, we found
             that this effect is different depending on the target form
             to which children are switching. Similarity between
             inflectional endings played a crucial role in facilitating
             the task, whereas the simple frequency of either source or
             target forms was not a decisive factor in either study.
             These findings undermine Dual-Mechanism models that posit
             all-ornone acquisition of abstract morphological rules, and
             they also present serious challenges for usage-based models,
             in which frequency typically plays a key role. © 2010
             Psychology Press.},
   Doi = {10.1080/01690965.2010.506062},
   Key = {fds351738}
}

@article{fds351739,
   Author = {Grünloh, T and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {German children use prosody to identify participant roles in
             transitive sentences},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {393-419},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2011.015},
   Abstract = {Most studies examining children's understanding of
             transitive sentences focus on the morphosyntactic properties
             of the construction and ignore prosody. But adults use
             prosody in many different ways to interpret ambiguous
             sentences. In two studies we investigated whether 5-year-old
             German children use prosody to determine participant roles
             in object-first (OVS) sentences with novel verbs (i.e.,
             whether they use prosodic marking to overrule word order as
             a cue). Results showed that children identify participant
             roles better in this atypically ordered construction when
             sentences are realized with the marked, OVS-typical
             intonational pattern, especially in combination with case
             marking (Study 1). In a second study, we embedded these
             sentences into an appropriate discourse context. The results
             show that, even in the absence of any case marking, children
             understand participant roles when they are realized with the
             appropriate intonational pattern. These findings demonstrate
             that young children can use intonation to help identify
             participant roles in transitive sentences, at least in
             marked constructions such as the German object-first (OVS)
             construction. © 2011 Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG,
             Berlin/New York.},
   Doi = {10.1515/COGL.2011.015},
   Key = {fds351739}
}

@article{fds351740,
   Author = {Moll, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Social Engagement Leads 2-Year-Olds to Overestimate Others'
             Knowledge},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {248-265},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00044.x},
   Abstract = {Previous research has found that young children recognize an
             adult as being acquainted with an object most readily when
             the child and adult have previously engaged socially with
             that object together. In the current study, we tested the
             hypothesis that such social engagement is so powerful that
             it can sometimes lead children to overestimate what has been
             shared. After having shared two objects with an adult in
             turn, 2-year-old children played with a third object the
             adult could not see. In three out of four conditions, the
             adult remained co-present and/or communicated to the child
             while she played with the third object. Children falsely
             perceived the adult as being acquainted with the third
             object when she remained co-present (whether or not she also
             communicated) but not when she clearly terminated the
             interaction by disengaging and leaving. These results
             suggest that when young children are engaged with a
             co-present person they tend to overestimate the other's
             knowledge. © International Society on Infant Studies
             (ISIS).},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00044.x},
   Key = {fds351740}
}

@article{fds351741,
   Author = {Schmidt, MFH and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children attribute normativity to novel actions
             without pedagogy or normative language.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {530-539},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01000.x},
   Abstract = {Young children interpret some acts performed by adults as
             normatively governed, that is, as capable of being performed
             either rightly or wrongly. In previous experiments, children
             have made this interpretation when adults introduced them to
             novel acts with normative language (e.g. 'this is the way it
             goes'), along with pedagogical cues signaling culturally
             important information, and with social-pragmatic marking
             that this action is a token of a familiar type. In the
             current experiment, we exposed children to novel actions
             with no normative language, and we systematically varied
             pedagogical and social-pragmatic cues in an attempt to
             identify which of them, if either, would lead children to
             normative interpretations. We found that young 3-year-old
             children inferred normativity without any normative language
             and without any pedagogical cues. The only cue they used was
             adult social-pragmatic marking of the action as familiar, as
             if it were a token of a well-known type (as opposed to
             performing it, as if inventing it on the spot). These
             results suggest that - in the absence of explicit normative
             language - young children interpret adult actions as
             normatively governed based mainly on the intentionality
             (perhaps signaling conventionality) with which they are
             performed.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01000.x},
   Key = {fds351741}
}

@article{fds366596,
   Author = {Brandt, S and Verhagen, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {German children's productivity with simple transitive and
             complement-clause constructions: Testing the effects of
             frequency and variability},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {325-357},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2011.013},
   Abstract = {The development of abstract schemas and productive rules in
             language is affected by both token and type frequencies.
             High token frequencies and surface similarities help to
             discover formal and functional commonalities between
             utterances and categorize them as instances of the same
             schema. High type frequencies and diversity help to develop
             slots in these schemas, which allow the production and
             comprehension of novel utterances. In the current study we
             looked at both token and type frequencies in two related
             constructions in German child-directed speech: simple
             transitive and complement-clause constructions. Both
             constructions contain high frequency verbs, which
             potentially support the development of verb-specific
             schemas. However, only the frequent verbs in the transitive
             constructions occur with a variety of subject types, which
             also supports the development of a slot in the subject
             position. We then used an elicited production task to
             compare 4- and 5-year-old German-speaking children's
             productivity with simple transitive constructions and
             complement-clause constructions. The children were prompted
             to change the subjects of high and low frequency simple
             transitive verbs, such as essen 'eat' and naschen 'nibble',
             mental-state complement-taking verbs, such as denken 'think'
             and vermuten 'presume', and communication complement-taking
             verbs, such as sagen 'say' and berichten 'report'. In
             accordance with earlier findings, children had less
             difficulty producing new utterances with high frequency
             transitive verbs than with low frequency transitive verbs.
             For the other verb classes, however, we found either reverse
             frequency effects or no frequency effects. For these verb
             classes, children's productivity can be determined by
             diversity rather than simple token frequency. We discuss how
             token frequency interacts with diversity, discourse
             function, semantic complexity, and syntactic complexity. ©
             2011 Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG, Berlin/New
             York.},
   Doi = {10.1515/COGL.2011.013},
   Key = {fds366596}
}

@article{fds351742,
   Author = {Jorschick, L and Endesfelder Quick and A and Glässer, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {German-English-speaking children's mixed NPs with 'correct'
             agreement},
   Journal = {Bilingualism},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {173-183},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000131},
   Abstract = {Previous research has reported that bilingual children
             sometimes produce mixed noun phrases with 'correct' gender
             agreement- A s in der dog (der being a masculine determiner
             in German and the German word for dog, hund, being masculine
             as well). However, these could obviously be due to chance or
             to the indiscriminate use of a default determiner. In the
             current study, we established with high statistical
             reliability that each of three German-English bilingual
             children, of 2-4 years of age, produced such mixed NPs with
             'correct' agreement at significantly greater than chance
             levels. Also noteworthy was the fact that all three children
             produced such NPs with German determiners and English nouns
             much more frequently than the reverse. These findings
             provide a solid statistical foundation for further studies
             into the phenomenon of mixed noun phrases with 'correct'
             gender agreement.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S1366728910000131},
   Key = {fds351742}
}

@article{fds351743,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Missana, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral
             transgressions.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {Pt 1},
   Pages = {124-130},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151010x532888},
   Abstract = {We investigated children's moral behaviour in situations in
             which a third party was harmed (the test case for possession
             of agent-neutral moral norms). A 3-year-old and two puppets
             each created a picture or clay sculpture, after which one
             puppet left the room. In the Harm condition, the remaining
             (actor) puppet then destroyed the absent (recipient)
             puppet's picture or sculpture. In a Control condition, the
             actor acted similarly but in a way that did not harm the
             recipient. Children protested during the actor's actions,
             and, upon the recipient's return, tattled on the actor and
             behaved prosocially towards the recipient more in the Harm
             than in the Control condition. This is the first study to
             show that children as young as 3 years of age actively
             intervene in third-party moral transgressions.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151010x532888},
   Key = {fds351743}
}

@article{fds351744,
   Author = {Schmelz, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees know that others make inferences.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {108},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {3077-3079},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1000469108},
   Abstract = {If chimpanzees are faced with two opaque boards on a table,
             in the context of searching for a single piece of food, they
             do not choose the board lying flat (because if food was
             under there it would not be lying flat) but, rather, they
             choose the slanted one- presumably inferring that some
             unperceived food underneath is causing the slant. Here we
             demonstrate that chimpanzees know that other chimpanzees in
             the same situation will make a similar inference. In a
             back-and-forth foraging game, when their competitor had
             chosen before them, chimpanzees tended to avoid the slanted
             board on the assumption that the competitor had already
             chosen it. Chimpanzees can determine the inferences that a
             conspecific is likely to make and then adjust their
             competitive strategies accordingly.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1000469108},
   Key = {fds351744}
}

@article{fds351745,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Nitzschner, M and Wobber, V and Tennie, C and Bräuer,
             J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational
             acts?},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {81},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {195-203},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.10.001},
   Abstract = {Range et al. (2007, Current Biology, 17, 868-872) found that
             dogs, Canis familiaris, copy others' means to achieve a goal
             more often when those means are the rational solution to a
             problem than when they are irrational. In our first
             experiment, we added a further control condition and failed
             to replicate this result, suggesting that dogs in the
             previous study may have been distracted in the irrational
             condition rather than selectively attending to the
             irrational nature of the action. In a second experiment, the
             demonstrator used an unusual means (an extended leg) to
             communicate the location of food, either rationally (her
             hands were occupied) or irrationally (she could have used
             her hand). Dogs succeeded in finding the food irrespective
             of whether the leg action was rational or irrational. Our
             results suggest that dogs do not distinguish rational from
             irrational acts, instead simply being proficient at
             monitoring human behavioural patterns. © 2010 The
             Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.10.001},
   Key = {fds351745}
}

@article{fds351746,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Warneken, F and Jensen, K and Schneider, AC and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food
             items},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
             Sciences},
   Volume = {278},
   Number = {1710},
   Pages = {1405-1413},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1735},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) sometimes help both humans and
             conspecifics in experimental situations in which immediate
             selfish benefits can be ruled out. However, in several
             experiments, chimpanzees have not provided food to a
             conspecific even when it would cost them nothing, leading to
             the hypothesis that prosociality in the food-provisioning
             context is a derived trait in humans. Here, we show that
             chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain both food and non-food
             items-given that the donor cannot get the food herself.
             Furthermore, we show that the key factor eliciting
             chimpanzees' targeted helping is the recipients' attempts to
             either get the food or get the attention of the potential
             donor. The current findings add to the accumulating body of
             evidence that humans and chimpanzees share the motivation
             and skills necessary to help others in situations in which
             they cannot selfishly benefit. Humans, however, show
             prosocial motives more readily and in a wider range of
             contexts. © 2010 The Royal Society.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2010.1735},
   Key = {fds351746}
}

@article{fds351747,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Individual differences in social, cognitive, and
             morphological aspects of infant pointing},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {16-29},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.10.001},
   Abstract = {Little is known about the origins of the pointing gesture.
             We sought to gain insight into its emergence by
             investigating individual differences in the pointing of
             12-month-old infants in two ways. First, we looked at
             differences in the communicative and interactional uses of
             pointing and asked how different hand shapes relate to point
             frequency, accompanying vocalizations, and mothers'
             pointing. Second, we looked at differences in
             social-cognitive skills of point comprehension and imitation
             and tested whether these were related to infants' own
             pointing. Infants' and mothers' spontaneous pointing
             correlated with one another, as did infants' point
             production and comprehension. In particular, infants'
             index-finger pointing had a profile different from simple
             whole-hand pointing. It was more frequent, it was more often
             accompanied by vocalizations, and it correlated more
             strongly with comprehension of pointing (especially to
             occluded referents). We conclude that whole-hand and
             index-finger pointing differ qualitatively and suggest that
             it is index-finger pointing that first embodies infants'
             understanding of communicative intentions. © 2011 Elsevier
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.10.001},
   Key = {fds351747}
}

@article{fds351749,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Lohse, K and Melis, AP and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children share the spoils after collaboration},
   Journal = {Psychological Science},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {267-273},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610395392},
   Abstract = {Egalitarian behavior is considered to be a species-typical
             component of human cooperation. Human adults tend to share
             resources equally, even if they have the opportunity to keep
             a larger portion for themselves. Recent experiments have
             suggested that this tendency emerges fairly late in human
             ontogeny, not before 6 or 7 years of age. Here we show that
             3-year-old children share mostly equally with a peer after
             they have worked together actively to obtain rewards in a
             collaboration task, even when those rewards could easily be
             monopolized. These findings contrast with previous findings
             from a similar experiment with chimpanzees, who tended to
             monopolize resources whenever they could. The potentially
             species-unique tendency of humans to share equally emerges
             early in ontogeny, perhaps originating in collaborative
             interactions among peers. © The Author(s)
             2011.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797610395392},
   Key = {fds351749}
}

@article{fds351750,
   Author = {Scheider, L and Grassmann, S and Kaminski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice
             when following a human pointing gesture.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {e21676},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021676},
   Abstract = {Domestic dogs are skillful at using the human pointing
             gesture. In this study we investigated whether dogs take
             contextual information into account when following pointing
             gestures, specifically, whether they follow human pointing
             gestures more readily in the context in which food has been
             found previously. Also varied was the human's tone of voice
             as either imperative or informative. Dogs were more
             sustained in their searching behavior in the 'context'
             condition as opposed to the 'no context' condition,
             suggesting that they do not simply follow a pointing gesture
             blindly but use previously acquired contextual information
             to inform their interpretation of that pointing gesture.
             Dogs also showed more sustained searching behavior when
             there was pointing than when there was not, suggesting that
             they expect to find a referent when they see a human point.
             Finally, dogs searched more in high-pitched informative
             trials as opposed to the low-pitched imperative trials,
             whereas in the latter dogs seemed more inclined to respond
             by sitting. These findings suggest that a dog's response to
             a pointing gesture is flexible and depends on the context as
             well as the human's tone of voice.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0021676},
   Key = {fds351750}
}

@article{fds351751,
   Author = {Bullinger, AF and Zimmermann, F and Kaminski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Different social motives in the gestural communication of
             chimpanzees and human children.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {58-68},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00952.x},
   Abstract = {Both chimpanzees and human infants use the pointing gesture
             with human adults, but it is not clear if they are doing so
             for the same social motives. In two studies, we presented
             chimpanzees and human 25-month-olds with the opportunity to
             point for a hidden tool (in the presence of a non-functional
             distractor). In one condition it was clear that the tool
             would be used to retrieve a reward for the pointing subject
             (so the pointing was selfish or 'for-me'), whereas in the
             other condition it was clear that the tool would be used to
             retrieve the reward for the experimenter (so the pointing
             was helpful or 'for-you'). The chimpanzees pointed reliably
             only when they themselves benefited, whereas the human
             children pointed reliably no matter who benefited. These
             results are interpreted as evidence for the especially
             cooperative nature of human communication.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00952.x},
   Key = {fds351751}
}

@article{fds351753,
   Author = {Grosse, G and Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {21-Month-olds understand the cooperative logic of
             requests},
   Journal = {Journal of Pragmatics},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {3377-3383},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2010.05.005},
   Abstract = {Human communication rests on a basic assumption of partner
             cooperativeness, including even requesting. In the current
             study, an adult made an ambiguous request for an object to
             21-month-old infants, with one potential referent being
             right in front of her and the other being across the room.
             In a normal situation (Hands-Free), infants interpreted the
             request as referring to the distant object-the one the adult
             needed help fetching. In contrast, in a situation in which
             the adult was constrained so that fetching either object
             herself would be difficult (Hands-Occupied), infants
             selected the far object much less often. These results
             suggest that infants just beginning to acquire language
             already understand something of the cooperative logic of
             requests. © 2010 Elsevier B.V.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.pragma.2010.05.005},
   Key = {fds351753}
}

@article{fds351754,
   Author = {Greenberg, JR and Hamann, K and Warneken, F and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzee helping in collaborative and noncollaborative
             contexts},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {80},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {873-880},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.08.008},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, help others in a variety of
             contexts. Some researchers have claimed that this only
             occurs when food is not involved and the recipient actively
             solicits help. In the current study, however, we found that
             chimpanzees often helped conspecifics obtain food in a
             pulling task with no solicitation whatsoever, in a situation
             in which, based on past experience, the conspecific's desire
             for the food was apparent. We also assessed whether the
             collaborative context of the situation impacted helping
             rates. Specifically, we compared how often both partners
             obtained rewards when one partner needed the help of the
             other, who had already received a reward for free (helping
             without collaboration), and when one partner needed the
             other's help after they had already begun collaborating
             (helping during collaboration). Partners provided assistance
             significantly more often in both of these helping conditions
             than in a control condition in which partners could provide
             unneeded help. However, unlike human children who have been
             tested in a similar task, chimpanzees did not help their
             partner more during (than without) collaboration. These
             results suggest that chimpanzees' helping behaviour is more
             robust than previously believed, but at the same time may
             have different evolutionary roots from the helping behaviour
             of humans. © 2010 The Association for the Study of Animal
             Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.08.008},
   Key = {fds351754}
}

@article{fds351755,
   Author = {Grassmann, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds' attention
             to a contextually new referent},
   Journal = {Journal of Pragmatics},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {3098-3105},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2010.04.019},
   Abstract = {From the very beginning of language acquisition, young
             children are sensitive to what is given versus what is new
             in their discourse with others. Here we ask whether
             24-month-olds use this skill to interpret prosodic
             highlighting as an invitation to focus their attention on
             what is new in the situation. Using an eye-tracking
             methodology, we compared children's visual fixation of
             referents that were given versus those that were new in the
             situation when the prosodic highlighting of their
             corresponding word varied. Results showed that 24-month-old
             children looked longer to the referents of prosodically
             stressed words when those referents were new to the context.
             Neither stress of the word alone nor newness of the referent
             alone was sufficient to induce children to focus their
             attention on the target referent. These results suggest that
             from an early age children understand at least one important
             communicative function of prosodic stress. © 2010 Elsevier
             B.V.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.pragma.2010.04.019},
   Key = {fds351755}
}

@article{fds351756,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children selectively avoid helping people with harmful
             intentions.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {81},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1661-1669},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01500.x},
   Abstract = {Two studies investigated whether young children are
             selectively prosocial toward others, based on the others'
             moral behaviors. In Study 1 (N = 54), 3-year-olds watched 1
             adult (the actor) harming or helping another adult. Children
             subsequently helped the harmful actor less often than a
             third (previously neutral) adult, but helped the helpful and
             neutral adults equally often. In Study 2 (N = 36),
             3-year-olds helped an actor who intended but failed to harm
             another adult less often than a neutral adult, but helped an
             accidentally harmful and a neutral adult equally often.
             Children's prosocial behavior was thus mediated by the
             intentions behind the actor's moral behavior, irrespective
             of outcome. Children thus selectively avoid helping those
             who cause--or even intend to cause--others
             harm.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01500.x},
   Key = {fds351756}
}

@article{fds351757,
   Author = {Grosse, G and Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Infants communicate in order to be understood.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {46},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1710-1722},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020727},
   Abstract = {Infants intentionally communicate with others from before
             their 1st birthday. But there is some question about how
             they understand the communicative process. Do they
             understand that for their request to work the recipient must
             both understand the request and be cooperatively disposed to
             fulfill it? On the basis of the study by Shwe and Markman
             (1997), we developed a new paradigm that tested whether and
             how 18-, 24-, and 30-month-old children repair a failed
             request. Children at all ages repaired their requests in the
             case of a misunderstanding even if they had obtained the
             requested object already. They also repaired differently
             depending on the precise reason for the communicative
             failure (e.g., misunderstanding the referent versus the
             communicative intent) and did not repair in the case of
             correct understanding, even if they did not get the
             requested object. Thus, from very early in their
             communicative careers, young children operate with a basic
             understanding of the mental and cooperative nature of human
             communication.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0020727},
   Key = {fds351757}
}

@article{fds351758,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Hamann, K and Warneken, F and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Bigger knows better: young children selectively learn rule
             games from adults rather than from peers.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {Pt 4},
   Pages = {785-798},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151009x479178},
   Abstract = {Preschoolers' selective learning from adult versus peer
             models was investigated. Extending previous research,
             children from age 3 were shown to selectively learn simple
             rule games from adult rather than peer models. Furthermore,
             this selective learning was not confined to preferentially
             performing certain acts oneself, but more specifically had a
             normative dimension to it: children understood the way the
             adult demonstrated an act not only as the better one, but as
             the normatively appropriate/correct one. This was indicated
             in their spontaneous normative interventions (protest,
             critique, etc.) in response to third party acts deviating
             from the one demonstrated by the adult model. Various
             interpretations of these findings are discussed in the
             broader context of the development of children's social
             cognition and cultural learning.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151009x479178},
   Key = {fds351758}
}

@article{fds351759,
   Author = {Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Infant cognition},
   Journal = {Current Biology},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {20},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.001},
   Abstract = {Until fairly recently, young infants were thought to be as
             cognitively incompetent as they were morally innocent. They
             were epistemological 'tabulae rasae', helpless 'bundles of
             reflexes' who spent all of their time sleeping, crying and
             sucking. In the famous words of William James, infants lived
             in "one great blooming, buzzing confusion". © 2010 Elsevier
             Ltd. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.001},
   Key = {fds351759}
}

@article{fds351761,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Infants' use of shared experience in declarative
             pointing},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {545-556},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00028.x},
   Abstract = {In this study, we asked whether 14- and 18-month-old infants
             use the experiences they have previously shared with others
             when deciding what to point to for them declaratively. After
             sharing a particular type of referent with an adult in an
             excited manner, 18-month-olds subsequently found a picture
             of that type of referent more worthy of declarative pointing
             than some other picture-but only for that adult, not for a
             different adult. Mixed results were found with
             14-month-olds. We thus show that by 18 months, infants
             accurately track their shared experiences with specific
             individuals and use this to make communicative decisions.
             These results also demonstrate that infants sometimes use
             declarative pointing to indicate not totally "new" things,
             as in the classic formulation, but things which are "old" in
             the sense that "we" should recognize them as similar to
             something we have previously shared. © International
             Society on Infant Studies (ISIS).},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00028.x},
   Key = {fds351761}
}

@article{fds351762,
   Author = {Kirschner Sebastian and S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old
             children},
   Journal = {Evolution and Human Behavior},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {354-364},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004},
   Abstract = {Humansw are the only primates that make music. But the
             evolutionary origins and functions of music are unclear.
             Given that in traditional cultures music making and dancing
             are often integral parts of important group ceremonies such
             as initiation rites, weddings or preparations for battle,
             one hypothesis is that music evolved into a tool that
             fosters social bonding and group cohesion, ultimately
             increasing prosocial in-group behavior and cooperation. Here
             we provide support for this hypothesis by showing that joint
             music making among 4-year-old children increases subsequent
             spontaneous cooperative and helpful behavior, relative to a
             carefully matched control condition with the same level of
             social and linguistic interaction but no music. Among other
             functional mechanisms, we propose that music making,
             including joint singing and dancing, encourages the
             participants to keep a constant audiovisual representation
             of the collective intention and shared goal of vocalizing
             and moving together in time - thereby effectively satisfying
             the intrinsic human desire to share emotions, experiences
             and activities with others. © 2010 Elsevier
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004},
   Key = {fds351762}
}

@article{fds351763,
   Author = {Brandt, S and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Development ofword order in german complement-clause
             constructions: Effects of input frequencies, lexical items,
             and discourse function},
   Journal = {Language},
   Volume = {86},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {583-610},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.2010.0010},
   Abstract = {We investigate the development of word order in German
             children's spontaneous production of complement clauses.
             From soon after their second birthday, young German children
             use both verb-final complements with complementizers and
             verb-second complements without complementizers. By their
             third birthday they use both kinds of complement clauses
             with a variety of complement-taking verbs. Early in
             development, however, verb-final complements and verbsecond
             complements are used with separate sets of complement-taking
             verbs, and they are used with separate sets of item-specific
             main-clause phrases. For example, initially phrases such as
             'I want to see' were used exclusively with verb-final
             complements, whereas phrases such as 'do you see' and 'you
             have to say' were used exclusively with verb-second
             complements. Only later in development-when specific
             complement-taking verbs were used with both verb-second and
             verbfinal complements, with a greater variety of main-clause
             phrases, and when specific main-clause phrases were used
             with both verb-second and verb-final complements-was there
             evidence for structural links between these various,
             item-based, complement-clause constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1353/lan.2010.0010},
   Key = {fds351763}
}

@article{fds320798,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Hare, B and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and
             chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {e12438},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012438},
   Abstract = {While bonobos and chimpanzees are both genetically and
             behaviorally very similar, they also differ in significant
             ways. Bonobos are more cautious and socially tolerant while
             chimpanzees are more dependent on extractive foraging, which
             requires tools. The similarities suggest the two species
             should be cognitively similar while the behavioral
             differences predict where the two species should differ
             cognitively. We compared both species on a wide range of
             cognitive problems testing their understanding of the
             physical and social world. Bonobos were more skilled at
             solving tasks related to theory of mind or an understanding
             of social causality, while chimpanzees were more skilled at
             tasks requiring the use of tools and an understanding of
             physical causality. These species differences support the
             role of ecological and socio-ecological pressures in shaping
             cognitive skills over relatively short periods of
             evolutionary time.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0012438},
   Key = {fds320798}
}

@article{fds351765,
   Author = {Seed, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Primate cognition.},
   Journal = {Topics in cognitive science},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {407-419},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01099.x},
   Abstract = {As the cognitive revolution was slow to come to the study of
             animal behavior, the vast majority of what we know about
             primate cognition has been discovered in the last 30 years.
             Building on the recognition that the physical and social
             worlds of humans and their living primate relatives pose
             many of the same evolutionary challenges, programs of
             research have established that the most basic cognitive
             skills and mental representations that humans use to
             navigate those worlds are already possessed by other
             primates. There may be differences between humans and other
             primates, however, in more complex cognitive skills, such as
             reasoning about relations, causality, time, and other minds.
             Of special importance, the human primate seems to possess a
             species-unique set of adaptations for "cultural
             intelligence," which are broad reaching in their effects on
             human cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01099.x},
   Key = {fds351765}
}

@article{fds351766,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What's in a manner of speaking? Children's sensitivity to
             partner-specific referential precedents.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {46},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {749-760},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019657},
   Abstract = {Do young children form "referential pacts"? If a person has
             referred to an object with a certain term (e.g., the horse),
             will children expect this person to use this term in the
             future but allow others to use a different expression (e.g.,
             the pony)? One hundred twenty-eight children between 3 and 5
             years old co-operated with an experimenter (E1) to move toys
             to new locations on a shelf. E1 established referential
             terms for all toys in a warm-up game. Then, either the
             original partner, E1, or a new partner, E2, played a second
             game with the same toys. In this game, the experimenters
             referred to toys using either their original terms from the
             warm-up game or new terms. Children were slower to react to
             new terms than old, and this difference in reaction times
             was greater in the original partner condition (but only on
             the first trial). Children sometimes protested at the use of
             new terms, doing so regardless of their interlocutor's
             identity. We contrast these findings with those for adults
             and discuss their implications for the debate regarding the
             nature of referential pacts.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0019657},
   Key = {fds351766}
}

@article{fds351767,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Herrmann, E},
   Title = {Ape and human cognition: What's the difference?},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {3-8},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359300},
   Abstract = {Humans share the vast majority of their cognitive skills
             with other great apes. In addition, however, humans have
             also evolved a unique suite of cognitive skills and
             motivations-collectively referred to as shared
             intentionality-for living collaboratively, learning
             socially, and exchanging information in cultural groups. ©
             The Author(s) 2010.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0963721409359300},
   Key = {fds351767}
}

@article{fds351768,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {36-month-olds conceal visual and auditory information from
             others.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {479-489},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00892.x},
   Abstract = {By three years of age, children are skilled at assessing
             under which circumstances others can see things. However,
             nothing is known about whether they can use this knowledge
             to guide their own deceptive behaviour. Here we investigated
             3-year-olds' ability to strategically inhibit or conceal
             forbidden actions that a nearby adult experimenter could see
             or hear. In the first experiment, children were more likely
             to disobey the adult when she did not have visual access to
             their activities than they were when she was looking at
             them. In the second experiment, in which the adult could
             never see the child, children refrained from making noise
             when engaging in a prohibited action that the adult might
             hear. These results suggest that by three years of age
             children use their knowledge of others' perceptual states to
             decide whether it is safe to commit a transgression and,
             moreover, actively conceal perceptual cues that could reveal
             to others their ongoing transgression.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00892.x},
   Key = {fds351768}
}

@article{fds351779,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings
             using the floating peanut task.},
   Journal = {PloS one},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {e10544},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010544},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>It is still unclear which observational
             learning mechanisms underlie the transmission of difficult
             problem-solving skills in chimpanzees. In particular, two
             different mechanisms have been proposed: imitation and
             emulation. Previous studies have largely failed to control
             for social factors when these mechanisms were
             targeted.<h4>Methods</h4>In an attempt to resolve the
             existing discrepancies, we adopted the 'floating peanut
             task', in which subjects need to spit water into a tube
             until it is sufficiently full for floating peanuts to be
             grasped. In a previous study only a few chimpanzees were
             able to invent the necessary solution (and they either did
             so in their first trials or never). Here we compared success
             levels in baseline tests with two experimental conditions
             that followed: 1) A full model condition to test whether
             social demonstrations would be effective, and 2) A social
             emulation control condition, in which a human experimenter
             poured water from a bottle into the tube, to test whether
             results information alone (present in both experimental
             conditions) would also induce successes. Crucially, we
             controlled for social factors in both experimental
             conditions. Both types of demonstrations significantly
             increased successful spitting, with no differences between
             demonstration types. We also found that younger subjects
             were more likely to succeed than older ones. Our analysis
             showed that mere order effects could not explain our
             results.<h4>Conclusion</h4>The full demonstration condition
             (which potentially offers additional information to
             observers, in the form of actions), induced no more
             successes than the emulation condition. Hence, emulation
             learning could explain the success in both conditions. This
             finding has broad implications for the interpretation of
             chimpanzee traditions, for which emulation learning may
             perhaps suffice.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0010544},
   Key = {fds351779}
}

@article{fds351772,
   Author = {Abbot-Smith, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The influence of frequency and semantic similarity on how
             children learn grammar},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {79-101},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723709350525},
   Abstract = {Lexically based learning and semantic analogy may both play
             a role in the learning of grammar. To investigate this,
             5-year-old German children were trained on a miniature
             language (nominally English) involving two grammatical
             constructions, each of which was associated with a different
             semantic verb class. Training was followed by elicited
             production and grammaticality judgement tests with trained
             verbs and a generalization test, involving untrained verbs.
             In the trained verbs judgement test the children were above
             chance at associating particular verbs with the
             constructions in which they had heard them. They did this
             significantly more often with verbs which they had heard
             especially frequently in particular constructions,
             indicating lexically based learning. There was also an
             interaction between frequency and semantic class (or the
             particular verbs). In the generalization judgement test the
             children were at chance overall. In the elicited production
             generalization test 75% of the children used the same
             construction for all items.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0142723709350525},
   Key = {fds351772}
}

@article{fds320799,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Hernández-Lloreda, MV and Call, J and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The structure of individual differences in the cognitive
             abilities of children and chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {102-110},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797609356511},
   Abstract = {Most studies of animal cognition focus on group performance
             and neglect individual differences and the correlational
             structure of cognitive abilities. Moreover, no previous
             studies have compared the correlational structure of
             cognitive abilities in nonhuman animals and humans. We
             compared the structure of individual differences of 106
             chimpanzees and 105 two-year-old human children using 15
             cognitive tasks that posed problems about the physical or
             social world. We found a similar factor of spatial cognition
             for the two species. But whereas the chimpanzees had only a
             single factor in addition to spatial cognition, the children
             had two distinct additional factors: one for physical
             cognition and one for social cognition. These findings, in
             combination with previous research, support the proposal
             that humans share many cognitive skills with nonhuman apes,
             especially for dealing with the physical world, but in
             addition have evolved some specialized skills of social
             cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797609356511},
   Key = {fds320799}
}

@article{fds351748,
   Author = {Ibbotson, P and Theakston, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The role of pronoun frames in early comprehension of
             transitive constructions in English},
   Journal = {Language Learning and Development},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {24-39},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441003732914},
   Abstract = {Case marking in English in available only on some pronouns
             and only in some cases. It is unknown whether young children
             acquiring English nevertheless make use of this highly
             restricted marking as a cue to sentence interpretation. The
             current study therefore examined how 2- and 3-year-old
             English children use case-marked pronoun frames and
             constructional word order cues (actives versus passives) to
             understand agent-patient relations in transitive sentences
             containing novel verbs. In a pointing comprehension test,
             2-year-olds used pronoun frames containing two case-marked
             pronouns to help them interpret grammatical sentences, both
             actives and passives, but they were unable to assign agent
             patient relationships in any consistent way with
             ungrammatical pronoun frames. Threeyear- olds also used
             pronoun frames to interpret grammatical active and passive
             sentences (with either one or two case-marked pronouns) but
             varied in their interpretation of ungrammatical sentences
             according to pronoun frame. These results suggest that the
             role of case-marked pronouns has been underestimated in
             English language acquisition, and that even very young
             English children use multiple cues to comprehend transitive
             sentences. © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15475441003732914},
   Key = {fds351748}
}

@article{fds351775,
   Author = {Kidd, E and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Lexical frequency and exemplar-based learning effects in
             language acquisition: evidence from sentential
             complements},
   Journal = {Language Sciences},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {132-142},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2009.05.002},
   Abstract = {Usage-based approaches to language acquisition argue that
             children acquire the grammar of their target language using
             general-cognitive learning principles. The current paper
             reports on an experiment that tested a central assumption of
             the usage-based approach: argument structure patterns are
             connected to high frequency verbs that facilitate
             acquisition. Sixty children (N = 60) aged 4- and 6-years
             participated in a sentence recall/lexical priming experiment
             that manipulated the frequency with which the target verbs
             occurred in the finite sentential complement construction in
             English. The results showed that the children performed
             better on sentences that contained high frequency verbs.
             Furthermore, the children's performance suggested that their
             knowledge of finite sentential complements relies most
             heavily on one particular verb - think, supporting arguments
             made by Goldberg [Goldberg, A.E., 2006. Constructions at
             Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford
             University Press, Oxford], who argued that skewed input
             facilitates language learning. Crown Copyright ©
             2009.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.langsci.2009.05.002},
   Key = {fds351775}
}

@article{fds351776,
   Author = {Salomo, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's sensitivity to new and given information
             when answering predicate-focus questions},
   Journal = {Applied Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {101-115},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S014271640999018X},
   Abstract = {In two studies we investigated 2-year-old children's answers
             to predicate-focus questions depending on the preceding
             context. Children were presented with a successive series of
             short video clips showing transitive actions (e.g., frog
             washing duck) in which either the action (action-new) or the
             patient (patient-new) was the changing, and therefore new,
             element. During the last scene the experimenter asked the
             question (e.g., What's the frog doing now?). We found that
             children expressed the action and the patient in the
             patient-new condition but expressed only the action in the
             action-new condition. These results show that children are
             sensitive to both the predicate-focus question and newness
             in context. A further finding was that children expressed
             new patients in their answers more often when there was a
             verbal context prior to the questions than when there was
             not. © 2009 Cambridge University Press.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S014271640999018X},
   Key = {fds351776}
}

@article{fds351777,
   Author = {Chan, A and Meints, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's comprehension of English SVO word order
             revisited: Testing the same children in act-out and
             intermodal preferential looking tasks},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {30-45},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.10.002},
   Abstract = {Act-out and intermodal preferential looking (IPL) tasks were
             administered to 67 English children aged 2-0, 2-9 and 3-5 to
             assess their comprehension of canonical SVO transitive word
             order with both familiar and novel verbs. Children at 3-5
             and at 2-9 showed evidence of comprehending word order in
             both verb conditions and both tasks, although children at
             2-9 performed better with familiar than with novel verbs in
             the act-out task. Children at 2-0 showed no evidence of
             comprehending word order in either task with novel verbs;
             with familiar verbs they showed competence in the IPL task
             but not in the act-out task. The difference in performance
             for familiar and novel verbs from the same children at 2-0,
             on the IPL task, and at 2-9, on the act-out task, is
             consistent with the hypothesis that early
             linguistic/cognitive representations are graded in strength,
             with early representations still weak and very task
             dependent. However, these representations also become more
             abstract with development, as indicated by the familiarity
             effect even in the more sensitive IPL task. © 2009 Elsevier
             Inc. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.10.002},
   Key = {fds351777}
}

@article{fds351778,
   Author = {Grassmann, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting
             acts of reference.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {252-263},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00871.x},
   Abstract = {Adults refer young children's attention to things in two
             basic ways: through the use of pointing (and other deictic
             gestures) and words (and other linguistic conventions). In
             the current studies, we referred young children (2- and
             4-year-olds) to things in conflicting ways, that is, by
             pointing to one object while indicating linguistically (in
             some way) a different object. In Study 1, a novel word was
             put into competition with a pointing gesture in a mutual
             exclusivity paradigm; that is, with a known and a novel
             object in front of the child, the adult pointed to the known
             object (e.g. a cup) while simultaneously requesting 'the
             modi'. In contrast to the findings of Jaswal and Hansen
             (2006), children followed almost exclusively the pointing
             gesture. In Study 2, when a known word was put into
             competition with a pointing gesture - the adult pointed to
             the novel object but requested 'the car'- children still
             followed the pointing gesture. In Study 3, the referent of
             the pointing gesture was doubly contradicted by the lexical
             information - the adult pointed to a known object (e.g. a
             cup) but requested 'the car'- in which case children
             considered pointing and lexical information equally strong.
             Together, these findings suggest that in disambiguating acts
             of reference, young children at both 2 and 4 years of age
             rely most heavily on pragmatic information (e.g. in a
             pointing gesture), and only secondarily on lexical
             conventions and principles.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00871.x},
   Key = {fds351778}
}

@article{fds352505,
   Author = {Whiten, A and McGrew, WC and Aiello, LC and Boesch, C and Boyd, R and Byrne, RW and Dunbar, RIM and Matsuzawa, T and Silk, JB and Tomasello,
             M and van Schaik, CP and Wrangham, R},
   Title = {Studying extant species to model our past.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {327},
   Number = {5964},
   Pages = {410},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.327.5964.410-a},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.327.5964.410-a},
   Key = {fds352505}
}

@article{fds320797,
   Author = {Hare, B and Rosati, AG and Kaminski, J and Braeuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The domestication hypothesis for dogs' skills with human
             communication: A response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne
             et al. (2008)},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {79},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {e1-e6},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2010},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.031},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.031},
   Key = {fds320797}
}

@article{fds320800,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees coordinate in a negotiation game},
   Journal = {Evolution and Human Behavior},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {381-392},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.05.003},
   Abstract = {A crucially important aspect of human cooperation is the
             ability to negotiate to cooperative outcomes when interests
             over resources conflict. Although chimpanzees and other
             social species may negotiate conflicting interests regarding
             travel direction or activity timing, very little is known
             about their ability to negotiate conflicting preferences
             over food. In the current study, we presented pairs of
             chimpanzees with a choice between two cooperative tasks-one
             with equal payoffs (e.g., 5-5) and one with unequal payoffs
             (higher and lower than in the equal option, e.g., 10-1).
             This created a conflict of interests between partners with
             failure to work together on the same cooperative task
             resulting in no payoff for either partner. The chimpanzee
             pairs cooperated successfully in as many as 78-94% of the
             trials across experiments. Even though dominant chimpanzees
             preferred the unequal option (as they would obtain the
             largest payoff), subordinate chimpanzees were able to get
             their way (the equal option) in 22-56% of trials across
             conditions. Various analyses showed that subjects were both
             strategic and also cognizant of the strategies used by their
             partners. These results demonstrate that one of our two
             closest primate relatives, the chimpanzee, can settle
             conflicts of interest over resources in mutually satisfying
             ways-even without the social norms of equity, planned
             strategies of reciprocity, and the complex communication
             characteristic of human negotiation. © 2009 Elsevier Inc.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.05.003},
   Key = {fds320800}
}

@article{fds351780,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Done wrong or said wrong? Young children understand the
             normative directions of fit of different speech
             acts.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {113},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {205-212},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.013},
   Abstract = {Young children use and comprehend different kinds of speech
             acts from the beginning of their communicative development.
             But it is not clear how they understand the conventional and
             normative structure of such speech acts. In particular,
             imperative speech acts have a world-to-word direction of
             fit, such that their fulfillment means that the world must
             change to fit the word. In contrast, assertive speech acts
             have a word-to-world direction of fit, such that their
             fulfillment means that the word must fit the world truly. In
             the current study, 3-year-olds understood this difference
             explicitly, as they directed their criticisms selectively to
             actors when they did not follow the imperatives of the
             speaker, but to speakers when they did not describe an
             actor's actions correctly. Two-year-olds criticized
             appropriately in the case of imperatives, but showed a more
             ambiguous pattern in the case of assertions. These findings
             identify another domain in which children's normative
             understanding of human activity emerges around the third
             year of life.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.013},
   Key = {fds351780}
}

@article{fds351781,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Tempelmann, S and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic
             signs.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {831-837},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00815.x},
   Abstract = {A key skill in early human development is the ability to
             comprehend communicative intentions as expressed in both
             nonlinguistic gestures and language. In the current studies,
             we confronted domestic dogs (some of whom knew many human
             'words') with a task in which they had to infer the intended
             referent of a human's communicative act via iconic
             signs--specifically, replicas and photographs. Both trained
             and untrained dogs successfully used iconic replicas to
             fetch the desired item, with many doing so from the first
             trial. Dogs' ability to use photographs in this same
             situation was less consistent. Because simple matching to
             sample in experimental contexts typically takes hundreds of
             trials (and because similarity between iconic sign and
             target item did not predict success), we propose that dogs'
             skillful performance in the current task reflects important
             aspects of the comprehension of human communicative
             intentions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00815.x},
   Key = {fds351781}
}

@article{fds351782,
   Author = {Bannard, C and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Modeling children's early grammatical knowledge.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {106},
   Number = {41},
   Pages = {17284-17289},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905638106},
   Abstract = {Theories of grammatical development differ in how much
             abstract knowledge they attribute to young children. Here,
             we report a series of experiments using a computational
             model to evaluate the explanatory power of child grammars
             based not on abstract rules but on concrete words and
             phrases and some local abstractions associated with these
             words and phrases. We use a Bayesian procedure to extract
             such item-based grammars from transcriptions of 28+ h of
             each of two children's speech at 2 and 3 years of age. We
             then use these grammars to parse all of the unique multiword
             utterances from transcriptions of separate recordings of
             these same children at each of the two ages. We found that
             at 2 years of age such a model had good coverage and
             predictive fit, with the children showing radically limited
             productivity. Furthermore, adding expert-annotated parts of
             speech to the induction procedure had little effect on
             coverage, with the exception of the category of noun. At age
             3, the children's productivity sharply increased and the
             addition of a verb and a noun category markedly improved the
             model's performance.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0905638106},
   Key = {fds351782}
}

@article{fds351783,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Universal grammar is dead},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {470-471},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09990744},
   Abstract = {The idea of a biologically evolved, universal grammar with
             linguistic content is a myth, perpetuated by three spurious
             explanatory strategies of generative linguists. To make
             progress in understanding human linguistic competence,
             cognitive scientists must abandon the idea of an innate
             universal grammar and instead try to build theories that
             explain both linguistic universals and diversity and how
             they emerge. © 2009 Cambridge University
             Press.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X09990744},
   Key = {fds351783}
}

@article{fds320801,
   Author = {Wobber, V and Hare, B and Koler-Matznick, J and Wrangham, R and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Breed differences in domestic dogs' (Canis familiaris)
             comprehension of human communicative signals},
   Journal = {Interaction Studies},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {206-224},
   Publisher = {John Benjamins Publishing Company},
   Editor = {Matsuzawa, T},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/is.10.2.06wob},
   Abstract = {Recent research suggests that some human-like social skills
             evolved in dogs (Canis familiaris) during domestication as
             an incidental by-product of selection for "tame" forms of
             behavior. It is still possible, however, that the social
             skills of certain dog breeds came under direct selection
             that led to further increases in social problem solving
             ability. To test this hypothesis, different breeds of
             domestic dogs were compared for their ability to use various
             human communicative behaviors to find hidden food. We found
             that even primitive breeds with little human contact were
             able to use communicative cues. Further, "working" dogs
             (shepherds and huskies: thought to be bred intentionally to
             respond to human cooperative communicative signals) were
             more skilled at using gestural cues than were non-working
             breeds (basenji and toy poodles: not thought to have been
             bred for their cooperative-communicative ability). This
             difference in performance existed regardless of whether the
             working breeds were more or less genetically wolf-like.
             These results suggest that subsequent to initial
             domesticating selection giving rise to cue-following skills,
             additional selection on communicative abilities in certain
             breeds has produced substantive differences in those breeds'
             abilities to follow cues. © John Benjamins Publishing
             Company.},
   Doi = {10.1075/is.10.2.06wob},
   Key = {fds320801}
}

@article{fds351784,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Society need not be selfish},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {461},
   Number = {7260},
   Pages = {41},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/461041a},
   Doi = {10.1038/461041a},
   Key = {fds351784}
}

@article{fds351785,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {397-402},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.008},
   Abstract = {Recent empirical research has shed new light on the
             perennial question of human altruism. A number of recent
             studies suggest that from very early in ontogeny young
             children have a biological predisposition to help others
             achieve their goals, to share resources with others and to
             inform others of things helpfully. Humans' nearest primate
             relatives, such as chimpanzees, engage in some but not all
             of these behaviors: they help others instrumentally, but
             they are not so inclined to share resources altruistically
             and they do not inform others of things helpfully. The
             evolutionary roots of human altruism thus appear to be much
             more complex than previously supposed.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.008},
   Key = {fds351785}
}

@article{fds351786,
   Author = {Grassmann, S and Stracke, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds exclude novel objects as potential referents
             of novel words based on pragmatics.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {112},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {488-493},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.010},
   Abstract = {Many studies have established that children tend to exclude
             objects for which they already have a name as potential
             referents of novel words. In the current study we asked
             whether this exclusion can be triggered by social-pragmatic
             context alone without pre-existing words as blockers.
             Two-year-old children watched an adult looking at a novel
             object while saying a novel word with excitement. In one
             condition the adult had not seen the object beforehand, and
             so the children interpreted the adult's utterance as
             referring to the gazed-at object. In another condition the
             adult and child had previously played jointly with the
             gazed-at object. In this case, children less often assumed
             that the adult was referring to the object but rather they
             searched for an alternative referent--presumably because
             they inferred that the gazed-at object was old news in their
             common ground with the adult and so not worthy of excited
             labeling. Since this inference based on exclusion is highly
             similar to that underlying the Principle of Contrast/Mutual
             Exclusivity, we propose that this principle is not purely
             lexical but rather is based on children's understanding of
             how and why people direct one another's attention to things
             either with or without language.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.010},
   Key = {fds351786}
}

@article{fds351787,
   Author = {Krachun, C and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Can chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) discriminate appearance
             from reality?},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {112},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {435-450},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.012},
   Abstract = {A milestone in human development is coming to recognize that
             how something looks is not necessarily how it is. We tested
             appearance-reality understanding in chimpanzees (Pan
             troglodytes) with a task requiring them to choose between a
             small grape and a big grape. The apparent relative size of
             the grapes was reversed using magnifying and minimizing
             lenses so that the truly bigger grape appeared to be the
             smaller one. Our Lens test involved a basic component
             adapted from standard procedures for children, as well as
             several components designed to rule out alternative
             explanations. There were large individual differences in
             performance, with some chimpanzees' responses suggesting
             they appreciated the appearance-reality distinction. In
             contrast, all chimpanzees failed a Reverse Contingency
             control test, indicating that those who passed the Lens test
             did not do so by learning a simple reverse contingency rule.
             Four-year-old children given an adapted version of the Lens
             test failed it while 4.5-year-olds passed. Our study
             constitutes the first direct investigation of
             appearance-reality understanding in chimpanzees and the
             first cross-species comparison of this capacity.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.012},
   Key = {fds351787}
}

@article{fds351788,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Kaminski, J},
   Title = {Behavior. Like infant, like dog.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {325},
   Number = {5945},
   Pages = {1213-1214},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1179670},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1179670},
   Key = {fds351788}
}

@article{fds351789,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do great apes use emotional expressions to infer
             desires?},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {688-698},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00802.x},
   Abstract = {Although apes understand others' goals and perceptions,
             little is known about their understanding of others'
             emotional expressions. We conducted three studies following
             the general paradigm of Repacholi and colleagues (1997,
             1998). In Study 1, a human reacted emotionally to the hidden
             contents of two boxes, after which the ape was allowed to
             choose one of the boxes. Apes distinguished between two of
             the expressed emotions (happiness and disgust) by choosing
             appropriately. In Studies 2 and 3, a human reacted either
             positively or negatively to the hidden contents of two
             containers; then the ape saw him eating something. When
             given a choice, apes correctly chose the container to which
             the human had reacted negatively, based on the inference
             that the human had just eaten the food to which he had
             reacted positively - and so the other container still had
             food left in it. These findings suggest that great apes
             understand both the directedness and the valence of some
             human emotional expressions, and can use this understanding
             to infer desires.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00802.x},
   Key = {fds351789}
}

@article{fds351790,
   Author = {Gräfenhain, M and Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of joint commitments.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1430-1443},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016122},
   Abstract = {When adults make a joint commitment to act together, they
             feel an obligation to their partner. In 2 studies, the
             authors investigated whether young children also understand
             joint commitments to act together. In the first study, when
             an adult orchestrated with the child a joint commitment to
             play a game together and then broke off from their joint
             activity, 3-year-olds (n = 24) reacted to the break
             significantly more often (e.g., by trying to re-engage her
             or waiting for her to restart playing) than when she simply
             joined the child's individual activity unbidden.
             Two-year-olds (n = 24) did not differentiate between these 2
             situations. In the second study, 3- and 4-year-old children
             (n = 30 at each age) were enticed away from their activity
             with an adult. Children acknowledged their leaving (e.g., by
             looking to the adult or handing her the object they had been
             playing with) significantly more often when they had made a
             joint commitment to act together than when they had not. By
             3 years of age, children thus recognize both when an adult
             is committed and when they themselves are committed to a
             joint activity.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0016122},
   Key = {fds351790}
}

@article{fds351791,
   Author = {Brandt, S and Kidd, E and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The discourse bases of relativization: An investigation of
             young German and English-speaking children's comprehension
             of relative clauses},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {539-570},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.024},
   Abstract = {In numerous comprehension studies, across different
             languages, children have performed worse on object relatives
             (e.g., the dog that the cat chased) than on subject
             relatives (e.g., the dog that chased the cat). One possible
             reason for this is that the test sentences did not exactly
             match the kinds of object relatives that children typically
             experience. Adults and children usually hear and produce
             object relatives with inanimate heads and pronominal
             subjects (e.g., the car that we bought last year) (cf. Kidd
             et al., Language and Cognitive Processes 22: 860-897, 2007).
             We tested young 3-year old German- and English-speaking
             children with a referential selection task. Children from
             both language groups performed best in the condition where
             the experimenter described inanimate referents with object
             relatives that contained pronominal subjects (e.g., Can you
             give me the sweater that he bought?). Importantly, when the
             object relatives met the constraints identified in spoken
             discourse, children understood them as well as subject
             relatives, or even better. These results speak against a
             purely structural explanation for children's difficulty with
             object relatives as observed in previous studies, but rather
             support the usage-based account, according to which
             discourse function and experience with language shape the
             representation of linguistic structures. © 2009 by Walter
             de Gruyter GmbH.},
   Doi = {10.1515/COGL.2009.024},
   Key = {fds351791}
}

@article{fds351792,
   Author = {Kirjavainen, M and Theakston, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {'I want hold Postman Pat': An investigation into the
             acquisition of infinitival marker 'to'},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {313-339},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723709105312},
   Abstract = {Infinitival-to omission errors (e.g., *I want hold Postman
             Pat) are produced by many English-speaking children early in
             development. This article aims to explain these omissions by
             investigating the emergence of infinitival-to, and its
             production/omission in obligatory contexts. A series of
             corpus analyses were conducted on the naturalistic data from
             one to 13 children between the ages of approximately 2;0 and
             3;1 testing three hypotheses from two theoretical
             viewpoints. The data suggest that the errors are associated
             with different verb sequences (e.g., going-to and going-X)
             and their frequencies in the language to which children are
             exposed. The article concludes that these constructions
             compete for output when children are producing those verbs
             and that this supports the usage-based/constructivist
             account of the omission errors. Copyright © 2009 The
             Author(s).},
   Doi = {10.1177/0142723709105312},
   Key = {fds351792}
}

@article{fds351793,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Lieven, E and Theakston, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist
             and usage-based accounts},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {599-626},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.026},
   Abstract = {This study tests accounts of co-reference errors whereby
             children allow "Mama Bear" and "her" to co-refer in
             sentences like "Mama Bear is washing her" (Chien and Wexler,
             Language Acquisition 1: 225-295, 1990). 63 children aged
             4;6, 5;6 and 6;6 participated in a truth-value judgment task
             augmented with a sentence production component. There were
             three major finding: 1) contrary to predictions of most
             generativist accounts, children accepted co-reference even
             in cases of bound anaphora e.g., "Every girl is washing her"
             2) contrary to Thornton and Wexler (Principle B, VP Ellipsis
             and Interpretation in Child Grammar, The MIT Press, 1999),
             errors did not appear to occur because children understood
             referring expressions to be denoting the same person in
             different guises 3) contrary to usage-based accounts, errors
             were less likely in sentences that contained lower as
             opposed to higher frequency verbs. Error rates also differed
             significantly according to pronoun type ("him", "her",
             "them"). These challenging results are discussed in terms of
             possible processing explanations. © 2009 by Walter de
             Gruyter GmbH.},
   Doi = {10.1515/COGL.2009.026},
   Key = {fds351793}
}

@article{fds351794,
   Author = {Lieven, E and Salomo, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-old children's production of multiword utterances:
             A usage-based analysis},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {481-507},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.022},
   Abstract = {Children generate novel utterances from the outset of
             multiword speech. In this study, we apply a usage-based
             method called 'traceback' to the multiword utterances of
             four two-year-olds to see how closely related these
             utterances are to their previous utterances. Data was
             collected from the age of 2;0 until 6 weeks later on a
             relatively dense sampling schedule. We attempted to match
             each novel multiword utterance in a two-hour corpus to
             lexical strings and schemas that the child had said before.
             Matches were found for between 78-92 percent of all
             multiword utterances. Between 62-91 percent of the slots in
             schemas created by these tracebacks were for referring
             expressions and were filled with nouns or noun phrases. For
             one child, recording continued throughout his third year and
             we compared his data at MLUs matched with the other three
             children to investigate developmental changes. We found
             that, with increasing MLU, and developmentally, children
             were less repetitive within sessions, the tracebacks
             required a wider range of semantic slots and the material
             placed in these slots increased in complexity. © 2009 by
             Walter de Gruyter GmbH.},
   Doi = {10.1515/COGL.2009.022},
   Key = {fds351794}
}

@article{fds351795,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The roots of human altruism.},
   Journal = {British journal of psychology (London, England :
             1953)},
   Volume = {100},
   Number = {Pt 3},
   Pages = {455-471},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712608x379061},
   Abstract = {Human infants as young as 14 to 18 months of age help others
             attain their goals, for example, by helping them to fetch
             out-of-reach objects or opening cabinets for them. They do
             this irrespective of any reward from adults (indeed external
             rewards undermine the tendency), and very likely with no
             concern for such things as reciprocation and reputation,
             which serve to maintain altruism in older children and
             adults. Humans' nearest primate relatives, chimpanzees, also
             help others instrumentally without concrete rewards. These
             results suggest that human infants are naturally altruistic,
             and as ontogeny proceeds and they must deal more
             independently with a wider range of social contexts,
             socialization and feedback from social interactions with
             others become important mediators of these initial
             altruistic tendencies.},
   Doi = {10.1348/000712608x379061},
   Key = {fds351795}
}

@article{fds351796,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative
             culture.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {364},
   Number = {1528},
   Pages = {2405-2415},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0052},
   Abstract = {Some researchers have claimed that chimpanzee and human
             culture rest on homologous cognitive and learning
             mechanisms. While clearly there are some homologous
             mechanisms, we argue here that there are some different
             mechanisms at work as well. Chimpanzee cultural traditions
             represent behavioural biases of different populations, all
             within the species' existing cognitive repertoire (what we
             call the 'zone of latent solutions') that are generated by
             founder effects, individual learning and mostly
             product-oriented (rather than process-oriented) copying.
             Human culture, in contrast, has the distinctive
             characteristic that it accumulates modifications over time
             (what we call the 'ratchet effect'). This difference results
             from the facts that (i) human social learning is more
             oriented towards process than product and (ii) unique forms
             of human cooperation lead to active teaching, social
             motivations for conformity and normative sanctions against
             non-conformity. Together, these unique processes of social
             learning and cooperation lead to humans' unique form of
             cumulative cultural evolution.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2009.0052},
   Key = {fds351796}
}

@article{fds351797,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding
             in an active helping paradigm.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {112},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {337-342},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006},
   Abstract = {Recently, several studies have claimed that soon after their
             first birthday infants understand others' false beliefs.
             However, some have questioned these findings based on
             criticisms of the looking-time paradigms used. Here we
             report a new paradigm to test false belief understanding in
             infants using a more active behavioral response: helping.
             Specifically, the task was for infants to help an adult
             achieve his goal - but to determine that goal infants had to
             take into account what the adult believed (i.e., whether or
             not he falsely believed there was a toy inside a box).
             Results showed that by 18 months of age infants successfully
             took into account the adult's belief in the process of
             attempting to determine his goal. Results for 16-month-olds
             were in the same direction but less clear. These results
             represent by far the youngest age of false belief
             understanding in a task with an active behavioral
             measure.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006},
   Key = {fds351797}
}

@article{fds351798,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's
             perspective},
   Journal = {Behaviour},
   Volume = {146},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {979-998},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853908X395530},
   Abstract = {We investigated dogs' ability to take the visual perspective
             of humans. In the main study, each of two toys was placed on
             the dog's side of two small barriers (one opaque, one
             transparent). In experimental conditions, a human sat on the
             opposite side of the barriers, such that she could see only
             the toy behind the transparent barrier. The experimenter
             then told the dog to 'Bring it here!' (without designating
             either toy in any way). In the Back Turned control E also
             sat on the opposite side but with her back turned so that
             she could see neither toy, and in the Same Side control she
             sat on the same side as the dog such that she could see both
             toys. When toys were differentiable dogs approached the toy
             behind the transparent barrier in experimental as compared
             to back turned and same side condition. Dogs did not
             differentiate between the two control conditions. In a
             second study dogs were not sensitive to what a human had or
             had not seen in the immediate past. These results suggest
             that, even in the absence of overt behavioural cues, dogs
             are sensitive to others visual access, even if that differs
             from their own. © 2009 BRILL.},
   Doi = {10.1163/156853908X395530},
   Key = {fds351798}
}

@article{fds351799,
   Author = {Krachun, C and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and
             apes.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {521-535},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00793.x},
   Abstract = {A nonverbal false belief task was administered to children
             (mean age 5 years) and two great ape species: chimpanzees
             (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Because apes
             typically perform poorly in cooperative contexts, our task
             was competitive. Two versions were run: in both, a human
             competitor witnessed an experimenter hide a reward in one of
             two containers. When the competitor then left the room
             (version A) or turned around (version B), the experimenter
             switched the locations of the containers. The competitor
             returned and reached with effort, but unsuccessfully,
             towards the incorrect container. Children displayed an
             understanding of the competitor's false belief by correctly
             choosing the other container to find the reward. Apes did
             not. However, in version A (but not version B), apes looked
             more often at the unchosen container in false belief trials
             than in true belief control trials, possibly indicating some
             implicit or uncertain understanding that needs to be
             investigated further.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00793.x},
   Key = {fds351799}
}

@article{fds351800,
   Author = {Matsui, T and Rakoczy, H and Miura, Y and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Understanding of speaker certainty and false-belief
             reasoning: a comparison of Japanese and German
             preschoolers.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {602-613},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00812.x},
   Abstract = {It has been repeatedly shown that when asked to identify a
             protagonist's false belief on the basis of his false
             statement, English-speaking 3-year-olds dismiss the
             statement and fail to attribute to him a false belief. In
             the present studies, we tested 3-year-old Japanese children
             in a similar task, using false statements accompanied by
             grammaticalized particles of speaker (un)certainty, as in
             everyday Japanese utterances. The Japanese children were
             directly compared with same-aged German children, whose
             native language does not have grammaticalized epistemic
             concepts. Japanese children profited from the explicit
             statement of the protagonist's false belief when it was
             marked with the attitude of certainty in a way that German
             children did not - presumably because Japanese but not
             German children must process such marking routinely in their
             daily discourse. These results are discussed in the broader
             context of linguistic and theory of mind
             development.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00812.x},
   Key = {fds351800}
}

@article{fds351801,
   Author = {Wyman, E and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children understand multiple pretend identities in
             their object play.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {Pt 2},
   Pages = {385-404},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151008x322893},
   Abstract = {This set of studies examined the ability of 3-year-olds to
             conceptualize multiple pretend identities with objects.
             Rather than relying on verbal response measures, as has been
             done in the past, children's creative and inferential
             pretend actions were used as indicators of their
             understanding. The common structure to all four studies was
             that children were confronted with one pretend scenario,
             moved to a second pretend scenario and then back again to
             the first. Children proficiently tailored their pretence to
             an object whose pretend identity changed between scenarios
             despite being less able to name each identity. Thus, using
             an inferential action methodology, these studies provide
             early and particularly convincing evidence that children can
             track the multiple pretend identities of
             objects.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151008x322893},
   Key = {fds351801}
}

@article{fds351802,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Brosche, N and Warneken, F and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young children's understanding of the context-relativity of
             normative rules in conventional games.},
   Journal = {The British journal of developmental psychology},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {Pt 2},
   Pages = {445-456},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151008x337752},
   Abstract = {We investigated young children's awareness of the
             context-relative rule structure of simple games. Two
             contexts were established in the form of spatial locations.
             Familiar objects were used in their conventional way at
             location 1, but acquired specific functions in a rule game
             at location 2. A third party then performed the conventional
             act at either of the two locations, constituting a mistake
             at location 2 (experimental condition), but appropriate at
             location 1 (control condition). Three-year-olds (but not
             2-year-olds) systematically distinguished the two
             conditions, spontaneously intervening with normative protest
             against the third party act in the experimental, but not in
             the control condition. Young children thus understand
             context-specific rules even when the context marking is
             non-linguistic. These results are discussed in the broader
             context of the development of social cognition and cultural
             learning.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151008x337752},
   Key = {fds351802}
}

@article{fds351803,
   Author = {Chan, A and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children's understanding of the agent-patient relations in
             the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons
             between Cantonese, German, and English},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {267-300},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.015},
   Abstract = {Cantonese-, German-, and English-speaking children aged 2;6,
             3,6, and 4,6 acted out transitive sentences containing novel
             verbs in three conditions: (1) agent and patient were cued
             redundantly by both word order and animacy; (2) agent and
             patient were marked only with word order; and (3) agent and
             patient were cued in conflicting ways with word order and
             animacy. All three age groups in all three languages
             comprehended the redundantly cued sentences. When word order
             was the only cue, English children showed the earliest
             comprehension at 2;6, then German, and then Cantonese
             children at 3;6. When the cues conflicted, none of the 2;6
             children in any language comprehended in adult-like ways,
             whereas all of the children at 3;6 and 4;6 preferred word
             order over animacy (but with some cross-linguistic
             differences in performance as well). When animacy contrast
             changed across sentence types, Cantonese children
             comprehended the sentences differently at all three age
             levels, German children did so at the two younger ages, and
             English children only at the youngest age. The findings
             correspond well with the informativeness of word order in
             the three languages, suggesting that children's learning of
             the syntactic marking of agent-patient relations is strongly
             influenced by nature of the language they hear around them.
             © 2009 by Walter de Gruyter GmbH.},
   Doi = {10.1515/COGL.2009.015},
   Key = {fds351803}
}

@article{fds351804,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Schäfer, M and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate
             about absent entities.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {654-660},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02346.x},
   Abstract = {One of the defining features of human language is
             displacement, the ability to make reference to absent
             entities. Here we show that prelinguistic, 12-month-old
             infants already can use a nonverbal pointing gesture to make
             reference to absent entities. We also show that
             chimpanzees-who can point for things they want humans to
             give them-do not point to refer to absent entities in the
             same way. These results demonstrate that the ability to
             communicate about absent but mutually known entities depends
             not on language, but rather on deeper social-cognitive
             skills that make acts of linguistic reference possible in
             the first place. These nonlinguistic skills for displaced
             reference emerged apparently only after humans' divergence
             from great apes some 6 million years ago.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02346.x},
   Key = {fds351804}
}

@article{fds351805,
   Author = {Wyman, E and Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Normativity and context in young children's pretend
             play},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {146-155},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.01.003},
   Abstract = {In two studies 3-year-olds' understanding of the
             context-specificity of normative rules was investigated
             through games of pretend play. In the first study, children
             protested against a character who joined a pretend game but
             treated the target object according to its real function.
             However, they did not protest when she performed the same
             action without having first joined the game. In the second
             study, children protested when the character mixed up an
             object's pretend identities between two different pretend
             games. However, they did not protest when she performed the
             same pretend action in its correct game context. Thus, the
             studies show that young children see the pretence-reality
             distinction, and the distinction between different pretence
             identities, as normative. More generally, the results of
             these studies demonstrate young children's ability to
             enforce normative rules in their pretence and to do so
             context-specifically. © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.01.003},
   Key = {fds351805}
}

@article{fds351806,
   Author = {Vaish, A and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its
             relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {534-543},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014322},
   Abstract = {In most research on the early ontogeny of sympathy, young
             children are presented with an overtly distressed person and
             their responses are observed. In the current study, the
             authors asked whether young children could also sympathize
             with a person to whom something negative had happened but
             who was expressing no emotion at all. They showed 18- and
             25-month-olds an adult either harming another adult by
             destroying or taking away her possessions (harm condition)
             or else doing something similar that did not harm her
             (neutral condition). The "victim" expressed no emotions in
             either condition. Nevertheless, in the harm as compared with
             the neutral condition, children showed more concern and
             subsequent prosocial behavior toward the victim. Moreover,
             children's concerned looks during the harmful event were
             positively correlated with their subsequent prosocial
             behavior. Very young children can sympathize with a victim
             even in the absence of overt emotional signals, possibly by
             some form of affective perspective taking.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0014322},
   Key = {fds351806}
}

@article{fds351807,
   Author = {Colombi, C and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M and Young, G and Warneken, F and Rogers, SJ},
   Title = {Examining correlates of cooperation in autism: Imitation,
             joint attention, and understanding intentions.},
   Journal = {Autism : the international journal of research and
             practice},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {143-163},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362361308098514},
   Abstract = {The goal of the current study was to examine the
             contribution of three early social skills that may provide a
             foundation for cooperative performance in autism: (1)
             imitation, (2) joint attention, and (3) understanding of
             other people's intentions regarding actions on objects.
             Fourteen children with autistic disorder (AD) and 15
             children with other developmental disabilities (DDs) matched
             on non-verbal developmental age (AD, mean 27.7, SD 9.8; DD,
             mean 33.4, SD 11.1) and verbal developmental age (AD, mean
             21.5, SD 12.3; DD, mean 28.4, SD 11.0) participated in the
             study. Children with autism showed poorer performance on
             imitation and joint attention measures, but not on the
             intentionality task. Multiple regression analyses showed
             that imitation skills and joint attention contributed
             independently to cooperation, above and beyond the
             understanding of intentions of actions on
             objects.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1362361308098514},
   Key = {fds351807}
}

@article{fds351808,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing
             gestures.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {264-271},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00758.x},
   Abstract = {We investigated whether 1-year-old infants use their shared
             experience with an adult to determine the meaning of a
             pointing gesture. In the first study, after two adults had
             each shared a different activity with the infant, one of the
             adults pointed to a target object. Eighteen- but not
             14-month-olds responded appropriately to the pointing
             gesture based on the particular activity they had previously
             shared with that particular adult. In the second study,
             14-month-olds were successful in a simpler procedure in
             which the pointing adult either had or had not shared a
             relevant activity with the infant prior to the pointing.
             Infants just beginning to learn language thus already show a
             complex understanding of the pragmatics of cooperative
             communication in which shared experience with particular
             individuals plays a crucial role.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00758.x},
   Key = {fds351808}
}

@article{fds351809,
   Author = {Kirschner, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization
             in preschool children.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental child psychology},
   Volume = {102},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {299-314},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.005},
   Abstract = {The human capacity to synchronize body movements to an
             external acoustic beat enables uniquely human behaviors such
             as music making and dancing. By hypothesis, these first
             evolved in human cultures as fundamentally social
             activities. We therefore hypothesized that children would
             spontaneously synchronize their body movements to an
             external beat at earlier ages and with higher accuracy if
             the stimulus was presented in a social context. A total of
             36 children in three age groups (2.5, 3.5, and 4.5 years)
             were invited to drum along with either a human partner, a
             drumming machine, or a drum sound coming from a speaker.
             When drumming with a social partner, children as young as
             2.5 years adjusted their drumming tempo to a beat outside
             the range of their spontaneous motor tempo. Moreover,
             children of all ages synchronized their drumming with higher
             accuracy in the social condition. We argue that drumming
             together with a social partner creates a shared
             representation of the joint action task and/or elicits a
             specific human motivation to synchronize movements during
             joint rhythmic activity.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.005},
   Key = {fds351809}
}

@article{fds351810,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange
             paradigm.},
   Journal = {American journal of primatology},
   Volume = {71},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {175-181},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20639},
   Abstract = {Recent studies have produced mixed evidence about inequity
             aversion in nonhuman primates. Brosnan et al. [Proceedings
             of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological
             Sciences 272:253-258, 2005] found inequity aversion in
             chimpanzees and argued that effort is crucial, if subjects
             are to evaluate how they are rewarded in comparison to a
             competitor for an identical performance. In this study we
             investigated inequity aversion with chimpanzees, bonobos and
             orangutans, using the method of Brosnan et al. [Proceedings
             of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological
             Sciences 272:253-258, 2005] after introducing some
             methodological improvements. Subjects always received a
             less-preferred food in exchange for a token, whereas the
             competitor received either the same type of food for their
             token (equity) or a more favored food for it (inequity).
             Apes did not refuse more of the less-preferred food when a
             competitor had received the more favored food. Thus, with an
             improved methodology we failed to reproduce the findings of
             Brosnan et al. [Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B. Biological Sciences 272:253-258, 2005] that apes
             show inequity aversion.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ajp.20639},
   Key = {fds351810}
}

@article{fds351812,
   Author = {Gräfenhain, M and Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {One-year-olds' understanding of nonverbal gestures directed
             to a third person},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {23-33},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.10.001},
   Abstract = {We investigated whether infants comprehend others' nonverbal
             communicative intentions directed to a third person, in an
             'overhearing' context. An experimenter addressed an
             assistant and indicated a hidden toy's location by either
             gazing ostensively or pointing to the location for her. In a
             matched control condition, the experimenter performed
             similar behaviors (absent-minded gazing and extended index
             finger) but did not communicate ostensively with the
             assistant. Infants could then search for the toy.
             Eighteen-month-old infants were skillful in using both
             communicative cues to find the hidden object, whereas
             14-month-olds performed above chance only with the pointing
             cue. Neither age group performed above chance in the control
             condition. This study thus shows that by 14-18 months of
             age, infants are beginning to monitor and comprehend some
             aspects of third party interactions. © 2008 Elsevier Inc.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.10.001},
   Key = {fds351812}
}

@article{fds351813,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's selective learning of rule games from
             reliable and unreliable models},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {61-69},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.004},
   Abstract = {We investigated preschoolers' selective learning from models
             that had previously appeared to be reliable or unreliable.
             Replicating previous research, children from 4 years
             selectively learned novel words from reliable over
             unreliable speakers. Extending previous research, children
             also selectively learned other kinds of acts - novel games -
             from reliable actors. More important, - and novel to this
             study, this selective learning was not just based on a
             preference for one model or one kind of act, but had a
             normative dimension to it. Children understood the way a
             reliable actor demonstrated an act not only as the better
             one, but as the normatively appropriate or correct one, as
             indicated in both their explicit verbal comments and their
             spontaneous normative interventions (e.g., protest,
             critique) in response to third-party acts deviating from the
             one demonstrated. These findings are discussed in the
             broader context of the development of children's social
             cognition and cultural learning. © 2008 Elsevier Inc. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.004},
   Key = {fds351813}
}

@article{fds351814,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Infants appreciate the social intention behind a pointing
             gesture: Commentary on "Children's understanding of
             communicative intentions in the middle of the second year of
             life" by T. Aureli, P. Perucchini and M.
             Genco},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {13-15},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.09.004},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.09.004},
   Key = {fds351814}
}

@article{fds351815,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Brandt, S},
   Title = {Flexibility in the semantics and syntax of children's early
             verb use.},
   Journal = {Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
             Development},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {113-126},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00523.x},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00523.x},
   Key = {fds351815}
}

@article{fds351816,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Collective intentionality and cultural development},
   Journal = {Deutsche Zeitschrift fur Philosophie},
   Volume = {56},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {401-410},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1524/dzph.2008.0031},
   Doi = {10.1524/dzph.2008.0031},
   Key = {fds351816}
}

@article{fds351817,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they
             believe.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {109},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {224-234},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.010},
   Abstract = {There is currently much controversy about which, if any,
             mental states chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates
             understand. In the current two studies we tested both
             chimpanzees' and human children's understanding of both
             knowledge-ignorance and false belief - in the same
             experimental paradigm involving competition with a
             conspecific. We found that whereas 6-year-old children
             understood both of these mental states, chimpanzees
             understood knowledge-ignorance but not false belief. After
             ruling out various alternative explanations of these and
             related findings, we conclude that in at least some
             situations chimpanzees know what others know. Possible
             explanations for their failure in the highly similar false
             belief task are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.010},
   Key = {fds351817}
}

@article{fds351818,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in
             20-month-olds.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1785-1788},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013860},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated the influence of rewards on
             very young children's helping behavior. After 20-month-old
             infants received a material reward during a treatment phase,
             they subsequently were less likely to engage in further
             helping during a test phase as compared with infants who had
             previously received social praise or no reward at all. This
             so-called overjustification effect suggests that even the
             earliest helping behaviors of young children are
             intrinsically motivated and that socialization practices
             involving extrinsic rewards can undermine this
             tendency.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0013860},
   Key = {fds351818}
}

@article{fds351819,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Assessing the validity of ape-human comparisons: a reply to
             Boesch (2007).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {122},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {449-452},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.122.4.449},
   Abstract = {Boesch (2007) criticizes research comparing ape and human
             cognition on the basis of both internal and external
             validity. The authors show here that most of those
             criticisms are not valid because: (i) most threats to
             internal validity (e.g., conspecific experimenters for
             humans but not apes) are controlled for experimentally; (ii)
             externally, there is no empirical evidence that captive apes
             have fewer cognitive skills than wild apes and indeed some
             evidence (especially from human-raised apes) that they have
             more; and (iii) externally, there is no empirical evidence
             that Western middle-class children have different cognitive
             skills from other children at very early ages in basic
             cognitive domains. Although difficult, with appropriate
             methodological care, experimental cross-species comparisons
             may be validly made.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.122.4.449},
   Key = {fds351819}
}

@article{fds351820,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately
             for knowledgeable and ignorant partners.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {108},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {732-739},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.013},
   Abstract = {In the current study we investigated whether 12-month-old
             infants gesture appropriately for knowledgeable versus
             ignorant partners, in order to provide them with needed
             information. In two experiments we found that in response to
             a searching adult, 12-month-olds pointed more often to an
             object whose location the adult did not know and thus needed
             information to find (she had not seen it fall down just
             previously) than to an object whose location she knew and
             thus did not need information to find (she had watched it
             fall down just previously). These results demonstrate that,
             in contrast to classic views of infant communication,
             infants' early pointing at 12 months is already premised on
             an understanding of others' knowledge and ignorance, along
             with a prosocial motive to help others by providing needed
             information.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.013},
   Key = {fds351820}
}

@article{fds351822,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Warneken, F},
   Title = {Human behaviour: Share and share alike.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {454},
   Number = {7208},
   Pages = {1057-1058},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/4541057a},
   Doi = {10.1038/4541057a},
   Key = {fds351822}
}

@article{fds351823,
   Author = {Dabrowska, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Rapid learning of an abstract language-specific category:
             Polish children's acquisition of the instrumental
             construction.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {533-558},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000908008660},
   Abstract = {Rapid acquisition of linguistic categories or constructions
             is sometimes regarded as evidence of innate knowledge. In
             this paper, we examine Polish children's early understanding
             of an idiosyncratic, language-specific construction
             involving the instrumental case - which could not be due to
             innate knowledge. Thirty Polish-speaking children aged 2 ; 6
             and 3 ; 2 participated in a elicited production experiment
             with novel verbs that were demonstrated as taking nouns in
             the instrumental case as patients. Children heard the verbs
             in sentences with either masculine or feminine nouns (which
             take different endings in the instrumental case), and were
             tested with new nouns of the same and of the opposite
             gender. In both age groups, a substantial majority of
             children succeeded in generalizing from one gendered form of
             the instrumental case to the other (especially to the
             masculine), thus indicating that they have some kind of
             abstract understanding of the instrumental case in this
             construction. This relatively early abstract knowledge of an
             idiosyncratic construction casts doubt on the view that
             early acquisition requires innate linguistic
             knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000908008660},
   Key = {fds351823}
}

@article{fds351824,
   Author = {Dittmar, M and Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Young German children's early syntactic competence: a
             preferential looking study.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {575-582},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00703.x},
   Abstract = {Using a preferential looking methodology with novel verbs,
             Gertner, Fisher and Eisengart (2006) found that 21-month-old
             English children seemed to understand the syntactic marking
             of transitive word order in an abstract, verb-general way.
             In the current study we tested whether young German children
             of this same age have this same understanding. Following
             Gertner et al. (2006), one group of German children was
             tested only after they had received a training/practice
             phase containing transitive sentences with familiar verbs
             and the exact same nouns as those used at test. A second
             group was tested after a training/practice phase consisting
             only of familiar verbs, without the nouns used at test. Only
             the group of children with the training on full transitive
             sentences was successful in the test. These findings suggest
             that for children this young to succeed in this test of
             syntactic understanding, they must first have some kind of
             relevant linguistic experience immediately prior to
             testing--which raises the question of the nature of
             children's linguistic representations at this early point in
             development.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00703.x},
   Key = {fds351824}
}

@article{fds351825,
   Author = {Dittmar, M and Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {German children's comprehension of word order and case
             marking in causative sentences.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {79},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1152-1167},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01181.x},
   Abstract = {Two comprehension experiments were conducted to investigate
             whether German children are able to use the grammatical cues
             of word order and word endings (case markers) to identify
             agents and patients in a causative sentence and whether they
             weigh these two cues differently across development.
             Two-year-olds correctly understood only sentences with both
             cues supporting each other--the prototypical form.
             Five-year-olds were able to use word order by itself but not
             case markers. Only 7-year-olds behaved like adults by
             relying on case markers over word order when the two cues
             conflicted. These findings suggest that prototypical
             instances of linguistic constructions with redundant
             grammatical marking play a special role in early
             acquisition, and only later do children isolate and weigh
             individual grammatical cues appropriately.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01181.x},
   Key = {fds351825}
}

@article{fds351826,
   Author = {Chang, F and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Automatic evaluation of syntactic learners in
             typologically-different languages},
   Journal = {Cognitive Systems Research},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {198-213},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.10.002},
   Abstract = {Human syntax acquisition involves a system that can learn
             constraints on possible word sequences in
             typologically-different human languages. Evaluation of
             computational syntax acquisition systems typically involves
             theory-specific or language-specific assumptions that make
             it difficult to compare results in multiple languages. To
             address this problem, a bag-of-words incremental generation
             (BIG) task with an automatic sentence prediction accuracy
             (SPA) evaluation measure was developed. The BIG-SPA task was
             used to test several learners that incorporated n-gram
             statistics which are commonly found in statistical
             approaches to syntax acquisition. In addition, a novel
             Adjacency-Prominence learner, that was based on
             psycholinguistic work in sentence production and syntax
             acquisition, was also tested and it was found that this
             learner yielded the best results in this task on these
             languages. In general, the BIG-SPA task is argued to be a
             useful platform for comparing explicit theories of syntax
             acquisition in multiple languages. © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.10.002},
   Key = {fds351826}
}

@article{fds351827,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Hedwig, D and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {An experimental study of nettle feeding in captive
             gorillas.},
   Journal = {American journal of primatology},
   Volume = {70},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {584-593},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20532},
   Abstract = {Mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) in Karisoke,
             Rwanda, feed on the stinging nettle Laportea alatipes by
             means of elaborate processing skills. Byrne [e.g.
             Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London,
             Series B, Biological Sciences 358:529-536, 2003] has claimed
             that individuals acquire these skills by means of the
             so-called program-level imitation, in which the overall
             sequence of problem-solving steps (not the precise actions)
             is reproduced. In this study we present western lowland
             gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) with highly similar
             nettles. Twelve gorillas in three different groups
             (including also one nettle-naïve gorilla) used the same
             program-level technique as wild mountain gorillas (with
             differences mainly on the action level). Chimpanzees,
             orangutans, and bonobos did not show these program-level
             patterns, nor did the gorillas when presented with a plant
             similar in structural design but lacking stinging defenses.
             We conclude that although certain aspects (i.e. single
             actions) of this complex skill may be owing to social
             learning, at the program level gorilla nettle feeding
             derives mostly from genetic predispositions and individual
             learning of plant affordances.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ajp.20532},
   Key = {fds351827}
}

@article{fds351828,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great
             apes.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {79},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {609-626},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01146.x},
   Abstract = {G. Gergely, H. Bekkering, and I. Király (2002) showed that
             14-month-old infants imitate rationally, copying an adult's
             unusual action more often when it was freely chosen than
             when it was forced by some constraint. This suggests that
             infants understand others' intentions as rational choices of
             action plans. It is important to test whether apes also
             understand others' intentions in this way. In each of the
             current 3 studies, a comparison group of 14-month-olds used
             a tool more often when a demonstrator freely chose to use it
             than when she had to use it, but apes generally used the
             tool equally often in both conditions (orangutans were an
             exception). Only some apes thus show an understanding of
             others' intentions as rational choices of action
             plans.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01146.x},
   Key = {fds351828}
}

@article{fds351829,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years
             later.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {187-192},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010},
   Abstract = {On the 30th anniversary of Premack and Woodruff's seminal
             paper asking whether chimpanzees have a theory of mind, we
             review recent evidence that suggests in many respects they
             do, whereas in other respects they might not. Specifically,
             there is solid evidence from several different experimental
             paradigms that chimpanzees understand the goals and
             intentions of others, as well as the perception and
             knowledge of others. Nevertheless, despite several seemingly
             valid attempts, there is currently no evidence that
             chimpanzees understand false beliefs. Our conclusion for the
             moment is, thus, that chimpanzees understand others in terms
             of a perception-goal psychology, as opposed to a
             full-fledged, human-like belief-desire psychology.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010},
   Key = {fds351829}
}

@article{fds351830,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The sources of normativity: young children's awareness of
             the normative structure of games.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {875-881},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.875},
   Abstract = {In two studies, the authors investigated 2- and 3-year-old
             children's awareness of the normative structure of
             conventional games. In the target conditions, an
             experimenter showed a child how to play a simple rule game.
             After the child and the experimenter had played for a while,
             a puppet came (controlled by a 2nd experimenter), asked to
             join in, and then performed an action that constituted a
             mistake in the game. In control conditions, the puppet
             performed the exact same action as in the experimental
             conditions, but the context was different such that this act
             did not constitute a mistake. Children's normative responses
             to the puppet's acts (e.g., protest, critique, or teaching)
             were scored. Both age groups performed more normative
             responses in the target than in the control conditions, but
             the 3-year-olds did so on a more explicit level. These
             studies demonstrate in a particularly strong way that even
             very young children have some grasp of the normative
             structure of conventional activities.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.875},
   Key = {fds351830}
}

@article{fds351831,
   Author = {Brandt, S and Diessel, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The acquisition of German relative clauses: a case
             study.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {325-348},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000907008379},
   Abstract = {This paper investigates the development of relative clauses
             in the speech of one German-speaking child aged 2 ; 0 to 5 ;
             0. The earliest relative clauses we found in the data occur
             in topicalization constructions that are only a little
             different from simple sentences: they contain a single
             proposition, express the actor prior to other participants,
             assert new information and often occur with main-clause word
             order. In the course of the development, more complex
             relative constructions emerge, in which the relative clause
             is embedded in a fully-fledged main clause. We argue that
             German relative clauses develop in an incremental fashion
             from simple non-embedded sentences that gradually evolve
             into complex sentence constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000907008379},
   Key = {fds351831}
}

@article{fds351832,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Albrecht, K and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Infants' visual and auditory communication when a partner is
             or is not visually attending.},
   Journal = {Infant behavior & development},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {157-167},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.011},
   Abstract = {In the current study we investigated infants' communication
             in the visual and auditory modalities as a function of the
             recipient's visual attention. We elicited pointing at
             interesting events from thirty-two 12-month olds and
             thirty-two 18-month olds in two conditions: when the
             recipient either was or was not visually attending to them
             before and during the point. The main result was that
             infants initiated more pointing when the recipient's visual
             attention was on them than when it was not. In addition,
             when the recipient did not respond by sharing interest in
             the designated event, infants initiated more repairs
             (repeated pointing) than when she did, again, especially
             when the recipient was visually attending to them.
             Interestingly, accompanying vocalizations were used
             intentionally and increased in both experimental conditions
             when the recipient did not share attention and interest.
             However, there was little evidence that infants used their
             vocalizations to direct attention to their gestures when the
             recipient was not attending to them.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.011},
   Key = {fds351832}
}

@article{fds351833,
   Author = {Riedel, J and Schumann, K and Kaminski, J and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The early ontogeny of human-dog communication},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {1003-1014},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.010},
   Abstract = {Although dogs, Canis familiaris, are skilful at responding
             to human social cues, the role of ontogeny in the
             development of these abilities has not been systematically
             examined. We studied the ability of very young dog puppies
             to follow human communicative cues and successfully find
             hidden food. In the first experiment we compared 6-, 8-, 16-
             and 24-week-old puppies in their ability to use pointing
             gestures or a marker as a cue. The results showed that
             puppies, independent of age, could use all human
             communicative cues provided; only their success at using the
             marker cue increased with age. In the second and third
             experiments we investigated the flexibility of the puppies'
             understanding by reducing the degree to which they could use
             local enhancement to solve these problems. Here, subjects
             could not simply approach the hand of the experimenter and
             follow its direction to the correct location because cups
             were placed next to the dog instead of next to the
             experimenter. Six-week-old puppies readily used all of the
             human communicative cues provided. These findings support
             the hypothesis that domestication played a critical role in
             shaping the ability of dogs to follow human-given cues. ©
             2007 The Association for the Study of Animal
             Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.010},
   Key = {fds351833}
}

@article{fds320803,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Call, J and Hernández-Lloreda, MV and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Response [3]},
   Journal = {Science},
   Volume = {319},
   Number = {5863},
   Pages = {569},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   Key = {fds320803}
}

@article{fds320804,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Call, J and Hernandez-Lloreda, MV and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Comparing social skills of children and apes -
             Response},
   Journal = {SCIENCE},
   Volume = {319},
   Number = {5863},
   Pages = {570-570},
   Publisher = {AMER ASSOC ADVANCEMENT SCIENCE},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   Key = {fds320804}
}

@article{fds351836,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Colombi, C and Rogers, SJ and Warneken, F and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Helping and cooperation in children with
             autism.},
   Journal = {Journal of autism and developmental disorders},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {224-238},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0381-5},
   Abstract = {Helping and cooperation are central to human social life.
             Here, we report two studies investigating these social
             behaviors in children with autism and children with
             developmental delay. In the first study, both groups of
             children helped the experimenter attain her goals. In the
             second study, both groups of children cooperated with an
             adult, but fewer children with autism performed the tasks
             successfully. When the adult stopped interacting at a
             certain moment, children with autism produced fewer attempts
             to re-engage her, possibly indicating that they had not
             formed a shared goal/shared intentions with her. These
             results are discussed in terms of the prerequisite cognitive
             and motivational skills and propensities underlying social
             behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10803-007-0381-5},
   Key = {fds351836}
}

@article{fds351837,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Response [6]},
   Journal = {Science},
   Volume = {319},
   Number = {5861},
   Pages = {284},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   Key = {fds351837}
}

@article{fds320802,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours?},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {76},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {951-962},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.014},
   Abstract = {Reciprocal interactions observed in animals may persist
             because individuals keep careful account of services
             exchanged with each group member. To test whether
             chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, possess the cognitive skills
             required for this type of contingency-based reciprocity, we
             gave chimpanzees the choice of cooperating with a
             conspecific who had helped them previously or one who had
             not helped them in two different experimental tasks. In the
             first experiment, one of the partners preferentially
             recruited the subjects to cooperate in a mutualistic task,
             while the other potential partner never chose to cooperate
             with the subject, but rather chose a different partner. In
             the second experiment, one of the partners altruistically
             helped the subjects to reach food, while the other partner
             never helped the subject, but rather took the food himself.
             In both experiments there was some evidence that the
             chimpanzees increased the amount they cooperated with or
             helped the partner who had been more helpful towards them
             compared to their baseline behaviour towards the same
             individual (or in a control condition). However, in both
             experiments this effect was relatively weak and subjects did
             not preferentially favour the individual who had favoured
             them over the one who had not in either experiment. Although
             taken together, these experiments provide some support for
             the hypothesis that chimpanzees are capable of contingent
             reciprocity, they also suggest that models of immediate
             reciprocation and detailed accounts of recent exchanges
             (e.g. Tit for Tat) may not play a large role in guiding the
             social decisions of chimpanzees. © 2008 The Association for
             the Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.014},
   Key = {fds320802}
}

@article{fds351838,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Grammar},
   Volume = {1-3},
   Pages = {38-50},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development,
             Three-Volume Set},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780123704603},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012370877-9.00073-6},
   Abstract = {To acquire competence with a natural language, young
             children must master the grammatical constructions of their
             language(s). In this article we outline the main theoretical
             issues in the field and trace the developmental path
             children follow from talking in single-unit 'holophrases' to
             using complex, abstract constructions. We describe the
             development of children's initial skills with word order,
             case marking, and morphology as abstract elements in early
             constructions, and we discuss the level of abstraction
             characteristic of young children's grammatical constructions
             at different stages of development and in some different
             languages of the world. Finally, we consider the learning
             processes that enable young children both to acquire and to
             abstract across grammatical constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-012370877-9.00073-6},
   Key = {fds351838}
}

@article{fds351840,
   Author = {Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Graded representations in the acquisition of English and
             German transitive constructions},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {48-66},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.11.002},
   Abstract = {English and German children aged 2 years 4 months and 4
             years heard both novel and familiar verbs in sentences whose
             form was grammatical, but which mismatched the event they
             were watching (e.g., 'The frog is pushing the lion', when
             the lion was actually the 'agent' or 'doer' of the pushing).
             These verbs were then elicited in new sentences. All
             children mostly corrected the familiar verb (i.e., they used
             the agent as the grammatical subject), but there were
             cross-linguistic differences among the two-year-olds
             concerning the novel verb. When English 2-year-olds used the
             novel verb they mostly corrected. However, their most
             frequent response was to avoid using the novel verb
             altogether. German 2-year-olds corrected the novel verb
             significantly more often than their English counterparts,
             demonstrating more robust verb-general representations of
             agent- and patient-marking. These findings provide support
             for a 'graded representations' view of development, which
             proposes that grammatical representations may be
             simultaneously abstract but 'weak'. © 2007 Elsevier Inc.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.11.002},
   Key = {fds351840}
}

@article{fds351841,
   Author = {Moll, H and Richter, N and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Fourteen-month-olds know what "we" have shared in a special
             way},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {90-101},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15250000701779402},
   Abstract = {People often express excitement to each other when
             encountering an object that they have shared together
             previously in some special way. This study investigated
             whether 14-month-old infants know precisely what they have
             and have not shared in a special way (and with whom). In the
             experimental condition an adult and infant shared an object
             (the target) excitedly because it unexpectedly reappeared in
             several places. They then shared 2 other objects (the
             distractors) in a more normal fashion. Later, the adult
             reacted excitedly to a tray containing all 3 objects and
             then made an ambiguous request for the infant to hand "it"
             to her. There were 2 control conditions. In 1 of them, a
             different adult, who knew none of the 3 objects, made the
             ambiguous request. In the other control condition, the adult
             who made the request had previously experienced the objects
             only alone, while the infant looked on unengaged. Infants in
             the experimental condition chose the target object more
             often than the distractors and more often than they chose it
             in either control condition. These results demonstrate that
             14-month-old infants can identify which one of a set of
             objects "we" - and not just I or you alone - have had a
             special experience with in the past.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15250000701779402},
   Key = {fds351841}
}

@article{fds351842,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Behavioral cues that great apes use to forage for hidden
             food.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {117-128},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0095-2},
   Abstract = {We conducted three studies to examine whether the four great
             ape species (chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas, and orangutans)
             are able to use behavioral experimenter-given cues in an
             object-choice task. In the subsequent experimental
             conditions subjects were presented with two eggs, one of
             which contained food and the other did not. In Study 1 the
             experimenter examined both eggs by smelling or shaking them,
             but only made a failed attempt to open (via biting) the egg
             containing food. In a control condition, the experimenter
             examined and attempted to open both eggs, but in reverse
             order to control for stimulus enhancement. The apes
             significantly preferred the egg that was first examined and
             then bitten, but had no preference in a baseline condition
             in which there were no cues. In Study 2, we investigated
             whether the apes could extend this ability to cues not
             observed in apes so far (i.e., attempting to pull apart the
             egg), as well as whether they made this discrimination based
             on the function of the action the experimenter performed.
             Subjects significantly preferred eggs presented with this
             novel cue, but did not prefer eggs presented with a novel
             but functionally irrelevant action. In Study 3, apes did not
             interpret human actions as cues to food-location when they
             already knew that the eggs were empty. Thus, great apes were
             able to use a variety of experimenter-given cues associated
             with foraging actions to locate hidden food and thereby were
             partially sensitive to the general purpose underlying these
             actions.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-007-0095-2},
   Key = {fds351842}
}

@article{fds351843,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees do not take into account what others can hear in
             a competitive situation.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {175-178},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0097-0},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) know what others can and
             cannot see in a competitive situation. Does this reflect a
             general understanding the perceptions of others? In a study
             by Hare et al. (2000) pairs of chimpanzees competed over two
             pieces of food. Subordinate individuals preferred to
             approach food that was behind a barrier that the dominant
             could not see, suggesting that chimpanzees can take the
             visual perspective of others. We extended this paradigm to
             the auditory modality to investigate whether chimpanzees are
             sensitive to whether a competitor can hear food rewards
             being hidden. Results suggested that the chimpanzees did not
             take what the competitor had heard into account, despite
             being able to locate the hiding place themselves by the
             noise.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-007-0097-0},
   Key = {fds351843}
}

@article{fds351844,
   Author = {Tolar, TD and Lederberg, AR and Gokhale, S and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The development of the ability to recognize the meaning of
             iconic signs.},
   Journal = {Journal of deaf studies and deaf education},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {225-240},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm045},
   Abstract = {Early developmental psychologists viewed iconic
             representation as cognitively less complex than other forms
             of symbolic thought. It is therefore surprising that iconic
             signs are not acquired more easily than arbitrary signs by
             young language learners. One explanation is that children
             younger than 3 years have difficulty interpreting iconicity.
             The current study assessed hearing children's ability to
             interpret the meaning of iconic signs. Sixty-six 2.5- to
             5-year-olds who had no previous exposure to signs were
             required to match iconic signs to pictures of referents.
             Whereas few of the 2.5-year-olds recognized the meaning of
             the iconic signs consistently, more than half of the
             3.0-year-olds and most of 3.5-year-olds performed above
             chance. Thus, the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic
             signs gradually develops during the preschool years.
             Implications of these findings for sign language
             development, receptive signed vocabulary tests, and the
             development of the ability to interpret iconic symbols are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1093/deafed/enm045},
   Key = {fds351844}
}

@article{fds351845,
   Author = {Moll, H and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Fourteen-month-olds know what others experience only in
             joint engagement.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {826-835},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00615.x},
   Abstract = {We investigated how 14-month-old infants know what others
             know. In two studies, an infant played with each of two
             objects in turn while an experimenter was present. Then the
             experimenter left the room, and the infant played with a
             third object with an assistant. The experimenter returned,
             faced all three objects, and said excitedly 'Look! Can you
             give it to me?' In Study 1, the experimenter experienced
             each of the first two toys in episodes of joint visual
             engagement (without manipulation) with the infant. In
             response to her excited request infants gave the
             experimenter the object she did not know, thus demonstrating
             that they knew which ones she knew. In Study 2, infants
             witnessed the experimenter jointly engage around each of the
             experienced toys with the assistant, from a third-person
             perspective. In response to her request, infants did not
             give the experimenter the object she had not experienced. In
             combination with other studies, these results suggest that
             to know what others have experienced 14-month-old infants
             must do more than just perceive others perceiving something;
             they must engage with them actively in joint
             engagement.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00615.x},
   Key = {fds351845}
}

@article{fds351846,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify
             referents for others: a training study.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {78},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1744-1759},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01098.x},
   Abstract = {This training study investigates how children learn to refer
             to things unambiguously. Two hundred twenty-four children
             aged 2.6, 3.6, and 4.6 years were pre- and posttested for
             their ability to request stickers from a dense array.
             Between test sessions, children were assigned to a training
             condition in which they (a) asked for stickers from an
             adult, (b) responded to an adult's requests for stickers,
             (c) observed 1 adult ask another for stickers, or (d) heard
             model descriptions of stickers. All conditions yielded
             improvements in referring strategies, with condition (a)
             being most effective. Four-year-olds additionally
             demonstrated learning effects in a transfer task. These
             results suggest that young children's communication skills
             develop best in response to feedback about their own
             attempts at reference.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01098.x},
   Key = {fds351846}
}

@article{fds351847,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an ultimatum
             game.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {318},
   Number = {5847},
   Pages = {107-109},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1145850},
   Abstract = {Traditional models of economic decision-making assume that
             people are self-interested rational maximizers. Empirical
             research has demonstrated, however, that people will take
             into account the interests of others and are sensitive to
             norms of cooperation and fairness. In one of the most robust
             tests of this finding, the ultimatum game, individuals will
             reject a proposed division of a monetary windfall, at a cost
             to themselves, if they perceive it as unfair. Here we show
             that in an ultimatum game, humans' closest living relatives,
             chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), are rational maximizers and
             are not sensitive to fairness. These results support the
             hypothesis that other-regarding preferences and aversion to
             inequitable outcomes, which play key roles in human social
             organization, distinguish us from our closest living
             relatives.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1145850},
   Key = {fds351847}
}

@article{fds366597,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive
             situation.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {439-448},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0088-1},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzee's perspective-taking abilities are currently
             disputed. Here we show that in some food competition
             contexts, subordinate chimpanzees do take the visual
             perspective of dominant individuals, preferentially
             targeting a hidden piece of the food that the dominant
             cannot see over a piece that is visible to both individuals.
             However, the space where the animals compete is critical in
             determining whether subjects demonstrate this skill. We
             suggest that competition intensity, as mediated by these
             spatial factors, may play an important role in determining
             the strategy chimpanzees utilize in competitive contexts.
             Since some strategies may not require visual perspective
             taking in order to be successful, chimpanzees may not always
             demonstrate this skill. Differences in spatial arrangement
             may therefore account for the conflicting results of past
             studies.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-007-0088-1},
   Key = {fds366597}
}

@article{fds320805,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Call, J and Hernàndez-Lloreda, MV and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition:
             the cultural intelligence hypothesis.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {317},
   Number = {5843},
   Pages = {1360-1366},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1146282},
   Abstract = {Humans have many cognitive skills not possessed by their
             nearest primate relatives. The cultural intelligence
             hypothesis argues that this is mainly due to a
             species-specific set of social-cognitive skills, emerging
             early in ontogeny, for participating and exchanging
             knowledge in cultural groups. We tested this hypothesis by
             giving a comprehensive battery of cognitive tests to large
             numbers of two of humans' closest primate relatives,
             chimpanzees and orangutans, as well as to 2.5-year-old human
             children before literacy and schooling. Supporting the
             cultural intelligence hypothesis and contradicting the
             hypothesis that humans simply have more "general
             intelligence," we found that the children and chimpanzees
             had very similar cognitive skills for dealing with the
             physical world but that the children had more sophisticated
             cognitive skills than either of the ape species for dealing
             with the social world.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1146282},
   Key = {fds320805}
}

@article{fds351848,
   Author = {Kidd, E and Brandt, S and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Object relatives made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of
             the constraints influencing young children's processing of
             relative clauses},
   Journal = {Language and Cognitive Processes},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {860-897},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960601155284},
   Abstract = {We present the results from four studies, two corpora and
             two experimental, which suggest that English- and
             German-speaking children (3;1-4;9 years) use multiple
             constraints to process and produce object relative clauses.
             Our two corpora studies show that children produce object
             relatives that reflect the distributional and discourse
             regularities of the input. Specifically, the results show
             that when children produce object relatives they most often
             do so with (a) an inanimate head noun, and (b) a pronominal
             relative clause subject. Our experimental findings show that
             children use these constraints to process and produce this
             construction type. Moreover, when children were required to
             repeat the object relatives they most often use in
             naturalistic speech, the subject-object asymmetry in
             processing of relative clauses disappeared. We also report
             cross-linguistic differences in children's rate of
             acquisition which reflect properties of the input language.
             Overall, our results suggest that children are sensitive to
             the same constraints on relative clause processing as
             adults.},
   Doi = {10.1080/01690960601155284},
   Key = {fds351848}
}

@article{fds351849,
   Author = {Grassmann, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new
             words.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {677-687},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000907008021},
   Abstract = {German children aged 2;1 heard a sentence containing a nonce
             noun and a nonce verb (Der Feks miekt). Either the noun or
             the verb was prosodically highlighted by increased pitch,
             duration and loudness. Independently, either the object or
             the action in the ongoing referential scene was the new
             element in the situation. Children learned the nonce noun
             only when it was both highlighted prosodically and the
             object in the scene was referentially new. They did not
             learn the nonce verb in any condition. These results suggest
             that from early in linguistic development, young children
             understand that prosodic salience in a sentence indicates
             referential newness.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000907008021},
   Key = {fds351849}
}

@article{fds351850,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {104},
   Number = {32},
   Pages = {13046-13050},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705555104},
   Abstract = {People are willing to punish others at a personal cost, and
             this apparently antisocial tendency can stabilize
             cooperation. What motivates humans to punish noncooperators
             is likely a combination of aversion to both unfair outcomes
             and unfair intentions. Here we report a pair of studies in
             which captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) did not inflict
             costs on conspecifics by knocking food away if the outcome
             alone was personally disadvantageous but did retaliate
             against conspecifics who actually stole the food from them.
             Like humans, chimpanzees retaliate against personally
             harmful actions, but unlike humans, they are indifferent to
             simply personally disadvantageous outcomes and are therefore
             not spiteful.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0705555104},
   Key = {fds351850}
}

@article{fds325191,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Hare, B and Melis, AP and Hanus, D and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young
             children.},
   Journal = {PLoS biology},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {e184},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050184},
   Abstract = {People often act on behalf of others. They do so without
             immediate personal gain, at cost to themselves, and even
             toward unfamiliar individuals. Many researchers have claimed
             that such altruism emanates from a species-unique psychology
             not found in humans' closest living evolutionary relatives,
             such as the chimpanzee. In favor of this view, the few
             experimental studies on altruism in chimpanzees have
             produced mostly negative results. In contrast, we report
             experimental evidence that chimpanzees perform basic forms
             of helping in the absence of rewards spontaneously and
             repeatedly toward humans and conspecifics. In two
             comparative studies, semi-free ranging chimpanzees helped an
             unfamiliar human to the same degree as did human infants,
             irrespective of being rewarded (experiment 1) or whether the
             helping was costly (experiment 2). In a third study,
             chimpanzees helped an unrelated conspecific gain access to
             food in a novel situation that required subjects to use a
             newly acquired skill on behalf of another individual. These
             results indicate that chimpanzees share crucial aspects of
             altruism with humans, suggesting that the roots of human
             altruism may go deeper than previous experimental evidence
             suggested.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pbio.0050184},
   Key = {fds325191}
}

@article{fds351851,
   Author = {Buttelmann, D and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {F31-F38},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00630.x},
   Abstract = {Human infants imitate others' actions 'rationally': they
             copy a demonstrator's action when that action is freely
             chosen, but less when it is forced by some constraint
             (Gergely, Bekkering & Király, 2002). We investigated
             whether enculturated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) also
             imitate rationally. Using Gergely and colleagues' (2002)
             basic procedure, a human demonstrator operated each of six
             apparatuses using an unusual body part (he pressed it with
             his forehead or foot, or sat on it). In the Hands Free
             condition he used this unusual means even though his hands
             were free, suggesting a free choice. In the Hands Occupied
             condition he used the unusual means only because his hands
             were occupied, suggesting a constrained or forced choice.
             Like human infants, chimpanzees imitated the modeled action
             more often in the Hands Free than in the Hands Occupied
             condition. Enculturated chimpanzees thus have some
             understanding of the rationality of others' intentional
             actions, and use this understanding when imitating
             others.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00630.x},
   Key = {fds351851}
}

@article{fds351852,
   Author = {Okamoto-Barth, S and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Great apes' understanding of other individuals' line of
             sight.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {462-468},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01922.x},
   Abstract = {Previous research has shown that many social animals follow
             the gaze of other individuals. However, knowledge about how
             this skill differs between species and whether it shows a
             relationship with genetic distance from humans is still
             fragmentary. In the present study of gaze following in great
             apes, we manipulated the nature of a visual obstruction and
             the presence/absence of a target. We found that bonobos,
             chimpanzees, and gorillas followed gaze significantly more
             often when the obstruction had a window than when it did
             not, just as human infants do. Additionally, bonobos and
             chimpanzees looked at the experimenter's side of a
             windowless obstruction more often than the other species.
             Moreover, bonobos produced more double looks when the
             barrier was opaque than when it had a window, indicating an
             understanding of what other individuals see. The most
             distant human relatives studied, orangutans, showed few
             signs of understanding what another individual saw. Instead,
             they were attracted to the target's location by the target's
             presence, but not by the experimenter's gaze. Great apes'
             perspective-taking skills seem to have increased in the
             evolutionary lineage leading to bonobos, chimpanzees, and
             humans.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01922.x},
   Key = {fds351852}
}

@article{fds351853,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Lieven, E and Theakston, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {French children's use and correction of weird word orders: a
             constructivist account.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {381-409},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s030500090600794x},
   Abstract = {Using the weird word order methodology (Akhtar, 1999), we
             investigated children's understanding of SVO word order in
             French, a language with less consistent argument ordering
             patterns than English. One hundred and twelve French
             children (ages 2; 10 and 3; 9) heard either high or low
             frequency verbs modelled in either SOV or VSO order (both
             ungrammatical). Results showed that: (1) children were more
             likely to adopt a weird word order if they heard lower
             frequency verbs, suggesting gradual learning; (2) children
             in the high frequency conditions tended to correct the
             ungrammatical model they heard to the closest grammatical
             alternative, suggesting different models activated different
             grammatical schemas; and (3) children were less likely to
             express the object of a transitive verb than were English
             children in an equivalent study, suggesting object
             expression is more difficult to master in French, perhaps
             because of its inconsistency in the input. These findings
             are discussed in the context of a usage-based model of
             language acquisition.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s030500090600794x},
   Key = {fds351853}
}

@article{fds351854,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M and Liszkowski, U},
   Title = {A new look at infant pointing.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {78},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {705-722},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x},
   Abstract = {The current article proposes a new theory of infant pointing
             involving multiple layers of intentionality and shared
             intentionality. In the context of this theory, evidence is
             presented for a rich interpretation of prelinguistic
             communication, that is, one that posits that when
             12-month-old infants point for an adult they are in some
             sense trying to influence her mental states. Moreover,
             evidence is also presented for a deeply social view in which
             infant pointing is best understood--on many levels and in
             many ways--as depending on uniquely human skills and
             motivations for cooperation and shared intentionality (e.g.,
             joint intentions and attention with others). Children's
             early linguistic skills are built on this already existing
             platform of prelinguistic communication.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x},
   Key = {fds351854}
}

@article{fds366598,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Behne, T and Moll,
             H},
   Title = {Understanding of intentions, shared intentions: The origins
             of cultural thinking},
   Journal = {Magyar Pszichologiai Szemle},
   Volume = {62},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {61-105},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/MPSzle.62.2007.1.4},
   Abstract = {We propose that the crucial difference between human
             cognition and that of other species is the ability to
             participate with others in collaborative activities with
             shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality.
             Participation in such activities requires not only
             especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural
             learning, but also a unique motivation to share
             psychological states with others and unique forms of
             cognitive representation for doing so. The result of
             participating in these activities is species-unique forms of
             cultural cognition and evolution, enabling everything from
             the creation and use of linguistic symbols to the
             construction of social norms and individual beliefs to the
             establishment of social institutions. In support of this
             proposal we argue and present evidence that great apes (and
             some children with autism) understand the basics of
             intentional action, but they still do not participate in
             activities involving joint intentions and attention (shared
             intentionality). Human children's skills of shared
             intentionality develop gradually during the first 14 months
             of life as two ontogenetic pathways intertwine: 1. the
             general ape line of understanding others as animate,
             goal-directed, and intentional agents; and 2. a
             species-unique motivation to share emotions, experience, and
             activities with other persons. The developmental outcome is
             children's ability to construct dialogic cognitive
             representations, which enable them to participate in earnest
             in the collectivity that is human cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1556/MPSzle.62.2007.1.4},
   Key = {fds366598}
}

@article{fds325192,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Hare, B and Lehmann, H and Call,
             J},
   Title = {Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great
             apes and human infants: the cooperative eye
             hypothesis.},
   Journal = {Journal of human evolution},
   Volume = {52},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {314-320},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.10.001},
   Abstract = {As compared with other primates, humans have especially
             visible eyes (e.g., white sclera). One hypothesis is that
             this feature of human eyes evolved to make it easier for
             conspecifics to follow an individual's gaze direction in
             close-range joint attentional and communicative
             interactions, which would seem to imply especially
             cooperative (mututalistic) conspecifics. In the current
             study, we tested one aspect of this cooperative eye
             hypothesis by comparing the gaze following behavior of great
             apes to that of human infants. A human experimenter "looked"
             to the ceiling either with his eyes only, head only (eyes
             closed), both head and eyes, or neither. Great apes followed
             gaze to the ceiling based mainly on the human's head
             direction (although eye direction played some role as well).
             In contrast, human infants relied almost exclusively on eye
             direction in these same situations. These results
             demonstrate that humans are especially reliant on eyes in
             gaze following situations, and thus, suggest that eyes
             evolved a new social function in human evolution, most
             likely to support cooperative (mututalistic) social
             interactions.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.10.001},
   Key = {fds325192}
}

@article{fds351856,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12
             months of age.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {F1-F7},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00552.x},
   Abstract = {There is currently controversy over the nature of
             1-year-olds' social-cognitive understanding and motives. In
             this study we investigated whether 12-month-old infants
             point for others with an understanding of their knowledge
             states and with a prosocial motive for sharing experiences
             with them. Declarative pointing was elicited in four
             conditions created by crossing two factors: an adult partner
             (1) was already attending to the target event or not, and
             (2) emoted positively or neutrally. Pointing was also coded
             after the event had ceased. The findings suggest that
             12-month-olds point to inform others of events they do not
             know about, that they point to share an attitude about
             mutually attended events others already know about, and that
             they can point (already prelinguistically) to absent
             referents. These findings provide strong support for a
             mentalistic and prosocial interpretation of infants'
             prelinguistic communication.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00552.x},
   Key = {fds351856}
}

@article{fds351857,
   Author = {Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have
             experienced.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {309-317},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.309},
   Abstract = {Fourteen- and 18-month-old infants observed an adult
             experiencing each of 2 objects (experienced objects) and
             then leaving the room; the infant then played with a 3rd
             object while the adult was gone (unexperienced object). The
             adult interacted with the 2 experienced objects in 1 of 3
             ways: by (a) sharing them with the infant in an episode of
             joint engagement, (b) actively manipulating and inspecting
             them on his or her own as the infant watched (individual
             engagement), or (c) looking at them from a distance as the
             infant played with them (onlooking). As evidenced in a
             selection task, infants of both ages knew which objects had
             been experienced by the adult in the joint engagement
             condition, only the 18-month-olds knew this in the
             individual engagement condition, and infants at neither age
             knew this in the onlooking condition. These results suggest
             that infants are 1st able to determine what adults know
             (have experienced) on the basis of their direct, triadic
             engagements with them.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.309},
   Key = {fds351857}
}

@article{fds351858,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Reference and attitude in infant pointing.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-20},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000906007689},
   Abstract = {We investigated two main components of infant declarative
             pointing, reference and attitude, in two experiments with a
             total of 106 preverbal infants at 1;0. When an experimenter
             (E) responded to the declarative pointing of these infants
             by attending to an incorrect referent (with positive
             attitude), infants repeated pointing within trials to
             redirect E's attention, showing an understanding of E's
             reference and active message repair. In contrast, when E
             identified infants' referent correctly but displayed a
             disinterested attitude, infants did not repeat pointing
             within trials and pointed overall in fewer trials, showing
             an understanding of E's unenthusiastic attitude about the
             referent. When E attended to infants' intended referent AND
             shared interest in it, infants were most satisfied, showing
             no message repair within trials and pointing overall in more
             trials. These results suggest that by twelve months of age
             infant declarative pointing is a full communicative act
             aimed at sharing with others both attention to a referent
             and a specific attitude about that referent.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000906007689},
   Key = {fds351858}
}

@article{fds351859,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {"This way!", "No! That way!"-3-year olds know that two
             people can have mutually incompatible desires},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {47-68},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.08.002},
   Abstract = {In theory of mind research, there is a long standing dispute
             about whether children come to understand the subjectivity
             of both desires and beliefs at the same time (around age 4),
             or whether there is an asymmetry such that desires are
             understood earlier. To address this issue, 3-year olds'
             understanding of situations in which two persons have
             mutually incompatible desires was tested in two studies.
             Results revealed that (i) children were quite proficient at
             ascribing incompatible desires to two persons, and in
             simpler scenarios even incompatible desire-dependent
             emotions; (ii) children showed this proficiency even though
             they mostly failed the false belief task. Overall, these
             results suggest that there is an asymmetry such that young
             children come to understand the subjective nature of desires
             before they understand the corresponding subjectivity of
             beliefs. Possible explanations for this asymmetry are
             discussed in light of conceptual change and
             information-processing accounts of theory of mind
             development. © 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.08.002},
   Key = {fds351859}
}

@article{fds351860,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {271-294},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x},
   Abstract = {Two experiments investigated the proclivity of 14-month-old
             infants (a) to altruistically help others toward individual
             goals, and (b) to cooperate toward a shared goal. The
             infants helped another person by handing over objects the
             other person was unsuccessfully roaching for, but did not
             help reliably in situations involving more complex goals.
             When a programmed adult partner interrupted a joint
             cooperative activity at specific moments, infants sometimes
             tried to reengage the adult, perhaps indicating that they
             understood the interdependency of actions toward a shared
             goal. However, as compared to 18- and 24-month-olds, their
             skills in behaviorally coordinating their actions with a
             social partner remained rudimentary. Results are integrated
             into a model of cooperative activities as they develop over
             the 2nd year of life. Copyright © 2007, Lawrence Erlbaum
             Associates, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x},
   Key = {fds351860}
}

@article{fds351861,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M},
   Title = {Shared intentionality.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {121-125},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x},
   Abstract = {We argue for the importance of processes of shared
             intentionality in children's early cognitive development. We
             look briefly at four important social-cognitive skills and
             how they are transformed by shared intentionality. In each
             case, we look first at a kind of individualistic version of
             the skill -- as exemplified most clearly in the behavior of
             chimpanzees -- and then at a version based on shared
             intentionality -- as exemplified most clearly in the
             behavior of human 1- and 2-year-olds. We thus see the
             following transformations: gaze following into joint
             attention, social manipulation into cooperative
             communication, group activity into collaboration, and social
             learning into instructed learning. We conclude by
             highlighting the role that shared intentionality may play in
             integrating more biologically based and more culturally
             based theories of human development.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x},
   Key = {fds351861}
}

@article{fds351862,
   Author = {Schwier, C and van Maanen, C and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {303-311},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in1003_6},
   Abstract = {Gergely, Bekkering, and Király (2002) demonstrated that
             14-month-old infants engage in "rational imitation." To
             investigate the development and flexibility of this skill,
             we tested 12-month-olds on a different but analogous task.
             Infants watched as an adult made a toy animal use a
             particular action to get to an endpoint. In 1 condition
             there was a barrier that prevented a more straightforward
             action and so gave the actor no choice but to use the
             demonstrated action. In the other condition there was no
             barrier, so the actor had a free choice to use the
             demonstrated action or not. Twelve-month-olds showed the
             same pattern of results as in Gergely and colleagues' study:
             They copied the particular action demonstrated more often
             when the adult freely chose to use the action than when she
             was forced to use it. Twelve-month-olds, too, thus show an
             understanding of others' intentions as rational choices and
             can use this understanding in cultural learning contexts.
             Copyright © 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15327078in1003_6},
   Key = {fds351862}
}

@article{fds351863,
   Author = {Tennie, C and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Push or pull: Imitation vs. emulation in great apes and
             human children},
   Journal = {Ethology},
   Volume = {112},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {1159-1169},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x},
   Abstract = {All four species of great apes and young human children
             (12-24 mo of age) were administered an imitation task
             designed to distinguish between results learning (emulation)
             and action learning (imitation). Some subjects were exposed
             to a demonstrator either pushing or pulling a door to open a
             box, whereas others simply saw the door of the box opening
             itself in one of the two directions (the ghost control).
             Most of the apes successfully opened the box in both
             experimental conditions, as well as in a baseline condition,
             but without being influenced either by the demonstrator's
             actions or by the door's motions. In contrast, human
             children over 12 mo of age were influenced by the
             demonstration: the 18-mo-olds were influenced by the
             demonstrator's actions, and the 24-mo-olds were influenced
             both by the demonstrator's actions and by the door's motions
             in the ghost control. These results provide support for the
             hypothesis that human children have a greater propensity
             than great apes for focusing either on a demonstrator's
             action or on the result of their action, as needed, in
             social learning situations. © 2006 The Authors.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x},
   Key = {fds351863}
}

@article{fds366599,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Are apes really inequity averse?},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {273},
   Number = {1605},
   Pages = {3123-3128},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.3693},
   Abstract = {Brosnan et al. (Brosnan, S. F. Schiff, H. C. & de Waal, F.
             B. M. 2005 Tolerance for inequity may increase with social
             closeness in chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B272, 253-258) found
             that chimpanzees showed increased levels of rejection for
             less-preferred food when competitors received better food
             than themselves and postulated as an explanation inequity
             aversion. In the present study, we extended these findings
             by adding important control conditions, and we investigated
             whether inequity aversion could also be found in the other
             great ape species and whether it would be influenced by
             subjects' relationship with the competitor. In the present
             study, subjects showed a pattern of food rejection opposite
             to the subjects of the above study by Brosnan et al. (2005).
             Our apes ignored fewer food pieces and stayed longer in
             front of the experimenter when a conspecific received better
             food than themselves. Moreover, chimpanzees begged more
             vigorously when the conspecific got favoured food. The most
             plausible explanation for these results is the food
             expectation hypothesis - seeing another individual receive
             high-quality food creates the expectation of receiving the
             same food oneself - and not inequity aversion.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2006.3693},
   Key = {fds366599}
}

@article{fds351864,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Goats' behaviour in a competitive food paradigm: Evidence
             for perspective taking?},
   Journal = {Behaviour},
   Volume = {143},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {1341-1356},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853906778987542},
   Abstract = {Many mammalian species are highly social, creating
             intra-group competition for such things as food and mates.
             Recent research with nonhuman primates indicates that in
             competitive situations individuals know what other
             individuals can and cannot see, and they use this knowledge
             to their advantage in various ways. In the current study, we
             extended these findings to a non-primate species, the
             domestic goat, using the conspecific competition paradigm
             developed by Hare et al. (2000). Like chimpanzees and some
             other nonhuman primates, goats live in fission-fusion
             societies, form coalitions and alliances, and are known to
             reconcile after fights. In the current study, a dominant and
             a subordinate individual competed for food, but in some
             cases the subordinate could see things that the dominant
             could not. In the condition where dominants could only see
             one piece of food but subordinates could see both,
             subordinates' preferences depended on whether they received
             aggression from the dominant animal during the experiment.
             Subjects who received aggression preferred the hidden over
             the visible piece of food, whereas subjects who never
             received aggression significantly preferred the visible
             piece. By using this strategy, goats who had not received
             aggression got significantly more food than the other goats.
             Such complex social interactions may be supported by
             cognitive mechanisms similar to those of chimpanzees. We
             discuss these results in the context of current issues in
             mammalian cognition and socio-ecology. © Brill Academic
             Publishers 2006.},
   Doi = {10.1163/156853906778987542},
   Key = {fds351864}
}

@article{fds351865,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds grasp the intentional structure of pretense
             acts.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {557-564},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00533.x},
   Abstract = {Twenty-two- and 27-month-old children were tested for their
             understanding of pretending as a specific intentional action
             form. Pairs of superficially similar behaviors - pretending
             to perform an action and trying to perform that action -
             were demonstrated to children. The 27-month-olds, and to
             some degree the 22-month-olds, showed in their responses
             that they understood the intentional structure of both kinds
             of behaviors: after pretense models, they themselves
             performed appropriate inferential pretense acts, whereas
             after the trying models they properly performed the action
             or tried to perform it with novel means. These findings are
             discussed in the light of recent debates about children's
             developing understanding of pretense and theory of
             mind.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00533.x},
   Key = {fds351865}
}

@article{fds325195,
   Author = {Hare, B and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by
             hiding.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {101},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {495-514},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.011},
   Abstract = {There is little experimental evidence that any non-human
             species is capable of purposefully attempting to manipulate
             the psychological states of others deceptively (e.g.,
             manipulating what another sees). We show here that
             chimpanzees, one of humans' two closest primate relatives,
             sometimes attempt to actively conceal things from others.
             Specifically, when competing with a human in three novel
             tests, eight chimpanzees, from their first trials, chose to
             approach a contested food item via a route hidden from the
             human's view (sometimes using a circuitous path to do so).
             These findings not only corroborate previous work showing
             that chimpanzees know what others can and cannot see, but
             also suggest that when competing for food chimpanzees are
             skillful at manipulating, to their own advantage, whether
             others can or cannot see them.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.011},
   Key = {fds325195}
}

@article{fds351866,
   Author = {Abbot-Smith, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based
             account of syntactic acquisition},
   Journal = {Linguistic Review},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {275-290},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/TLR.2006.011},
   Abstract = {The early phases of syntactic acquisition are characterized
             by many input frequency and item effects, which argue
             against theories assuming innate access to classical
             syntactic categories. In formulating an alternative view, we
             consider both prototype and exemplar-learning models of
             categorization. We argue for a 'hybrid' usage-based view in
             which acquisition depends on exemplar learning and
             retention, out of which permanent abstract schemas gradually
             emerge and are immanent across the summed similarity of
             exemplar collections. These schemas are graded in strength
             depending on the number of exemplars and the degree to which
             semantic similarity is reinforced by phonological, lexical,
             and distributional similarity. © Walter de Gruyter
             2006.},
   Doi = {10.1515/TLR.2006.011},
   Key = {fds351866}
}

@article{fds351867,
   Author = {Moll, H and Koring, C and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Infants determine others' focus of attention by pragmatics
             and exlusion},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {411-430},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0703_9},
   Abstract = {In the studies presented here, infants' understanding of
             others' attention was assessed when gaze direction cues were
             not diagnostic. Fourteen-, 18- and 24-month-olds witnessed
             an adult look to the side of an object and express
             excitement. In 1 experimental condition this object was new
             for the adult because she was not present while the child
             and someone else played with it earlier. Children responded
             to this as if they assumed that the adult was excited about
             this new object as a whole. In the other condition the
             object was one with which the infant and this adult had just
             previously played for a minute. In this case children
             appeared to assume that the adult could not be excited about
             this object in itself. They responded either by attending to
             a specific part of the object or, more frequently, by
             looking around the room for another object. These results
             suggest that 1-year-olds can determine what others are
             attending to based on a pragmatic assessment of what is new
             and what is old for them combined with a form of reasoning
             by exclusion. Copyright © 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum
             Associates, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15327647jcd0703_9},
   Key = {fds351867}
}

@article{fds351868,
   Author = {Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Level I perspective-taking at 24 months of
             age},
   Journal = {British Journal of Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {603-613},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151005X55370},
   Abstract = {The current study sought to determine the age at which
             children first engage in Level I visual perspective-taking,
             in which they understand that the content of what another
             person sees in a situation may sometimes differ from what
             they see. An adult entered the room searching for an object.
             One candidate object was out in the open, whereas another
             was visible for the child but behind an occluder from the
             adults perspective. When asked to help the adult find the
             sought-for object, 24-month-old children, but not
             18-month-old children, handed him the occluded object
             (whereas in a control condition they showed no preference
             for the occluded toy). We argue that the performance of the
             24-month-olds requires Level I visual perspective-taking
             skills and that this is the youngest age at which these
             skills have been demonstrated. © 2006 The British
             Psychological Society.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151005X55370},
   Key = {fds351868}
}

@article{fds351869,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and
             competitive motives in a communicative situation.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {518-529},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00519.x},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus)
             (Study 1) and 18- and 24-month-old human children (Study 2)
             participated in a novel communicative task. A human
             experimenter (E) hid food or a toy in one of two opaque
             containers before gesturing towards the reward's location in
             one of two ways. In the Informing condition, she attempted
             to help the subject find the hidden object by simply
             pointing to the correct container. In the Prohibiting
             condition, E held out her arm toward the correct container
             (palm out) and told the subject firmly 'Don't take this
             one.' As in previous studies, the apes were at chance in the
             Informing condition. However, they were above chance in the
             new Prohibiting condition. Human 18-month-olds showed this
             same pattern of results, whereas 24-month-olds showed the
             opposite pattern: they were better in the Informing
             condition than in the Prohibiting condition. In our
             interpretation, success in the Prohibiting condition
             requires subjects to understand E's goal toward them and
             their behavior, and then to make an inference (she would
             only prohibit if there were something good in there).
             Success in the Informing condition requires subjects to
             understand a cooperative communicative motive - which
             apparently apes and young infants find difficult.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00519.x},
   Key = {fds351869}
}

@article{fds325194,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance
             constraints on cooperation},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {72},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {275-286},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.018},
   Abstract = {The cooperative abilities of captive chimpanzees, Pan
             troglodytes, in experiments do not match the sophistication
             that might be predicted based on their naturally occurring
             cooperative behaviours. This discrepancy might partly be
             because in previous experiments potential chimpanzee
             cooperators were partnered without regard to their social
             relationship. We investigated the ability of chimpanzee
             dyads to solve a physical task cooperatively in relation to
             their interindividual tolerance levels. Pairs that were most
             capable of sharing food outside the test were also able to
             cooperate spontaneously (by simultaneously pulling two
             ropes) to obtain food. In contrast, pairs that were less
             inclined to share food outside of the test were unlikely to
             cooperate. Furthermore, previously successful subjects
             stopped cooperating when paired with a less tolerant
             partner, even when the food rewards were presented in a
             dispersed and divisible form to reduce competition between
             subjects. These results show that although chimpanzees are
             capable of spontaneous cooperation in a novel instrumental
             task, tolerance acts as a constraint on their ability to
             solve such cooperative problems. This finding highlights the
             importance of controlling such social constraints in future
             experiments on chimpanzee cooperation, and suggests that the
             evolution of human-like cooperative skills might have been
             preceded by the evolution of a more egalitarian social
             system and a more human-like temperament. © 2006 The
             Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.018},
   Key = {fds325194}
}

@article{fds351870,
   Author = {Ambridge, B and Rowland, CF and Theakston, AL and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Comparing different accounts of inversion errors in
             children's non-subject wh-questions: 'What experimental data
             can tell us?'.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {519-557},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000906007513},
   Abstract = {This study investigated different accounts of children's
             acquisition of non-subject wh-questions. Questions using
             each of 4 wh-words (what, who, how and why), and 3
             auxiliaries (BE, DO and CAN) in 3sg and 3pl form were
             elicited from 28 children aged 3;6-4;6. Rates of
             noninversion error (Who she is hitting?) were found not to
             differ by wh-word, auxiliary or number alone, but by lexical
             auxiliary subtype and by wh-word+lexical auxiliary
             combination. This finding counts against simple rule-based
             accounts of question acquisition that include no role for
             the lexical subtype of the auxiliary, and suggests that
             children may initially acquire wh-word + lexical auxiliary
             combinations from the input. For DO questions,
             auxiliary-doubling errors (What does she does like?) were
             also observed, although previous research has found that
             such errors are virtually non-existent for positive
             questions. Possible reasons for this discrepancy are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000906007513},
   Key = {fds351870}
}

@article{fds351871,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Carpenter, M and Striano, T and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for
             others},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {173-187},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2},
   Abstract = {Classically, infants are thought to point for 2 main
             reasons: (a) They point imperatively when they want an adult
             to do something for them (e.g., give them something;
             "Juice!"), and (b) they point declaratively when they want
             an adult to share attention with them to some interesting
             event or object ("Look!"). Here we demonstrate the existence
             of another motive for infants' early pointing gestures: to
             inform another person of the location of an object that
             person is searching for. This informative motive for
             pointing suggests that from very early in ontogeny humans
             conceive of others as intentional agents with informational
             states and they have the motivation to provide such
             information communicatively. Copyright © 2006, Lawrence
             Erlbaum Associates, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2},
   Key = {fds351871}
}

@article{fds366600,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M and Striano, T},
   Title = {The role of experience and discourse in children's
             developing understanding of pretend play
             actions},
   Journal = {British Journal of Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {305-335},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151005X36001},
   Abstract = {The present work investigated the development of an explicit
             understanding of pretend play actions. Study I revealed a
             long décalage between earlier implicit understanding of
             pretence as an intentional activity and a later more
             explicit understanding. Study 2 was a training study. It
             tested for two factors - systematic pretence experience and
             explicit pretence discourse - that may be important in
             development from early implicit to later explicit pretence
             understanding. Two training groups of 3.5-year-old children
             received the same pretence experiences involving systematic
             contrasts between pretending, really performing and trying
             to perform actions. In the 'explicit' group, these
             experiences were talked about with explicit 'pretend to' and
             'pretend that' language. In the 'implicit' group no such
             discourse was used, but only implicit discourse in talking
             about pretence versus real actions. The two training groups
             were compared with a control group that received functional
             play experience. After training, only the explicit group
             showed improvement in their explicit pretence understanding.
             In none of the groups was there any transfer to tasks
             tapping mental state understanding, false belief (FB) and
             appearance-reality, (A-R). The findings are discussed in the
             context of current theories about the developmental
             relations between pretence, discourse, and mental state
             understanding. © 2006 The British Psychological
             Society.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151005X36001},
   Key = {fds366600}
}

@article{fds351873,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory
             information from others.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {120},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {154-162},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.2.154},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) competed with a human for
             food. The human sat inside a booth, with 1 piece of food to
             her left and 1 to her right, which she could retract from
             her chimpanzee competitor's reach as needed. In Experiment
             1, chimpanzees could approach either side of the booth
             unseen but then had to reach through 1 of 2 tunnels (1
             clear, 1 opaque) for the food. In Experiment 2, both tunnels
             were clear and the human was looking away, but 1 of the
             tunnels made a loud noise when it was opened. Chimpanzees
             preferentially reached through the opaque tunnel in the
             first study and the silent tunnel in the second,
             successfully concealing their taking of the food from the
             human competitor in both cases. These results suggest that
             chimpanzees can, in some circumstances, actively manipulate
             the visual and auditory perception of others by concealing
             information from them.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.120.2.154},
   Key = {fds351873}
}

@article{fds351874,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Chen, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cooperative activities in young children and
             chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {77},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {640-663},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00895.x},
   Abstract = {Human children 18-24 months of age and 3 young chimpanzees
             interacted in 4 cooperative activities with a human adult
             partner. The human children successfully participated in
             cooperative problem-solving activities and social games,
             whereas the chimpanzees were uninterested in the social
             games. As an experimental manipulation, in each task the
             adult partner stopped participating at a specific point
             during the activity. All children produced at least one
             communicative attempt to reengage him, perhaps suggesting
             that they were trying to reinstate a shared goal. No
             chimpanzee ever made any communicative attempt to reengage
             the partner. These results are interpreted as evidence for a
             uniquely human form of cooperative activity involving shared
             intentionality that emerges in the second year of
             life.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00895.x},
   Key = {fds351874}
}

@article{fds325193,
   Author = {Jensen, K and Hare, B and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What's in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and
             spite in chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Proceedings. Biological sciences},
   Volume = {273},
   Number = {1589},
   Pages = {1013-1021},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3417},
   Abstract = {Sensitivity to fairness may influence whether individuals
             choose to engage in acts that are mutually beneficial,
             selfish, altruistic, or spiteful. In a series of three
             experiments, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) could pull a rope
             to access out-of-reach food while concomitantly pulling
             another piece of food further away. In the first study, they
             could make a choice that solely benefited themselves
             (selfishness), or both themselves and another chimpanzee
             (mutualism). In the next two experiments, they could choose
             between providing food solely for another chimpanzee
             (altruism), or for neither while preventing the other
             chimpanzee from receiving a benefit (spite). The main result
             across all studies was that chimpanzees made their choices
             based solely on personal gain, with no regard for the
             outcomes of a conspecific. These results raise questions
             about the origins of human cooperative behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rspb.2005.3417},
   Key = {fds325193}
}

@article{fds351875,
   Author = {Ambridge, B and Theakston, AL and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The distributed learning effect for children's acquisition
             of an abstract syntactic construction},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {174-193},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.09.003},
   Abstract = {In many cognitive domains, learning is more effective when
             exemplars are distributed over a number of sessions than
             when they are all presented within one session. The present
             study investigated this distributed learning effect with
             respect to English-speaking children's acquisition of a
             complex grammatical construction. Forty-eight children aged
             3;6-5;10 (Experiment 1) and 72 children aged 4;0-5;0
             (Experiment 2) were given 10 exposures to the construction
             all in one session (massed), or on a schedule of two trials
             per day for 5 days (distributed-pairs), or one trial per day
             for 10 days (distributed). Children in both the
             distributed-pairs and distributed conditions learnt the
             construction better than children in the massed condition,
             as evidenced by productive use of this construction with a
             verb that had not been presented during training.
             Methodological and theoretical implications of this finding
             are discussed, with particular reference to single-process
             accounts of language acquisition. © 2005 Elsevier Inc. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.09.003},
   Key = {fds351875}
}

@article{fds351876,
   Author = {Kidd, E and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Examining the role of lexical frequency in the acquisition
             and processing of sentential complements},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {93-107},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.006},
   Abstract = {We present empirical data showing that the relative
             frequency with which a verb normally appears in a syntactic
             construction predicts young children's ability to remember
             and repeat sentences instantiating that construction.
             Children aged 2;10-5;8 years were asked to repeat
             grammatical and ungrammatical sentential complement
             sentences (e.g., 'I think + S'). The sentences contained
             complement-taking verbs (CTVs) used with differing
             frequencies in children's natural speech. All children
             repeated sentences containing high frequency CTVs (e.g.,
             think) more accurately than those containing low frequency
             CTVs (e.g., hear), and made more sophisticated corrections
             to ungrammatical sentences containing high frequency CTVs.
             The data suggest that, like adults, children are sensitive
             to lexico-constructional collocations. The implications for
             language acquisition are discussed. © 2006 Elsevier Inc.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.006},
   Key = {fds351876}
}

@article{fds351877,
   Author = {Herrmann, E and Melis, AP and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice
             task.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {118-130},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-005-0013-4},
   Abstract = {In previous studies great apes have shown little ability to
             locate hidden food using a physical marker placed by a human
             directly on the target location. In this study, we
             hypothesized that the perceptual similarity between an
             iconic cue and the hidden reward (baited container) would
             help apes to infer the location of the food. In the first
             two experiments, we found that if an iconic cue is given in
             addition to a spatial/indexical cue - e.g., picture or
             replica of a banana placed on the target location - apes
             (chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, gorillas) as a group
             performed above chance. However, we also found in two
             further experiments that when iconic cues were given on
             their own without spatial/indexical information (iconic cue
             held up by human with no diagnostic spatial/indexical
             information), the apes were back to chance performance. Our
             overall conclusion is that although iconic information helps
             apes in the process of searching hidden food, the poor
             performance found in the last two experiments is due to
             apes' lack of understanding of the informative (cooperative)
             communicative intention of the experimenter.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-005-0013-4},
   Key = {fds351877}
}

@article{fds325196,
   Author = {Melis, AP and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {311},
   Number = {5765},
   Pages = {1297-1300},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1123007},
   Abstract = {Humans collaborate with non-kin in special ways, but the
             evolutionary foundations of these collaborative skills
             remain unclear. We presented chimpanzees with collaboration
             problems in which they had to decide when to recruit a
             partner and which potential partner to recruit. In an
             initial study, individuals recruited a collaborator only
             when solving the problem required collaboration. In a second
             study, individuals recruited the more effective of two
             partners on the basis of their experience with each of them
             on a previous day. Therefore, recognizing when collaboration
             is necessary and determining who is the best collaborative
             partner are skills shared by both chimpanzees and humans, so
             such skills may have been present in their common ancestor
             before humans evolved their own complex forms of
             collaboration.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1123007},
   Key = {fds325196}
}

@article{fds351878,
   Author = {Warneken, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Altruistic helping in human infants and young
             chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {311},
   Number = {5765},
   Pages = {1301-1303},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1121448},
   Abstract = {Human beings routinely help others to achieve their goals,
             even when the helper receives no immediate benefit and the
             person helped is a stranger. Such altruistic behaviors
             (toward non-kin) are extremely rare evolutionarily, with
             some theorists even proposing that they are uniquely human.
             Here we show that human children as young as 18 months of
             age (prelinguistic or just-linguistic) quite readily help
             others to achieve their goals in a variety of different
             situations. This requires both an understanding of others'
             goals and an altruistic motivation to help. In addition, we
             demonstrate similar though less robust skills and
             motivations in three young chimpanzees.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1121448},
   Key = {fds351878}
}

@article{fds351879,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Kaminski, J and Riedel, J and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social
             dog, causal ape.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {120},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {38-47},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.38},
   Abstract = {Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and great apes from the
             genus Pan were tested on a series of object choice tasks. In
             each task, the location of hidden food was indicated for
             subjects by some kind of communicative, behavioral, or
             physical cue. On the basis of differences in the ecologies
             of these 2 genera, as well as on previous research, the
             authors hypothesized that dogs should be especially skillful
             in using human communicative cues such as the pointing
             gesture, whereas apes should be especially skillful in using
             physical, causal cues such as food in a cup making noise
             when it is shaken. The overall pattern of performance by the
             2 genera strongly supported this social-dog, causal-ape
             hypothesis. This result is discussed in terms of apes'
             adaptations for complex, extractive foraging and dogs'
             adaptations, during the domestication process, for
             cooperative communication with humans.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.38},
   Key = {fds351879}
}

@article{fds351872,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Lieven, E and Theakston, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on
             young children's use of referring expressions},
   Journal = {Applied Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {403-422},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716406060334},
   Abstract = {Choosing appropriate referring expressions requires
             assessing whether a referent is "available" to the addressee
             either perceptually or through discourse. In Study 1, we
             found that 3- and 4-year-olds, but not 2-year-olds, chose
             different referring expressions (noun vs. pronoun) depending
             on whether their addressee could see the intended referent
             or not. In Study 2, in more neutral discourse contexts than
             previous studies, we found that 3- and 4-year-olds clearly
             differed in their use of referring expressions according to
             whether their addressee had already mentioned a referent.
             Moreover, 2-year-olds responded with more naming
             constructions when the referent had not been mentioned
             previously. This suggests that, despite early
             social-cognitive developments, (a) it takes time to master
             the given/new contrast linguistically, and (b) children
             understand the contrast earlier based on discourse, rather
             than perceptual context. © 2006 Cambridge University
             Press.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0142716406060334},
   Key = {fds351872}
}

@article{fds351880,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Pika, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Gestural communication of orangutans (pongo
             pygmaeus)},
   Journal = {Gesture},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-38},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/gest.6.1.02lie},
   Abstract = {This study represents a systematic investigation of the
             communicative repertoire of Sumatran orangutans (Pongo
             pygmaeus abelii), with a focus on intentional signals in two
             groups of captive orangutans. The goal was to analyze the
             signal repertoire with respect to (1) the number and
             frequency of signals (gestures, facial expressions, and
             actions), (2) the variability of individual repertoires as a
             function of group, age class, and sex, and (3) the
             flexibility of use in terms of ‘means-end dissociation’
             and ‘audience effects’ and to interpret the findings in
             terms of the ecology, social structure and socio-cognitive
             skills of orangutans. The results show that orangutans use a
             remarkable number of signals including tactile and visual
             gestures as well as several more complex actions, though few
             facial expressions and no auditory gestures were observed.
             One third of signals were used within a play context,
             followed by one fourth of interactions in the context of
             ingestion. Although the repertoire included several visual
             gestures, most of the signals produced were tactile gestures
             and they were used particularly in the contexts of
             affiliation and agonism, whereas visual gestures dominated
             in the context of grooming, ingestion and sexual behavior.
             Individual repertoires showed a remarkable degree of
             variability as a function of age and group affiliation.
             Orangutans used their signals flexibly in several functional
             contexts and adjusted the signal they used depending on the
             attentional state of the recipient, similar to findings of
             other great ape species and gibbons. Thus, the communicative
             behavior of orangutans is characterized by a variable and
             flexible use of signals possibly reflecting their highly
             variable social structure and their sophisticated
             socio-cognitive skills, with the dominance of tactile
             gestures corresponding to the arboreal nature of this
             species. © 2006 John Benjamins Publishing
             Company.},
   Doi = {10.1075/gest.6.1.02lie},
   Key = {fds351880}
}

@article{fds351881,
   Author = {Riedel, J and Buttelmann, D and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to
             locate hidden food.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {27-35},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-005-0256-0},
   Abstract = {Dogs can use the placement of an arbitrary marker to locate
             hidden food in an object-choice situation. We tested
             domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in three studies aimed at
             pinning down the relative contributions of the human's hand
             and the marker itself. We baited one of two cups (outside of
             the dogs' view) and gave the dog a communicative cue to find
             the food. Study 1 systematically varied dogs' perceptual
             access to the marker placing event, so that dogs saw either
             the whole human, the hand only, the marker only, or nothing.
             Follow-up trials investigated the effect of removing the
             marker before the dog's choice. Dogs used the marker as a
             communicative cue even when it had been removed prior to the
             dog's choice and attached more importance to this cue than
             to the hand that placed it although the presence of the hand
             boosted performance when it appeared together with the
             marker. Study 2 directly contrasted the importance of the
             hand and the marker and revealed that the effect of the
             marker diminished if it had been associated with both cups.
             In contrast touching both cups with the hand had no effect
             on performance. Study 3 investigated whether the means of
             marker placement (intentional or accidental) had an effect
             on dogs' choices. Results showed that dogs did not
             differentiate intentional and accidental placing of the
             marker. These results suggest that dogs use the marker as a
             genuine communicative cue quite independently from the
             experimenter's actions.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-005-0256-0},
   Key = {fds351881}
}

@article{fds351882,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Beyond formalities: The case of language
             acquisition},
   Journal = {Linguistic Review},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2-4},
   Pages = {183-197},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.183},
   Abstract = {Generative grammar retained from American structural
             linguistics the 'formal' approach, which basically effaces
             the semantic and pragmatic dimensions of grammar. This
             creates serious problems for an account of language
             acquisition, most especially the problem of how to link
             universal grammar to some particular language (the linking
             problem). Parameters do not help the situation, as they
             depend on a prior linking of the lexical and functional
             categories of a language to universal grammar. In contrast,
             usage-based accounts of language acquisition do not posit an
             innate universal grammar and so have no linking problem. And
             if children's cognitive and social skills are conceptualized
             in the right way, there is no poverty of the stimulus in
             this approach either. In general, the only fully adequate
             accounts of language acquisition are those that give a
             prominent role to children's comprehension of communicative
             function in everything from words to grammatical morphemes
             to complex syntactic constructions. © Walter de
             Gruyter.},
   Doi = {10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.183},
   Key = {fds351882}
}

@article{fds351883,
   Author = {Wittek, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {German-speaking children's productivity with syntactic
             constructions and case morphology: Local cues act
             locally},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {103-125},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723705049120},
   Abstract = {It has been proposed that children acquiring case-marking
             languages might be quicker to acquire certain constructions
             than children acquiring word order languages, because the
             cues involved in grammatical morphology are more 'local',
             whereas word order is an inherently distributed cue (Slobin,
             1982). In the current studies using nonce nouns and verbs,
             we establish that German-speaking children are not
             productive with passive and active transitive sentence-level
             constructions at an earlier age than English-speaking
             children; the majority of children learning both languages
             are not productive until after their third birthdays. In
             contrast, in the second and third studies reported here, the
             majority of German-speaking children were productive with
             nominative and accusative case marking inside NPs before
             their third birthdays - and these are of course the very
             same case markers centrally involved in passive and active
             transitive constructions. We conclude from these results
             that, whereas for some functions mastering local cues is all
             that is required, and this is fairly simple, in other cases,
             such as the case marking involved in sentence-level
             syntactic constructions, the mastery of local cues is only
             one part of the process of forming complex analogical
             relationships among utterances. Copyright © 2005 SAGE
             Publications.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0142723705049120},
   Key = {fds351883}
}

@article{fds351884,
   Author = {Diessel, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A new look at the acquisition of relative
             clauses},
   Journal = {Language},
   Volume = {81},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {882-906},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0169},
   Abstract = {This study reconsiders the acquisition of relative clauses
             based on data from two sentence-repetition tasks. Using
             materials modeled on the relative constructions of
             spontaneous child speech, we asked four-year-old English-
             and German-speaking children to repeat six different types
             of relative clauses. Although English and German relative
             clauses are structurally very different, the results were
             similar across studies: intransitive subject relatives
             caused fewer errors than transitive subject relatives and
             direct object relatives, which in turn caused fewer errors
             than indirect object relatives and oblique relatives;
             finally, genitive relatives caused by far the most problems.
             Challenging previous analyses in which the acquisition of
             relative clauses has been explained by the varying distance
             between filler and gap, we propose a multifactorial analysis
             in which the acquisition process is determined primarily by
             the similarity between the various types of relative clauses
             and their relationship to simple sentences.},
   Doi = {10.1353/lan.2005.0169},
   Key = {fds351884}
}

@article{fds351885,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M and Striano, T},
   Title = {Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing
             infants and children with autism},
   Journal = {Infancy},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {253-278},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0803_4},
   Abstract = {Three types of role reversal imitation were investigated in
             typically developing 12-and 18-month-old infants and in
             children with autism and other developmental delays. Many
             typically developing infants at both ages engaged in each of
             the 2 types of dyadic, body-oriented role reversal
             imitation: self-self reversals, in which the adult acted on
             herself and the child then acted on himself, and other-other
             reversals, in which the adult acted on the child and the
             child then acted back on the adult. However, 12-month-olds
             had more difficulty than 18-month-olds with triadic,
             object-mediated role reversals involving interactions around
             objects. There was little evidence of any type of role
             reversal imitation in children with autism. Positive
             relations were found between role reversal imitation and
             various measures of language development for 18-month-olds
             and children with autism. Copyright © 2005, Lawrence
             Erlbaum Associates, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15327078in0803_4},
   Key = {fds351885}
}

@article{fds352506,
   Author = {Riches, NG and Tomasello, M and Conti-Ramsden,
             G},
   Title = {Verb learning in children with SLI: frequency and spacing
             effects.},
   Journal = {Journal of speech, language, and hearing research :
             JSLHR},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1397-1411},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/097)},
   Abstract = {<h4>Purpose</h4>This study explored the effect of frequency
             (number of presentations), and spacing (period between
             presentations) on verb learning in children with specific
             language impairment (SLI). Children learn words more
             efficiently when presentations are frequent and
             appropriately spaced, and this study investigated whether
             children with SLI likewise benefit. Given that these
             children demonstrate greater frequency dependence and rapid
             forgetting of recently acquired words, an investigation of
             frequency and spacing in this population is especially
             warranted.<h4>Method</h4>Twenty-four children with SLI (mean
             age 5;6 [years;months]) and 24 language-matched control
             children (mean age 3;4) were taught novel verbs during play
             sessions. In a repeated measures design, 4 experimental
             conditions combined frequency (12 or 18 presentations) and
             spacing (all presentations in 1 session, or spread over 4
             days). Comprehension and production probes were administered
             after the final session and 1 week later.<h4>Results</h4>Although
             the children with SLI benefited significantly from frequent
             and widely spaced presentations, there were no significant
             effect in the control group. The language-impaired children
             showed rapid forgetting.<h4>Conclusions</h4>The frequency
             and spacing of presentations crucially affect the verb
             learning of children with SLI. A training regimen
             characterized by appropriately spaced intervals and moderate
             repetition will optimally benefit lexical
             learning.},
   Doi = {10.1044/1092-4388(2005/097)},
   Key = {fds352506}
}

@article{fds351886,
   Author = {Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind
             gestures in a hiding game.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {492-499},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00440.x},
   Abstract = {This study explored infants' ability to infer communicative
             intent as expressed in non-linguistic gestures. Sixty
             children aged 14, 18 and 24 months participated. In the
             context of a hiding game, an adult indicated for the child
             the location of a hidden toy by giving a communicative cue:
             either pointing or ostensive gazing toward the container
             containing the toy. To succeed in this task children had to
             do more than just follow the point or gaze to the target
             container. They also had to infer that the adult's behaviour
             was relevant to the situation at hand - she wanted to inform
             them that the toy was inside the container toward which she
             gestured. Children at all three ages successfully used both
             types of cues. We conclude that infants as young as 14
             months of age can, in some situations, interpret an adult
             behaviour as a relevant communicative act done for
             them.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00440.x},
   Key = {fds351886}
}

@article{fds351887,
   Author = {Wittek, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's sensitivity to listener knowledge and
             perceptual context in choosing referring
             expressions},
   Journal = {Applied Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {541-558},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050290},
   Abstract = {Speakers use different types of referring expressions
             depending on what the listener knows or is attending to; for
             example, they use pronouns for objects that are already
             present in the immediate discourse or perceptual context. In
             a first study we found that 2.5- and 3.5-year-old children
             are strongly influenced by their interlocutor's knowledge of
             a referent as expressed in her immediately preceding
             utterance. Specifically, when they are asked a question
             about a target object ("Where is the broom?"), they tend to
             use null references or pronouns to refer to that object ("On
             the shelf" or "It's on the shelf"); in contrast, when they
             are asked more general questions ("What do we need?") or
             contrast questions ("Do we need a mop?") that reveal no
             knowledge of the target object they tend to use lexical
             nouns ("A broom" or "No, a broom"). In a second study we
             found that children at around their second birthday are not
             influenced by immediately preceding utterances in this same
             way. Finally, in a third study we found that 2.5- and
             3.5-year-old children's choice of referring expressions is
             very little influenced by the physical arrangements of
             objects in the perceptual context, whether it is absent or
             needs to be distinguished from a close-by alternative, when
             they request a target object from a silent adult. These
             results are discussed in terms of children's emerging
             understanding of the knowledge and attentional states and
             other persons. © 2005 Cambridge University
             Press.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0142716405050290},
   Key = {fds351887}
}

@article{fds351888,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Behne, T and Moll,
             H},
   Title = {In search of the uniquely human},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {721-727},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05540123},
   Abstract = {As Bruner so eloquently points out, and Gauvain echoes,
             human beings are unique in their "locality." Individual
             groups of humans develop their own unique ways of
             symbolizing and doing things - and these can be very
             different from the ways of other groups, even those living
             quite nearby. Our attempt in the target article was to
             propose a theory of the social-cognitive and
             social-motivational bases of humans' ability and propensity
             to live in this local, that is, this cultural, way - which
             no other species does - focusing on such things as the
             ability to collaborate and to create shared material and
             symbolic artifacts. © 2005 Cambridge University
             Press.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X05540123},
   Key = {fds351888}
}

@article{fds351889,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Behne, T and Moll,
             H},
   Title = {Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of
             cultural cognition.},
   Journal = {The Behavioral and brain sciences},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {675-691},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x05000129},
   Abstract = {We propose that the crucial difference between human
             cognition and that of other species is the ability to
             participate with others in collaborative activities with
             shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality.
             Participation in such activities requires not only
             especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural
             learning, but also a unique motivation to share
             psychological states with others and unique forms of
             cognitive representation for doing so. The result of
             participating in these activities is species-unique forms of
             cultural cognition and evolution, enabling everything from
             the creation and use of linguistic symbols to the
             construction of social norms and individual beliefs to the
             establishment of social institutions. In support of this
             proposal we argue and present evidence that great apes (and
             some children with autism) understand the basics of
             intentional action, but they still do not participate in
             activities involving joint intentions and attention (shared
             intentionality). Human children's skills of shared
             intentionality develop gradually during the first 14 months
             of life as two ontogenetic pathways intertwine: (1) the
             general ape line of understanding others as animate,
             goal-directed, and intentional agents; and (2) a
             species-unique motivation to share emotions, experience, and
             activities with other persons. The developmental outcome is
             children's ability to construct dialogic cognitive
             representations, which enable them to participate in earnest
             in the collectivity that is human cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x05000129},
   Key = {fds351889}
}

@article{fds325197,
   Author = {Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Human-like social skills in dogs?},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {439-444},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003},
   Abstract = {Domestic dogs are unusually skilled at reading human social
             and communicative behavior--even more so than our nearest
             primate relatives. For example, they use human social and
             communicative behavior (e.g. a pointing gesture) to find
             hidden food, and they know what the human can and cannot see
             in various situations. Recent comparisons between canid
             species suggest that these unusual social skills have a
             heritable component and initially evolved during
             domestication as a result of selection on systems mediating
             fear and aggression towards humans. Differences in
             chimpanzee and human temperament suggest that a similar
             process may have been an important catalyst leading to the
             evolution of unusual social skills in our own species. The
             study of convergent evolution provides an exciting
             opportunity to gain further insights into the evolutionary
             processes leading to human-like forms of cooperation and
             communication.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003},
   Key = {fds325197}
}

@article{fds351890,
   Author = {Call, J and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Copying results and copying actions in the process of social
             learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children
             (Homo sapiens).},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {151-163},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0237-8},
   Abstract = {There is currently much debate about the nature of social
             learning in chimpanzees. The main question is whether they
             can copy others' actions, as opposed to reproducing the
             environmental effects of these actions using their own
             preexisting behavioral strategies. In the current study,
             chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo
             sapiens) were shown different demonstrations of how to open
             a tube-in both cases by a conspecific. In different
             experimental conditions, demonstrations consisted of (1)
             action only (the actions necessary to open the tube without
             actually opening it); (2) end state only (the open tube,
             without showing any actions); (3) both of these components
             (in a full demonstration); or (4) neither of these
             components (in a baseline condition). In the first three
             conditions subjects saw one of two different ways that the
             tube could open (break in middle; caps off ends). Subjects'
             behavior in each condition was assessed for how often they
             opened the tube, how often they opened it in the same
             location as the demonstrator, and how often they copied the
             demonstrator's actions or style of opening the tube. Whereas
             chimpanzees reproduced mainly the environmental results of
             the demonstrations (emulation), human children often
             reproduced the demonstrator's actions (imitation). Because
             the procedure used was similar in many ways to the procedure
             that Meltzoff (Dev Psych 31:1, 1995) used to study the
             understanding of others' unfulfilled intentions, the
             implications of these findings with regard to chimpanzees'
             understanding of others' intentions are also
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-004-0237-8},
   Key = {fds351890}
}

@article{fds351891,
   Author = {Kemp, N and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's knowledge of the "determiner" and
             "adjective" categories.},
   Journal = {Journal of speech, language, and hearing research :
             JSLHR},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {592-609},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/041)},
   Abstract = {Children's understanding of the grammatical categories of
             "determiner" and "adjective" was examined using 2 different
             methodologies. In Experiment 1, children heard novel nouns
             combined with either a or the. Few 2-year-olds, but nearly
             all 3- and 4-year-olds, subsequently produced the novel
             nouns with a different determiner from the modeled
             combination. Experiment 2 used a priming methodology.
             Children age 2, 3, 4, and 6 years repeated descriptions of
             pictures, before describing target pictures themselves. When
             the primes consisted of a varied determiner + noun, all age
             groups produced more determiner + noun descriptions. When
             the primes consisted of a determiner + adjective + noun,
             2-year-olds showed no priming. Three- to 6-year-olds showed
             item-specific priming, but only 6-year-olds (and to a
             limited extent 4-year-olds) showed both item-specific and
             structural priming. These results suggest that children
             build an understanding of determiners and adjectives
             gradually, perhaps from individual lexical items, over a
             number of years, and that pragmatic correctness may be
             attained particularly late.},
   Doi = {10.1044/1092-4388(2005/041)},
   Key = {fds351891}
}

@article{fds351892,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and
             around barriers.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {119},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {145-154},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.145},
   Abstract = {Following the gaze direction of conspecifics is an adaptive
             skill that enables individuals to obtain useful information
             about the location of food, predators, and group mates. In
             the current study, the authors compared the gaze-following
             skills of all 4 great ape species. In the 1st experiment, a
             human either looked to the ceiling or looked straight ahead.
             Individuals from all species reliably followed the human's
             gaze direction and sometimes even checked back when they
             found no target. In a 2nd experiment, the human looked
             behind some kind of barrier. Results showed that individuals
             from all species reliably put themselves in places from
             which they could see what the experimenter was looking at
             behind the barrier. These results support the hypothesis
             that great apes do not just orient to a target that another
             is oriented to, but they actually attempt to take the visual
             perspective of the other.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.145},
   Key = {fds351892}
}

@article{fds351893,
   Author = {Behne, T and Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of
             intentional action.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {41},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {328-337},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.328},
   Abstract = {Infants experienced a female adult handling them toys.
             Sometimes, however, the transaction failed, either because
             the adult was in various ways unwilling to give the toy
             (e.g., she teased the child with it or played with it
             herself) or else because she was unable to give it (e.g.,
             she accidentally dropped it). Infants at 9, 12, and 18
             months of age reacted with more impatience (e.g., reaching,
             looking away) when the adult was unwilling to give them the
             toy than when she was simply unable to give it.
             Six-month-olds, in contrast, showed no evidence of this
             differentiation. Because infants' behavioral responses were
             appropriately adapted to different kinds of intentional
             actions, and because the adult's actions sometimes produced
             results that did not match her goal (when having accidents
             or failed attempts), these findings provide especially rich
             evidence that infants first begin to understand
             goal-directed action at around 9 months of
             age.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.328},
   Key = {fds351893}
}

@article{fds351894,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Riedel, J and Call, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use
             social cues in an object choice task},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {69},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {11-18},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.008},
   Abstract = {Gaze following is a basic social cognitive skill with many
             potential benefits for animals that live in social groups.
             At least five primate species are known to follow the gaze
             of conspecifics, but there have been no studies on gaze
             following in other mammals. We investigated whether domestic
             goats can use the gaze direction of a conspecific as a cue
             to find food. They were able to do this, at a level
             comparable to that of primates. In a second experiment, we
             tested goats' ability to use gaze and other communicative
             cues given by a human in a so-called object choice
             situation. An experimenter hid food out of sight of the
             subject under one of two cups. After baiting the cup the
             experimenter indicated the location of the food to the
             subject by using different cues. The goats used
             communicative cues (touching and pointing) but not gaze by
             itself. Since domestic dogs are very skilled in this task,
             whereas wolves are not, one hypothesis is that the use of
             communicative cues in the object choice task is a
             side-effect of domestication. © 2004 The Association for
             the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.008},
   Key = {fds351894}
}

@article{fds351895,
   Author = {Matthews, D and Lieven, E and Theakston, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word
             order},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {121-136},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.08.001},
   Abstract = {Akhtar [Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring basic word order:
             Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure.
             Journal of Child Language, 26, 339-356] taught children
             novel verbs in ungrammatical word orders. Her results
             suggested that the acquisition of canonical word order is a
             gradual, data-driven process. The current study adapted this
             methodology, using English verbs of different frequencies,
             to test whether children's use of word order as a
             grammatical marker depends upon the frequency of the lexical
             items being ordered. Ninety-six children in two age groups
             (2;9 and 3;9) heard either high frequency, medium frequency
             or low frequency verbs that were modeled in SOV order.
             Children aged 2;9 who heard low frequency verbs were
             significantly more likely to adopt the weird word order than
             those who heard higher frequency verbs. Children aged 3;9
             preferred to use SVO order regardless of verb frequency.
             Furthermore, the younger children reverted to English word
             order using more arguments as verb frequency increased and
             used more pronouns than their older counterparts. This
             suggests that the ability to use English word order develops
             from lexically specific schemas formed around frequent,
             distributionally regular items (e.g. verbs, pronouns) into
             more abstract, productive schemas as experience of the
             language is accrued. © 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.08.001},
   Key = {fds351895}
}

@article{fds351896,
   Author = {Pika, S and Liebal, K and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The gestural communication of apes},
   Journal = {Gesture},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {1-2},
   Pages = {41-56},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/gest.5.1-2.05pik},
   Abstract = {Gestural communication of nonhuman primates may allow
             insight into the evolutionary scenario of human
             communication given the flexible use and learning of
             gestures as opposed to vocalizations. This paper provides an
             overview of the work on the gestural communication of apes
             with the focus on their repertoire, learning mechanisms, and
             the flexibility of gesture use during interactions with
             conspecifics. Although there is a variation between the
             species in the types and numbers of gestures performed, the
             influence of ecology, social structure and cognitive skills
             on their gestural repertoires is relatively restricted. As
             opposed to humans, apes do not use their gestures
             referentially nor do their gestures show the symbolic or
             conventionalized features of human gestural communication.
             However, since the gestural repertoires of apes are
             characterized by a high degree of individual variability and
             flexibility of use as opposed to their vocalizations it
             seems plausible that the gestures were the modality within
             which symbolic communication first evolved.},
   Doi = {10.1075/gest.5.1-2.05pik},
   Key = {fds351896}
}

@article{fds351897,
   Author = {Diessel, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Particle placement in early child language: A multifactorial
             analysis},
   Journal = {Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {89-112},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cllt.2005.1.1.89},
   Abstract = {Recent studies of the English verb particle construction
             have shown that particle placement varies with a variety of
             linguistic features, which seem to influence the speaker's
             choice of a particular position. The current study
             investigates whether children's use of the particle varies
             with the same features as in adult language. Using corpus
             data from two English-speaking children, we conducted a
             multifactorial analysis of six linguistic variables that are
             correlated with particle placement in adult language. Our
             analysis reveals significant associations between the
             position of the particle and two of the six variables, the
             NP type of the direct object and the meaning of the
             particle, suggesting that children as young as two years of
             age process at least some of the features that motivate
             particle placement in adult speakers. © Walter de
             Gruyter.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cllt.2005.1.1.89},
   Key = {fds351897}
}

@article{fds351898,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Carpenter, M},
   Title = {The emergence of social cognition in three young
             chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
             Development},
   Volume = {70},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {vii-132},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2005.00324.x},
   Abstract = {We report a series of 10 studies on the social-cognitive
             abilities of three young chimpanzees. The studies were all
             ones previously conducted with human infants. The
             chimpanzees were 1-5 years of age, had been raised mostly by
             humans, and were tested mostly directly by a familiar human
             experimenter. First, in a longitudinal investigation with
             repeated measurements from a social-cognitive test battery,
             the three young chimpanzees were similar in many ways to
             human infants; the major difference was a total lack of
             attempts to share attention with others either in joint
             attentional interactions or through declarative gestures.
             Second, in imitation-based tests of the understanding of
             intentional action, the chimpanzees, like human infants,
             showed an understanding of failed attempts and accidents;
             but they did not pay attention to the behavioral style of
             the actor or the actor's reasons for choosing a particular
             behavioral means. Third, in tests of their understanding of
             visual perception, the chimpanzees followed the gaze
             direction of a human to an out-of-sight location behind a
             barrier and gestured more to a human who could see them than
             to one who could not; but they showed no understanding that
             perceivers can focus their attention on one thing, or one
             aspect of a thing, within their perceptual fields for a
             reason. Finally, in tests of joint intentions and joint
             attention, the chimpanzees showed no ability to either
             reverse roles with a partner in a collaborative interaction
             or to set up a joint attentional framework for understanding
             the communicative intentions behind a pointing gesture.
             Taken together, these findings support the idea that the
             early ontogeny of human social cognition comprises two
             distinct trajectories, each with its own evolutionary
             history: one for understanding the basics of goal-directed
             action and perception, common to all apes, and another for
             sharing psychological states with others in collaborative
             acts involving joint intentions and attention, unique to the
             human species.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1540-5834.2005.00324.x},
   Key = {fds351898}
}

@article{fds351899,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of
             goals.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {F13-F20},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00385.x},
   Abstract = {In the context of an imitation game, 12- and 18-month-old
             infants saw an adult do such things as make a toy mouse hop
             across a mat (with sound effects). In one condition (House),
             the adult ended by placing the mouse in a toy house, whereas
             in another condition (No House) there was no house present
             at the final location. Infants at both ages usually simply
             put the mouse in the house (ignoring the hopping motion and
             sound effects) in the House condition, presumably because
             they interpreted the adult's action in terms of this final
             goal and so ignored the behavioral means. In contrast,
             infants copied the adult's action (both the hopping motion
             and the sound effects) when no house was present, presumably
             because here infants saw the action itself as the adult's
             only goal. From very early, infants' social learning is
             flexible: infants focus on and copy either the end or the
             means of an adult action as required by the
             context.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00385.x},
   Key = {fds351899}
}

@article{fds351900,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M and Striano, T},
   Title = {On tools and toys: how children learn to act on and pretend
             with 'virgin objects'.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {57-73},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00393.x},
   Abstract = {The focus of the present study was the role of cultural
             learning in infants' acquisition of pretense actions with
             objects. In three studies, 18- and 24-month-olds (n = 64)
             were presented with novel objects, and either pretense or
             instrumental actions were demonstrated with these. When
             children were then allowed to act upon the objects
             themselves, qualitatively similar patterns of cultural
             (imitative) learning both of pretend and of instrumental
             actions were observed, suggesting that both types of actions
             can be acquired in similar ways through processes of
             cultural learning involving one or another form of
             collective intentionality. However, both absolute imitation
             rates and creativity were lower in pretense compared to
             instrumental actions, suggesting that the collective
             intentionality that constitutes pretense is especially
             difficult for children to comprehend. An additional analysis
             of children's gazes to the experimenter during their actions
             revealed that 24-month-olds looked more often to the
             experimenter during pretense actions than during
             instrumental actions - suggesting that pretense is
             culturally learned in a similar fashion as practical
             actions, but that young children understand pretense as a
             more inherently social, intersubjective activity.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00393.x},
   Key = {fds351900}
}

@article{fds351901,
   Author = {Pika, S and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus):
             repertoire and use.},
   Journal = {American journal of primatology},
   Volume = {65},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {39-61},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20096},
   Abstract = {This article aims to provide an inventory of the
             communicative gestures used by bonobos (Pan paniscus), based
             on observations of subadult bonobos and descriptions of
             gestural signals and similar behaviors in wild and captive
             bonobo groups. In addition, we focus on the underlying
             processes of social cognition, including learning mechanisms
             and flexibility of gesture use (such as adjustment to the
             attentional state of the recipient). The subjects were seven
             bonobos, aged 1-8 years, living in two different groups in
             captivity. Twenty distinct gestures (one auditory, eight
             tactile, and 11 visual) were recorded. We found individual
             differences and similar degrees of concordance of the
             gestural repertoires between and within groups, which
             provide evidence that ontogenetic ritualization is the main
             learning process involved. There is suggestive evidence,
             however, that some form of social learning may be
             responsible for the acquisition of special gestures.
             Overall, the present study establishes that the gestural
             repertoire of bonobos can be characterized as flexible and
             adapted to various communicative circumstances, including
             the attentional state of the recipient. Differences from and
             similarities to the other African ape species are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ajp.20096},
   Key = {fds351901}
}

@article{fds351902,
   Author = {Namy, LL and Campbell, AL and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The changing role of iconicity in non-verbal symbol
             learning: A U-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of
             arbitrary gestures},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {37-57},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0501_3},
   Abstract = {This article reports 2 experiments examining the changing
             role of iconicity in symbol learning and its implications
             regarding the mechanisms supporting symbol-to-referent
             mapping. Experiment 1 compared 18- and 26-month-olds'
             mapping of iconic gestures (e.g., hopping gesture for a
             rabbit) vs. arbitrary gestures (e.g., dropping motion for a
             rabbit). Experiment 2 replicated this comparison with
             4-year-olds. All ages successfully mapped iconic gestures.
             Eighteen-month-olds and 4-year-olds but not 26-month-olds
             mapped arbitrary gestures, revealing a U-shaped
             developmental function. These findings imply that (a) there
             is no advantage for iconicity in early symbol learning and
             (b) the range of symbols mapped becomes more restricted at
             26 months, re-emerging more flexibly during the preschool
             years. We argue that the decline in arbitrary gesture
             learning is a function of developing appreciation of
             communicative conventions. We propose that the re-emergence
             of arbitrary gestures at 4 years is driven by a wider range
             of symbolic experiences, and enhanced sensitivity to others'
             communicative intent. Copyright © 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum
             Associates, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15327647jcd0501_3},
   Key = {fds351902}
}

@article{fds351903,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two hypotheses about primate cognition},
   Journal = {Deutsche Zeitschrift fur Philosophie},
   Volume = {52},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {585-601},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {December},
   Key = {fds351903}
}

@article{fds351904,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What kind of evidence could refute the UG hypothesis?:
             Commentary on Wunderlich},
   Journal = {Studies in Language},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {642-645},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/sl.28.3.13tom},
   Doi = {10.1075/sl.28.3.13tom},
   Key = {fds351904}
}

@article{fds351905,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {American journal of primatology},
   Volume = {64},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {377-396},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20087},
   Abstract = {Gestural communication in a group of 19 captive chimpanzees
             (Pan troglodytes) was observed, with particular attention
             paid to gesture sequences (combinations). A complete
             inventory of gesture sequences is reported. The majority of
             these sequences were repetitions of the same gestures, which
             were often tactile gestures and often occurred in play
             contexts. Other sequences combined gestures within a
             modality (visual, auditory, or tactile) or across
             modalities. The emergence of gesture sequences was ascribed
             to a recipient's lack of responsiveness rather than a
             premeditated combination of gestures to increase the
             efficiency of particular gestures. In terms of audience
             effects, the chimpanzees were sensitive to the attentional
             state of the recipient, and therefore used visually-based
             gestures mostly when others were already attending, as
             opposed to tactile gestures, which were used regardless of
             whether the recipient was attending or not. However, the
             chimpanzees did not use gesture sequences in which the first
             gesture served to attract the recipient's visual attention
             before they produced a second gesture that was
             visually-based. Instead, they used other strategies, such as
             locomoting in front of the recipient, before they produced a
             visually-based gesture.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ajp.20087},
   Key = {fds351905}
}

@article{fds351906,
   Author = {Maslen, RJC and Theakston, AL and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {A dense corpus study of past tense and plural
             overregularization in English.},
   Journal = {Journal of speech, language, and hearing research :
             JSLHR},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1319-1333},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/099)},
   Abstract = {In the "blocking-and-retrieval-failure" account of
             overregularization (OR; G. F. Marcus, 1995; G. F. Marcus et
             al., 1992), the claim that a symbolic rule generates regular
             inflection is founded on pervasively low past tense OR rates
             and the lack of a substantive difference between past tense
             and plural OR rates. Evidence of extended periods of OR in
             the face of substantial correct input (M. Maratsos, 2000)
             and of an initial period in which nouns are more likely to
             be overregularized than verbs (V. A. Marchman, K. Plunkett,
             & J. Goodman, 1997) casts doubt on the blocking account and
             suggests instead an interplay between type and token
             frequency effects that is more consistent with usage-based
             approaches (e.g., J. Bybee, 1995; K. Köpcke, 1998; K.
             Plunkett & V. Marchman, 1993). However, previous
             naturalistic studies have been limited by data that account
             for only 1-2% of child speech. The current study reports
             analyses of verb and noun ORs in a dense naturalistic corpus
             (1 child, 2;00.12-3;11.06 [years;months.days]) that captures
             8-10% of child speech and input. The data show (a) a marked
             difference in verb and noun OR rates; (b) evidence of a
             relationship between relative regular/irregular type
             frequencies and the onset and rate of past tense and plural
             ORs; (c) substantial OR periods for some verbs and nouns
             despite hundreds of correct tokens in child speech and
             input; and (d) a strong negative correlation between input
             token frequencies and OR rates for verbs and nouns. The
             implications of these findings for blocking and other
             accounts of OR are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1044/1092-4388(2004/099)},
   Key = {fds351906}
}

@article{fds351907,
   Author = {Bräuer, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the
             presence of barriers},
   Journal = {Applied Animal Behaviour Science},
   Volume = {88},
   Number = {3-4},
   Pages = {299-317},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.03.004},
   Abstract = {Previous studies have shown that dogs have developed a
             special sensitivity to the communicative signals and
             attentional states of humans. The aim of the current study
             was to further investigate what dogs know about the visual
             perception of humans and themselves. In the first two
             experiments we investigated whether dogs were sensitive to
             the properties of barriers as blocking the visual access of
             humans. We presented dogs with a situation in which a human
             forbade them to take a piece of food, but the type and
             orientation of the barrier allowed the dog to take the food
             undetected in some conditions. Dogs differentiated between
             effective and ineffective barriers, based on their
             orientation or the particular features of the barriers such
             as size or the presence of window. In the third study we
             investigated whether dogs know about what they themselves
             have seen. We presented subjects with two boxes and placed
             food in one of them. In the Seen condition the location of
             the food was shown to the dogs while in the Unseen condition
             dogs were prevented from seeing the destination of the food.
             Before selecting one of the boxes by pressing a lever, dogs
             had the opportunity to seek extra information regarding the
             contents of the boxes, which would be particularly useful in
             the condition in which they had not seen where the food was
             hidden. Dogs rarely used the opportunity to seek information
             about the contents of the box before making their choice in
             any condition. Therefore, we found no evidence suggesting
             that dogs have access to what they themselves have seen,
             which contrasts with the positive evidence about visual
             perspective taking in others from the first two experiments
             and previous studies. © 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.applanim.2004.03.004},
   Key = {fds351907}
}

@article{fds351908,
   Author = {Kaminski, J and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Body orientation and face orientation: two factors
             controlling apes' behavior from humans.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {216-223},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0214-2},
   Abstract = {A number of animal species have evolved the cognitive
             ability to detect when they are being watched by other
             individuals. Precisely what kind of information they use to
             make this determination is unknown. There is particular
             controversy in the case of the great apes because different
             studies report conflicting results. In experiment 1, we
             presented chimpanzees, orangutans, and bonobos with a
             situation in which they had to request food from a human
             observer who was in one of various attentional states. She
             either stared at the ape, faced the ape with her eyes
             closed, sat with her back towards the ape, or left the room.
             In experiment 2, we systematically crossed the observer's
             body and face orientation so that the observer could have
             her body and/or face oriented either towards or away from
             the subject. Results indicated that apes produced more
             behaviors when they were being watched. They did this not
             only on the basis of whether they could see the experimenter
             as a whole, but they were sensitive to her body and face
             orientation separately. These results suggest that body and
             face orientation encode two different types of information.
             Whereas face orientation encodes the observer's perceptual
             access, body orientation encodes the observer's disposition
             to transfer food. In contrast to the results on body and
             face orientation, only two of the tested subjects responded
             to the state of the observer's eyes.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-004-0214-2},
   Key = {fds351908}
}

@article{fds351909,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes
             revisited.},
   Journal = {Animal cognition},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {213-215},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0227-x},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10071-004-0227-x},
   Key = {fds351909}
}

@article{fds325198,
   Author = {Call, J and Hare, B and Carpenter, M and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {'Unwilling' versus 'unable': chimpanzees' understanding of
             human intentional action.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {488-498},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00368.x},
   Abstract = {Understanding the intentional actions of others is a
             fundamental part of human social cognition and behavior. An
             important question is therefore whether other animal
             species, especially our nearest relatives the chimpanzees,
             also understand the intentional actions of others. Here we
             show that chimpanzees spontaneously (without training)
             behave differently depending on whether a human is unwilling
             or unable to give them food Chimpanzees produced more
             behaviors and left the testing station earlier with an
             unwilling compared to an unable (but willing) experimenter
             These data together with other recent studies on
             chimpanzees' knowledge about others' visual perception show
             that chimpanzees know more about the intentional actions and
             perceptions of others than previously demonstrated},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00368.x},
   Key = {fds325198}
}

@article{fds325199,
   Author = {Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in
             cooperative cognitive tasks},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {68},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {571-581},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011},
   Abstract = {In a series of four experiments, chimpanzees, Pan
             troglodytes, were given two cognitive tasks, an object
             choice task and a discrimination task (based on location),
             each in the context of either cooperation or competition. In
             both tasks chimpanzees performed more skilfully when
             competing than when cooperating, with some evidence that
             competition with conspecifics was especially facilitatory in
             the discrimination location task. This is the first study to
             demonstrate a facilitative cognitive effect for competition
             in a single experimental paradigm. We suggest that
             chimpanzee cognitive evolution is best understood in its
             socioecological context. © 2004 The Association for the
             Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011},
   Key = {fds325199}
}

@article{fds351910,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Syntax or semantics? Response to Lidz et
             al.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {93},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {139-140},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.015},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.015},
   Key = {fds351910}
}

@article{fds351912,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The pragmatics of primate communication},
   Journal = {Psychologie Francaise},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {209-218},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2003.11.006},
   Abstract = {Pragmatics is about how individuals use their inventory of
             semiotic devices, the strategic choices they make, in
             particular acts of communication. An interesting question is
             the degree to which other animal species, especially our
             nearest primate relatives, employ pragmatic strategies in
             their vocal and gestural communication. Based on a review of
             the evidence, it is concluded in this essay that primate
             communication displays almost none of the pragmatic
             dimensions that characterize human linguistic communication.
             The most fundamental reason is that nonhuman animal
             communication does not really take place on the mental or
             intersubjective plane at all. It is directed at the behavior
             and emotional states of others, not at their attentional or
             mental states. © 2004 Publié par Elsvier SAS pour
             Société française de psychologie.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.psfr.2003.11.006},
   Key = {fds351912}
}

@article{fds351913,
   Author = {Liszkowski, U and Carpenter, M and Henning, A and Striano, T and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and
             interest.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {297-307},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x},
   Abstract = {Infants point for various motives. Classically, one such
             motive is declarative, to share attention and interest with
             adults to events. Recently, some researchers have questioned
             whether infants have this motivation. In the current study,
             an adult reacted to 12-month-olds' pointing in different
             ways, and infants' responses were observed. Results showed
             that when the adult shared attention and interest (i.e
             alternated gaze and emoted), infants pointed more frequently
             across trials and tended to prolong each point--presumably
             to prolong the satisfying interaction. However, when the
             adult emoted to the infant alone or looked only to the
             event, infants pointed less across trials and repeated
             points more within trials--presumably in an attempt to
             establish joint attention. Results suggest that
             12-month-olds point declaratively and understand that others
             have psychological states that can be directed and
             shared.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x},
   Key = {fds351913}
}

@article{fds351914,
   Author = {Rakoczy, H and Tomasello, M and Striano, T},
   Title = {Young children know that trying is not pretending: a test of
             the "behaving-as-if" construal of children's early concept
             of pretense.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {388-399},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.388},
   Abstract = {In 3 studies, young children were tested for their
             understanding of pretend actions. In Studies 1 and 2, pairs
             of superficially similar behaviors were presented to 26- and
             36-month-old children in an imitation game. In one case the
             behavior was marked as trying (signs of effort), and in the
             other case as pretending (signs of playfulness).
             Three-year-olds, and to some degree 2-year-olds, performed
             the real action themselves (or tried to really perform it)
             after the trying model, whereas after the pretense model,
             they only pretended. Study 3 ruled out a simple mimicking
             explanation by showing that children not only imitated
             differentially but responded differentially with appropriate
             productive pretending to pretense models and with
             appropriate productive tool use to trying models. The
             findings of the 3 studies demonstrate that by 2 to 3 years
             of age, children have a concept of pretense as a specific
             type of intentional activity.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.388},
   Key = {fds351914}
}

@article{fds351915,
   Author = {Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Training 2;6-year-olds to produce the transitive
             construction: the role of frequency, semantic similarity and
             shared syntactic distribution.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {48-55},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00322.x},
   Abstract = {Childers and Tomasello (2001) found that training 2
             1/2-year-olds on the English transitive construction greatly
             improves their performance on a post-test in which they must
             use novel verbs in that construction. In the current study,
             we replicated Childers and Tomasello's finding, but using a
             much lower frequency of transitive verbs and models in
             training. We also used novel verbs that were of a different
             semantic class to our training verbs, demonstrating that
             semantic homogeneity is not crucial for generalization. We
             also replicated the finding that 4-year-olds are
             significantly more productive than 2 1/2-year-olds with the
             transitive construction, with the new finding that this is
             also true for verbs of emission. In addition, 'shared
             syntactic distribution' of novel verb and training verbs was
             found to have no observable effect on the number of 2
             1/2-year-olds who were productive in the
             post-test.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00322.x},
   Key = {fds351915}
}

@article{fds351916,
   Author = {Moll, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind
             barriers.},
   Journal = {Developmental science},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {F1-F9},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00315.x},
   Abstract = {Infants follow the gaze direction of others from the middle
             of the first year of life. In attempting to determine how
             infants understand the looking behavior of adults, a number
             of recent studies have blocked the adult's line of sight in
             some way (e.g. with a blindfold or with a barrier). In
             contrast, in the current studies an adult looked behind a
             barrier which blocked the child's line of sight. Using two
             different control conditions and several different barrier
             types, 12- and 18-month-old infants locomoted a short
             distance in order to gain the proper viewing angle to follow
             an experimenter's gaze to locations behind barriers. These
             results demonstrate that, contra Butterworth, even
             12-month-old infants can follow gaze to locations outside of
             their current field of view. They also add to growing
             evidence that 12-month-olds have some understanding of the
             looking behaviors of others as an act of
             seeing.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00315.x},
   Key = {fds351916}
}

@article{fds351917,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Stahl, D},
   Title = {Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is
             enough?},
   Journal = {Journal of Child Language},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {101-121},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000903005944},
   Abstract = {There has been relatively little discussion in the field of
             child language acquisition about how best to sample from
             children's spontaneous speech, particularly with regard to
             quantitative issues. Here we provide quantitative
             information designed to help researchers make decisions
             about how best to sample children's speech for particular
             research questions (and/or how confident to be in existing
             analyses). We report theoretical analyses in which the major
             parameters are: (1) the frequency with which a phenomenon
             occurs in the real world, and (2) the temporal density with
             which a researcher samples the child's speech. We look at
             the influence of these two parameters in using spontaneous
             speech samples to estimate such things as: (a) the
             percentage of the real phenomenon actually captured, (b) the
             probability of capturing at least one target in any given
             sample, (c) the confidence we can have in estimating the
             frequency of occurrence of a target from a given sample, and
             (d) the estimated age of emergence of a target structure. In
             addition, we also report two empirical analyses of
             relatively infrequent child language phenomena, in which we
             sample in different ways from a relatively dense corpus (two
             children aged 2;0 to 3;0) and compare the different results
             obtained. Implications of these results for various issues
             in the study of child language acquisition are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0305000903005944},
   Key = {fds351917}
}

@article{fds351918,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Pika, S and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {To move or not to move: How apes adjust to the attentional
             state of others},
   Journal = {Interaction Studies},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {199-219},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/is.5.2.03lie},
   Abstract = {A previous observational study suggested that when faced
             with a partner with its back turned, chimpanzees tend to
             move around to the front of a non-attending partner and then
             gesture-rather than gesturing once to attract attention and
             then again to convey a specific intent.We investigated this
             preference experimentally by presenting six orangutans, five
             gorillas, nine chimpanzees, and four bonobos with a food
             begging situation in which we varied the body orientation of
             an experimenter (E) with respect to the subject (front vs.
             back) and the location of the food (in front or behind E).
             These manipulations allowed us to measure whether subjects
             preferred to move around to face E or to use signals to
             attract her attention before they begged for food. Results
             showed that all species moved around to face E and then
             produced visual gestures, instead of using tactile/ auditory
             gestures behind E to call her attention. Species differences
             were apparent particularly when the food and E were in
             different locations. Unlike gorillas and orangutans,
             chimpanzees and bonobos (from genus Pan) produced their
             gestures in front of E in all conditions, including that in
             which subjects had to leave the food behind to communicate
             with her. Implications of these results are discussed in the
             context of the evolution of social cognition in great apes.
             © John Benjamins Publishing Company.},
   Doi = {10.1075/is.5.2.03lie},
   Key = {fds351918}
}

@article{fds351919,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Learning through others},
   Journal = {Daedalus},
   Volume = {133},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {51-58},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/001152604772746693},
   Doi = {10.1162/001152604772746693},
   Key = {fds351919}
}

@article{fds351920,
   Author = {Liebal, K and Pika, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Social communication in siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus):
             use of gestures and facial expressions.},
   Journal = {Primates; journal of primatology},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {41-57},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10329-003-0063-7},
   Abstract = {The current study represents the first systematic
             investigation of the social communication of captive
             siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus). The focus was on
             intentional signals, including tactile and visual gestures,
             as well as facial expressions and actions. Fourteen
             individuals from different groups were observed and the
             signals used by individuals were recorded. Thirty-one
             different signals, consisting of 12 tactile gestures, 8
             visual gestures, 7 actions, and 4 facial expressions, were
             observed, with tactile gestures and facial expressions
             appearing most frequently. The range of the signal
             repertoire increased steadily until the age of six, but
             declined afterwards in adults. The proportions of the
             different signal categories used within communicative
             interactions, in particular actions and facial expressions,
             also varied depending on age. Group differences could be
             traced back mainly to social factors or housing conditions.
             Differences in the repertoire of males and females were most
             obvious in the sexual context. Overall, most signals were
             used flexibly, with the majority performed in three or more
             social contexts and almost one-third of signals used in
             combination with other signals. Siamangs also adjusted their
             signals appropriately for the recipient, for example, using
             visual signals most often when the recipient was already
             attending (audience effects). These observations are
             discussed in the context of siamang ecology, social
             structure, and cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10329-003-0063-7},
   Key = {fds351920}
}

@article{fds351921,
   Author = {Savage, C and Lieven, E and Theakston, A and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Testing the abstractness of children's linguistic
             representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic
             constructions in young children},
   Journal = {Developmental Science},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {557-567},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00312},
   Abstract = {The current studies used a priming methodology to assess the
             abstractness of children's early syntactic constructions. In
             the main study, 3-, 4- and 6-year-old children were asked to
             describe a prime picture by repeating either an active or a
             passive sentence, and then they were left to their own
             devices to describe a target picture. For half the children
             at each age, the prime sentences they repeated had high
             lexical overlap with the sentence they were likely to
             produce for the target, whereas for the other half there was
             very low lexical overlap between prime and target. The main
             result was that 6-year-old children showed both lexical and
             structural priming for both the active transitive and
             passive constructions, whereas 3- and 4-year-old children
             showed lexical priming only. This pattern of results would
             seem to indicate that 6-year-old children have relatively
             abstract representations of these constructions, whereas 3-
             and 4-year-old children have as an integral part of their
             representations certain specific lexical items, especially
             pronouns and some grammatical morphemes. In a second study
             it was found that children did not need to repeat the prime
             out loud in order to be primed - suggesting that the priming
             effect observed concerns not just peripheral production
             mechanisms but underlying linguistic representations common
             to comprehension and production. These results support the
             view that young children develop abstract linguistic
             representations gradually during the preschool
             years.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-7687.00312},
   Key = {fds351921}
}

@article{fds351922,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Haberl, K},
   Title = {Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is
             new for other persons.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {906-912},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.906},
   Abstract = {Infants at 12 and 18 months of age played with 2 adults and
             2 new toys. For a 3rd toy, however, 1 of the adults left the
             room while the child and the other adult played with it.
             This adult then returned, looked at all 3 toys aligned on a
             tray, showed great excitement ("Wow! Cool!"), and then
             asked, "Can you give it to me?' To retrieve the toy the
             adult wanted, infants had to (a) know that people attend to
             and get excited about new things and (b) identify what was
             new for the adult even though it was not new for them.
             Infants at both ages did this successfully, lending support
             to the hypothesis that 1-year-old infants possess a genuine
             understanding of other persons as intentional and
             attentional agents.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.906},
   Key = {fds351922}
}

@article{fds351923,
   Author = {Call, J and Bräuer, J and Kaminski, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the
             attentional state of humans.},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {117},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {257-263},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.257},
   Abstract = {Twelve domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) were given a series
             of trials in which they were forbidden to take a piece of
             visible food. In some trials, the human continued to look at
             the dog throughout the trial (control condition), whereas in
             others, the human (a) left the room, (b) turned her back,
             (c) engaged in a distracting activity, or (d) closed her
             eyes. Dogs behaved in clearly different ways in most of the
             conditions in which the human did not watch them compared
             with the control condition, in which she did. In particular,
             when the human looked at them, dogs retrieved less food,
             approached it in a more indirect way, and sat (as opposed to
             laid down) more often than in the other conditions. Results
             are discussed in terms of domestic dogs' social-cognitive
             skills and their unique evolutionary and ontogenetic
             histories.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.257},
   Key = {fds351923}
}

@article{fds351924,
   Author = {Theakston, AL and Lieven, EVM and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The role of the input in the acquisition of third person
             singular verbs in English.},
   Journal = {Journal of speech, language, and hearing research :
             JSLHR},
   Volume = {46},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {863-877},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/067)},
   Abstract = {During the early stages of language acquisition, children
             pass through a stage of development when they produce both
             finite and nonfinite verb forms in finite contexts (e.g.,
             "it go there," "it goes there"). Theorists who assume that
             children operate with an abstract understanding of tense and
             agreement marking from the beginnings of language use tend
             to explain this phenomenon in terms of either performance
             limitations in production (e.g., V. Valian, 1991) or the
             optional use of finite forms in finite contexts due to a
             lack of knowledge that tense and agreement marking is
             obligatory (the optional infinitive hypothesis; K. Wexler,
             1994, 1996). An alternative explanation, however, is that
             children's use of nonfinite forms is based on the presence
             of questions in the input ("Where does it go?") where the
             grammatical subject is immediately followed by a nonfinite
             verb form. To compare these explanations, 2 groups of 24
             children aged between 2 years 6 months and 3 years were
             exposed to 6 known and 3 novel verbs produced in either
             declaratives or questions or in both declaratives and
             questions. The children were then questioned to elicit use
             of the verbs in either finite or nonfinite contexts. The
             results show that for novel verbs, the children's patterns
             of verb use were closely related to the patterns of verb use
             modeled in the language to which they were exposed. For
             known verbs, there were no differences in the children's use
             of individual verbs, regardless of the specific patterns of
             verb use modeled in the language they heard. The
             implications of these findings for theories of early verb
             use are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1044/1092-4388(2003/067)},
   Key = {fds351924}
}

@article{fds351925,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Akhtar, N},
   Title = {What paradox? A response to Naigles (2002).},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {88},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {317-323},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00048-9},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00048-9},
   Key = {fds351925}
}

@article{fds351926,
   Author = {Lohmann, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The role of language in the development of false belief
             understanding: a training study.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1130-1144},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00597},
   Abstract = {The current study used a training methodology to determine
             whether different kinds of linguistic interaction play a
             causal role in children's development of false belief
             understanding. After 3 training sessions, 3-year-old
             children improved their false belief understanding both in a
             training condition involving perspective-shifting discourse
             about deceptive objects (without mental state terms) and in
             a condition in which sentential complement syntax was used
             (without deceptive objects). Children did not improve in a
             condition in which they were exposed to deceptive objects
             without accompanying language. Children showed most
             improvement in a condition using both perspective-shifting
             discourse and sentential complement syntax, suggesting that
             each of these types of linguistic experience plays an
             independent role in the ontogeny of false belief
             understanding.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-8624.00597},
   Key = {fds351926}
}

@article{fds351927,
   Author = {Pika, S and Liebal, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla):
             gestural repertoire, learning, and use.},
   Journal = {American journal of primatology},
   Volume = {60},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {95-111},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10097},
   Abstract = {In the present study we investigated the gestural
             communication of gorillas (Gorilla gorilla). The subjects
             were 13 gorillas (1-6 years old) living in two different
             groups in captivity. Our goal was to compile the gestural
             repertoire of subadult gorillas, with a special focus on
             processes of social cognition, including attention to
             individual and developmental variability, group variability,
             and flexibility of use. Thirty-three different gestures (six
             auditory, 11 tactile, and 16 visual gestures) were recorded.
             We found idiosyncratic gestures, individual differences, and
             similar degrees of concordance between and within groups, as
             well as some group-specific gestures. These results provide
             evidence that ontogenetic ritualization is the main learning
             process involved, but some form of social learning may also
             be responsible for the acquisition of special gestures. The
             present study establishes that gorillas have a multifaceted
             gestural repertoire, characterized by a great deal of
             flexibility with accommodations to various communicative
             circumstances, including the attentional state of the
             recipient. The possibility of assigning Seyfarth and
             Cheney's [1997] model for nonhuman primate vocal development
             to the development of nonhuman primate gestural
             communication is discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1002/ajp.10097},
   Key = {fds351927}
}

@article{fds326035,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J and Hare, B},
   Title = {Chimpanzees versus humans: It's not that
             simple},
   Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {239-240},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00107-4},
   Doi = {10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00107-4},
   Key = {fds326035}
}

@article{fds351928,
   Author = {Lieven, E and Behrens, H and Speares, J and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Early syntactic creativity: a usage-based
             approach.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {333-370},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000903005592},
   Abstract = {The aim of the current study was to determine the degree to
             which a sample of one child's creative utterances related to
             utterances that the child previously produced. The
             utterances to be accounted for were all of the intelligible,
             multi-word utterances produced by the child in a single hour
             of interaction with her mother early in her third year of
             life (at age 2;1.11). We used a high-density database
             consisting of 5 hours of recordings per week together with a
             maternal diary for the previous 6 weeks. Of the 295
             multi-word utterances on tape, 37% were 'novel' in the sense
             that they had not been said in their entirety before. Using
             a morpheme-matching method, we identified the way(s) in
             which each novel utterance differed from its closest match
             in the preceding corpus. In 74% of the cases we required
             only one operation to match the previous utterance and the
             great majority of these consisted of the substitution of a
             word (usually a noun) into a previous utterance or schema.
             Almost all the other single-operation utterances involved
             adding a word onto the beginning or end of a previous
             utterance. 26% of the novel, multi-word utterances required
             more than one operation to match the closest previous
             utterance, although many of these only involved a
             combination of the two operations seen for the
             single-operation utterances. Some others were, however, more
             complex to match. The results suggest that the relatively
             high degree of creativity in early English child language
             could be at least partially based upon entrenched schemas
             and a small number of simple operations to modify them. We
             discuss the implications of these results for the interplay
             in language production between strings registered in memory
             and categorial knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000903005592},
   Key = {fds351928}
}

@article{fds325200,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J and Hare, B},
   Title = {Chimpanzees understand psychological states - The question
             is which ones and to what extent},
   Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {153-156},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00035-4},
   Abstract = {New data suggest that relatively drastic revisions are
             needed in our theoretical accounts of what other animal
             species understand about the psychological states of others.
             Specifically, chimpanzees seem to understand some things
             about what others do and do not see, or have and have not
             seen in the immediate past, as well as some things about
             others' goal-directed activities. This is especially so in
             competitive situations. They clearly do not have a
             human-like theory of mind, however, and so the challenge is
             to specify precisely how ape and human social cognition are
             similar and different.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00035-4},
   Key = {fds325200}
}

@article{fds351929,
   Author = {Childers, JB and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Children extend both words and non-verbal actions to novel
             exemplars},
   Journal = {Developmental Science},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {185-190},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00270},
   Abstract = {Markson and Bloom (1997) found that some learning processes
             involved in children's acquisition of a new word are also
             involved in their acquisition of a new fact. They argued
             that these findings provided evidence against a
             domain-specific system for word learning. However, Waxman
             and Booth (2000) found that whereas children quite readily
             extend newly learned words to novel exemplars within a
             category, they do not do this with newly learned facts. They
             therefore argued that because children did not extend some
             facts in a principled way, word learning and fact learning
             may result from different domain-specific processes. In the
             current study, we argue that facts are a poor comparison in
             this argument since facts vary in whether they are tied to
             particular individuals. A more appropriate comparison is a
             conventional non-verbal action on an object - 'what we do
             with things like this' - since they are routinely
             generalized categorically to new objects. Our study shows
             that 2 1/2-year-old children extend novel non-verbal actions
             to new objects in the same way that they extend novel words
             to new objects. The findings provide support for the view
             that word learning represents a unique configuration of more
             general learning processes.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-7687.00270},
   Key = {fds351929}
}

@article{fds366601,
   Author = {Lohmann, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Language and social understanding: Commentary on Nelson et
             al.},
   Journal = {Human Development},
   Volume = {46},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {47-50},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000067778},
   Doi = {10.1159/000067778},
   Key = {fds366601}
}

@article{fds326346,
   Author = {Hare, B and Addessi, E and Call, J and Tomasello, M and Visalberghi,
             E},
   Title = {Do capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, know what conspecifics do
             and do not see?},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {65},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {131-142},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.2017},
   Abstract = {Capuchin monkeys were tested in five experiments in which
             two individuals competed over food. When given a choice
             between retrieving a piece of food that was visible or
             hidden from the dominant, subordinate animals preferred to
             retrieve hidden food. This preference is consistent with the
             hypotheses that either (1) the subordinate knew what the
             dominant could and could not see or (2) the subordinate was
             monitoring the behaviour of the dominant and avoiding the
             piece of food that it approached. To test between these
             alternatives, we released subordinates with a slight head
             start forcing them to make their choice (between a piece of
             food hidden or visible to the dominant) before the dominant
             entered the area. Unlike chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes,
             subordinates that were given a head start did not
             preferentially approach hidden pieces of food first.
             Therefore, our experiments provide little support for the
             hypothesis that capuchin monkeys are sensitive to what
             another individual does or does not see. We compare our
             results with those obtained with chimpanzees in the same
             paradigm and discuss the evolution of primate social
             cognition. © 2003 The Association for the Study of Animal
             Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1006/anbe.2002.2017},
   Key = {fds326346}
}

@article{fds351930,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Rakoczy, H},
   Title = {What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared
             to collective intentionality},
   Journal = {Mind and Language},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {121-147},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00217},
   Abstract = {It is widely believed that what distinguishes the social
             cognition of humans from that of other animals is the
             belief-desire psychology of four-year-old children and
             adults (so-called theory of mind). We argue here that this
             is actually the second ontogenetic step in uniquely human
             social cognition. The first step is one year old children's
             understanding of persons as intentional agents, which
             enables skills of cultural learning and shared
             intentionality. This initial step is 'the real thing' in the
             sense that it enables young children to participate in
             cultural activities using shared, perspectival symbols with
             a conventional/normative/reflective dimension - for example,
             linguistic communication and pretend play - thus
             inaugurating children's understanding of things mental.
             Understanding beliefs and participating in collective
             intentionality at four years of age - enabling the
             comprehension of such things as money and marriage - results
             from several years of engagement with other persons in
             perspective-shifting and reflective discourse containing
             propositional attitude constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1468-0017.00217},
   Key = {fds351930}
}

@article{fds351931,
   Author = {Cameron-Faulkner, T and Lieven, E and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {A construction based analysis of child directed
             speech},
   Journal = {Cognitive Science},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {843-873},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsci.2003.06.001},
   Abstract = {The child directed speech of twelve English-speaking mothers
             was analyzed in terms of utterance-level constructions.
             First, the mothers' utterances were categorized in terms of
             general constructional categories such as Wh-questions,
             copulas and transitives. Second, mothers' utterances within
             these categories were further specified in terms of the
             initial words that framed the utterance, item-based phrases
             such as Are you ..., I'll ..., It's ..., Let's ..., What did
             .... The findings were: (i) overall, only about 15% of all
             maternal utterances had SVO form (most were questions,
             imperatives, copulas, and fragments); (ii) 51% of all
             maternal utterances began with one of 52 item-based phrases,
             mostly consisting of two words or morphemes (45% began with
             one of just 17 words); and (iii) children used many of these
             same item-based phrases, in some cases at a rate that
             correlated highly with their own mother's frequency of use.
             We suggest that analyses of adult-child linguistic
             interaction should take into account not just general
             constructional categories, but also the item-based
             constructions that adults and children use and the frequency
             with which they use them. © 2003 Cognitive Science Society,
             Inc. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cogsci.2003.06.001},
   Key = {fds351931}
}

@article{fds351932,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Things are what they do: Katherine Nelson's functional
             approach to language and cognition},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {5-19},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0301_2},
   Abstract = {This article attempts to summarize Katherine Nelson's
             theoretical and empirical contributions to the ontogenetic
             study of language and cognition. Nelson's approach has
             consistently emphasized the function of language and
             linguistic concepts in children's larger conceptual and
             social lives and, conversely, how children's emerging
             understanding of the function of linguistic symbols in
             larger conceptual and social structures makes language
             acquisition possible in the first place. This approach has
             led to an especially fruitful body of theoretical and
             empirical work. Copyright © 2002, Lawrence Erlbaum
             Associates, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/S15327647JCD0301_2},
   Key = {fds351932}
}

@article{fds325201,
   Author = {Hare, B and Brown, M and Williamson, C and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {The domestication of social cognition in
             dogs.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {298},
   Number = {5598},
   Pages = {1634-1636},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1072702},
   Abstract = {Dogs are more skillful than great apes at a number of tasks
             in which they must read human communicative signals
             indicating the location of hidden food. In this study, we
             found that wolves who were raised by humans do not show
             these same skills, whereas domestic dog puppies only a few
             weeks old, even those that have had little human contact, do
             show these skills. These findings suggest that during the
             process of domestication, dogs have been selected for a set
             of social-cognitive abilities that enable them to
             communicate with humans in unique ways.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.1072702},
   Key = {fds325201}
}

@article{fds351933,
   Author = {Childers, JB and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional
             actions from massed or distributed exposures.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {967-978},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.967},
   Abstract = {Two-year-old children were taught either 6 novel nouns, 6
             novel verbs, or 6 novel actions over 1 month. In each
             condition, children were exposed to some items in massed
             presentations (on a single day) and some in distributed
             presentations (over the 2 weeks). Children's comprehension
             and production was tested at 3 intervals after training. In
             comprehension, children learned all types of items in all
             training conditions at all retention intervals. For
             production, the main findings were that (a) production was
             better for nonverbal actions than for either word type, (b)
             children produced more new nouns than verbs, (c) production
             of words was better following distributed than massed
             exposure, and (d) time to testing (immediate, 1 day, 1 week)
             did not affect retention. A follow-up study showed that the
             most important timing variable was the number of different
             days of exposure, with more days facilitating production.
             Results are discussed in terms of 2 key issues: (a) the
             domain-generality versus domain-specificity of processes of
             word learning and (b) the relative ease with which children
             learn nouns versus verbs.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.967},
   Key = {fds351933}
}

@article{fds351934,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A new false belief test for 36-month-olds},
   Journal = {British Journal of Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {393-420},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151002320620316},
   Abstract = {We report two studies that suggest that some 36-month-old
             (and younger) children understand others' false beliefs. In
             the false belief conditions, children and two adults (E1 and
             E2) watched as an object was put into a container. E1 left
             the room, and E2 switched that object with another. E1
             returned, expressed her desire for the object, and struggled
             to open the container (without succeeding). She spied both
             objects across the room and said to the child. 'Oh, there it
             is. Can you get it for me?' In other conditions, the object
             was not switched or E1 witnessed the switch. Other
             variations included using a novel word for the object and
             removing the 'pull of the real'. Measures of children's
             latency and uncertainty were taken to determine whether
             those who were correct on the false belief tasks were
             guessing (luckily) or truly understanding. Results showed
             that between one-third and almost two-thirds of children
             took account of the adult's false belief when deciding which
             object the adult was requesting. We compare this task with
             other implicit and explicit tasks. We also conclude that
             certain task demands (e.g. the pull of the real) in
             traditional theory of mind assessments are
             unnecessary.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151002320620316},
   Key = {fds351934}
}

@article{fds351935,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to
             imitatively learn a complex task.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {73},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1431-1441},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00481},
   Abstract = {This study investigated children's understanding of others'
             intentions in a social learning context. Specifically, it
             investigated whether knowing an adult's prior intention
             before the adult gives a demonstration influences what
             children learn from the demonstration. In the five main
             experimental conditions, ninety-six 2-year-old children
             watched as an experimenter (E) pulled out a pin and opened
             the door of a box. Children in two No Prior Intention
             conditions saw this demonstration alone or paired with an
             irrelevant action. Children in three Prior Intention
             conditions knew what E was trying to do before the
             demonstration: they first saw E either attempt
             unsuccessfully to open the door, or visit and open several
             other containers, or they first saw that the door opened.
             Children opened the box themselves more often in each of
             these three conditions than in the two No Prior Intention
             conditions, even though children in all five conditions saw
             the exact same demonstration of how to open the
             box.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-8624.00481},
   Key = {fds351935}
}

@article{fds351936,
   Author = {Wittek, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {German children's productivity with tense morphology: the
             Perfekt (present perfect).},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {567-589},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000902005147},
   Abstract = {Two nonce-word studies examined German-speaking children's
             productivity with the Perfekt (present perfect) from 2;6 to
             3;6. The German Perfekt consists of the past participle of
             the main verb and an inflected form of an auxiliary (either
             haben 'have' or sein 'be'). In Study 1, nonce verbs were
             either introduced in the infinitival form, and children
             (seventy-two children, aged 2;6 to 3;6) were tested on their
             ability to produce the Perfekt, or introduced in the
             Perfekt, and children were tested on their ability to
             produce the infinitive. In Study 2 twenty-four children aged
             3;6 were given the past participle form of nonce verbs to
             see if they could supply the appropriate auxiliary (based
             mainly on verb semantics). The results were that many
             children as young as 2;6 used past participles productively
             (more than used infinitival forms productively), but all
             children had much difficulty in supplying both auxiliaries
             appropriately. The current findings suggest that mastery of
             the Perfekt construction as a whole does not take place
             before the age of four and that frequency of exposure is an
             important factor in determining the age at which children
             acquire grammatical constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000902005147},
   Key = {fds351936}
}

@article{fds351937,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Abbot-Smith, K},
   Title = {A tale of two theories: response to Fisher.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {207-214},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00172-x},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00172-x},
   Key = {fds351937}
}

@article{fds351938,
   Author = {Childers, JB and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The role of pronouns in young children's acquisition of the
             English transitive construction.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {739-748},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.37.6.739},
   Abstract = {Two studies investigating the linguistic representations
             underlying English-speaking 2 1/2-year-olds' production of
             transitive utterances are reported. The first study was a
             training study in which half the children heard utterances
             with full nouns as agent and patient, and half the children
             heard utterances with both pronouns (i.e., He's [verb]-ing
             it) and also full nouns. In subsequent testing, only
             children who had been trained with pronouns and nouns were
             able to produce a transitive utterance creatively with a
             nonce verb. The second study reported an analogous set of
             findings, but in comprehension. Together, the results of
             these 2 studies suggest that English-speaking children build
             many of their early linguistic constructions around certain
             specific lexical or morphological items and patterns,
             perhaps especially around particular pronoun
             configurations.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0012-1649.37.6.739},
   Key = {fds351938}
}

@article{fds351939,
   Author = {Campbell, AL and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The acquisition of English dative constructions},
   Journal = {Applied Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {253-267},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716401002065},
   Abstract = {We analyzed the three main types of English dative
             constructions - the double-object dative, the to dative, and
             the for dative - in the spontaneous speech of seven children
             from the age of 1;6 to 5;0. The main findings were as
             follows. First, the double-object dative was acquired by
             most of the children before either of the prepositional
             datives; this was attributed to the greater frequency with
             which children heard this construction with individual
             verbs. Second, the verbs children used with these
             constructions were not only the adult prototypical ones, but
             also a number of the less prototypical ones; again, this was
             very likely due to their frequency and saliency in the
             language children heard. Third, no support was found for
             Ninio's (1999) analysis of the emergence of constructions in
             terms of a single "pathbreaking" verb; rather, children
             began using the double-object dative with many different
             verbs and did not follow the trajectory proposed by Ninio
             (i.e., a single verb is used for some months before an
             "explosion" of new verbs is introduced in the construction).
             Finally, most of the verbs initially used in the three
             dative constructions were first used in other constructions
             (e.g., a simple transitive); this was even true for some
             obligatory datives, such as give and show. The current
             results provide a starting point for determining the
             underlying representations for the different kinds of dative
             constructions and for explicating how children understand
             the interrelations among these and other
             constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0142716401002065},
   Key = {fds351939}
}

@article{fds325202,
   Author = {Hare, B and Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {61},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {139-151},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1518},
   Abstract = {We conducted three experiments on social problem solving by
             chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. In each experiment a
             subordinate and a dominant individual competed for food,
             which was placed in various ways on the subordinate's side
             of two opaque barriers. In some conditions dominants had not
             seen the food hidden, or food they had seen hidden was moved
             elsewhere when they were not watching (whereas in control
             conditions they saw the food being hidden or moved). At the
             same time, subordinates always saw the entire baiting
             procedure and could monitor the visual access of their
             dominant competitor as well. If subordinates were sensitive
             to what dominants did or did not see during baiting, they
             should have preferentially approached and retrieved the food
             that dominants had not seen hidden or moved. This is what
             they did in experiment 1 when dominants were either
             uninformed or misinformed about the food's location. In
             experiment 2 subordinates recognized, and adjusted their
             behaviour accordingly, when the dominant individual who
             witnessed the hiding was replaced with another dominant
             individual who had not witnessed it, thus demonstrating
             their ability to keep track of precisely who has witnessed
             what. In experiment 3 subordinates did not choose
             consistently between two pieces of hidden food, one of which
             dominants had seen hidden and one of which they had not seen
             hidden. However, their failure in this experiment was likely
             to be due to the changed nature of the competition under
             these circumstances and not to a failure of social-cognitive
             skills. These findings suggest that at least in some
             situations (i.e. competition with conspecifics) chimpanzees
             know what conspecifics have and have not seen (do and do not
             know), and that they use this information to devise
             effective social-cognitive strategies. © 2001 The
             Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1006/anbe.2000.1518},
   Key = {fds325202}
}

@article{fds325570,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Hare, B and Fogleman, T},
   Title = {The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan
             troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca
             mulatta},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {61},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {335-343},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1598},
   Abstract = {Primates follow the gaze direction of conspecifics to
             outside objects. We followed the ontogeny of this
             social-cognitive skill for two species: rhesus macaques and
             chimpanzees, in the first two experiments, using both a
             cross-sectional and a longitudinal design, we exposed
             individuals of different ages to a human looking in a
             specified direction. Rhesus infants first began reliably to
             follow the direction of this gaze at the end of the early
             infancy period, at about 5.5 months of age. Chimpanzees did
             not reliably follow human gaze until 3-4 years; this
             corresponds to the latter part of the late infancy period
             for this species. In the third experiment we exposed
             individuals of the same two species to a human repeatedly
             looking to the same location (with no special object at that
             location) to see if subjects would learn to ignore the
             looks. Only adults of the two species diminished their
             gaze-following behaviour over trials. This suggests that in
             the period between infancy and adulthood individuals of both
             species come to integrate their gaze-following skills with
             their more general social-cognitive knowledge about other
             animate beings and their behaviour, and so become able to
             deploy their gaze-following skills in a more flexible
             manner. © 2001 The Association for the Study of Animal
             Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1006/anbe.2000.1598},
   Key = {fds325570}
}

@article{fds351940,
   Author = {Diessel, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Development of Relative Clauses in Spontaneous Child
             Speech},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1-2},
   Pages = {131-151},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2001.006},
   Abstract = {This study examines the development of relative clauses in
             the speech of four English-speaking children between 1;9 and
             5;2 years of age. It is shown that the earliest relative
             clauses occur in presentational constructions that express a
             single proposition in two finite clauses. Starting from such
             simple sentences, children gradually learn the use of more
             complex constructions in which the relative clause modifes
             the noun of a full-edged main clause. Five factors are
             considered that might contribute to the development of
             relative clauses in spontaneous child speech: (1) the
             ambient language, (2) the formulaic character of the main
             clause, (3) the information structure of the whole
             utterance, (4) the communicative function of presentational
             relatives, and (5) the limited processing capacity of young
             children. © 2001, 2000 by Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cogl.2001.006},
   Key = {fds351940}
}

@article{fds351941,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {First steps toward a usage-based theory of language
             acquisition},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1-2},
   Pages = {61-82},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2001.012},
   Abstract = {Usage-based models of language focus on the specific
             communicative events in which people learn and use language.
             In these models, the psycholinguistic units with which
             individuals operate are determined not by theoretical fiat
             but by observation of actual language use in actual
             communicative events. This data-based approach make these
             models especially congenial for the analysis of children's
             language, since children do not learn and use the same units
             as adults. In this paper I employ a usage-based model of
             language to argue for five fundamental facts about child
             language acquisition: (1) the primary psycholinguistic unit
             of child language acquisition is the utterance, which has as
             its foundation the expression and understanding of
             communicative intentions; (2) early in their language
             development children are attempting to reproduce not adult
             words but whole adult utterances; (3) children's earliest
             utterances are almost totally concrete in the sense that
             they are instantiations of item-based schemas or
             constructions; (4) abstractions result from children
             generalizing across the type variation they observe at
             particular “slots” in otherwise recurrent tokens of the
             same utterance; and (5) children create novel utterances for
             themselves via usage-based syntactic operations in which
             they begin with an utterance-level schema and then modify
             that schema for the exigencies of the particular
             communicative situation (usage event) at hand. © 2001, 2000
             by Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cogl.2001.012},
   Key = {fds351941}
}

@article{fds351942,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cultural Transmission:A View from Chimpanzees and Human
             Infants},
   Journal = {Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {135-146},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022101032002002},
   Abstract = {Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways
             that other primates are not, as evidenced most clearly by
             the fact that only human cultural traditions accumulate
             modifications over historical time (the ratchet effect). The
             key adaptation is one that enables individuals to understand
             other individuals as intentional agents like the self. This
             species-unique form of social cognition emerges in human
             ontogeny at around 1 year of age as infants begin to engage
             with other persons in various kinds of joint attentional
             activities involving gaze following, social referencing, and
             gestural communication. Young children–s joint attentional
             skills then engender some uniquely powerful forms of
             cultural learning, enabling the acquisition of language,
             discourse skills, tool use practices, and many other
             conventional activities. These novel forms of cultural
             learning allow human beings to pool their cognitive
             resources both contemporaneously and over historical time in
             ways that are unique in the animal kingdom. © 2015, Sage
             Publications. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0022022101032002002},
   Key = {fds351942}
}

@article{fds351943,
   Author = {Diessel, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A
             corpus-based analysis},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {97-142},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cogl.12.2.97},
   Abstract = {This article examines the development of finite complement
             clauses in the speech of seven English-speaking children
             aged 1;2 to 5;2.It shows that in most of children's complex
             utterances that seem to include a finite complement clause,
             the main clause does not express a full proposition; rather,
             it functions as an epistemic marker, attention getter, or
             marker of illocutionary force. The whole construction thus
             contains only a single proposition expressed by the apparent
             complement clause. As children grow older, some of the
             “main clauses” become more substantial and new
             complement-taking verbs emerge that occur with truly
             embedded complement clauses. However, since the use of these
             constructions is limited to only a few verbs, we argue that
             they are not yet licensed by a general schema or rule;
             rather, they are “constructional islands” organized
             around individual verbs. © 2001, Walter de Gruyter. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cogl.12.2.97},
   Key = {fds351943}
}

@article{fds351944,
   Author = {Pika, S and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {'Separating the wheat from the chaff': A novel food
             processing technique in captive Gorillas (Gorilla g.
             gorilla)},
   Journal = {Primates},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {167-170},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02558144},
   Abstract = {Observation of a novel food processing technique is reported
             for captive zoo gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla). It is similar
             in function to that of Japanese macaques' wheat placer
             mining behaviour and consists of puffing/blowing air with
             the mouth onto a mixture of oat grains and chaff in order to
             separate out the oat grains. Three females in two of four
             groups regularly use this behaviour. Other individuals in
             these groups or individuals of the two other groups in the
             same zoo do not use it. However, a very similar behaviour
             has been observed in three other individuals in a gorilla
             group of another zoo. The existence of this technique in
             spatially separated groups implies that multiple individuals
             have invented it for themselves. The possible role of social
             transmission is still to be investigated.},
   Doi = {10.1007/BF02558144},
   Key = {fds351944}
}

@article{fds351945,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Could we please lose the mapping metaphor,
             please?},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1119-1120},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01390131},
   Abstract = {Although Bloom gives more credit to social cognition (mind
             reading) than do most other theorists of word learning, he
             does not go far enough. He still relies fundamentally on a
             learning process of association (or mapping), neglecting the
             joint attentional and cultural learning skills from which
             linguistic communication emerges at one year of
             age.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x01390131},
   Key = {fds351945}
}

@article{fds351946,
   Author = {Abbot-Smith, K and Lieven, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {What preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical
             word orders},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {679-692},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00054-5},
   Abstract = {Akhtar [J. Child Lang. 26 (1999) 339.] found that when
             4-year-old English-speaking children hear novel verbs in
             transitive utterances with ungrammatical word orders (e.g.,
             Elmo the tree meeked), they correct them to canonical SVO
             order almost all of the time. However, when 3-year-olds and
             older 2-year-olds hear these same utterances, they waver
             between correcting and using the ungrammatical ordering. In
             the current study, we adapted this task for children at 2;4,
             using an intransitive construction. The major finding was
             that children corrected the noncanonical word order less
             than half as often as Akhtar's 2-year-old subjects who were
             approximately 4 months older. At the same time, however,
             children showed in several ways that they had some implicit
             understanding of canonical SV order; for example, they used
             the novel verb which they heard used in grammatical word
             order more often than the novel verb which they heard in
             ungrammatical word order, and they consistently used
             pronouns and the progressive -s auxiliary in appropriate
             ways. The current findings thus contribute to a growing body
             of theory and research suggesting that the ontogenetic
             emergence of linguistic categories and schemas is a gradual
             process, as is the emergence of categories in other domains
             of cognitive development. © 2001 Elsevier Science
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00054-5},
   Key = {fds351946}
}

@article{fds366602,
   Author = {Striano, T and Tomasello, M and Rochat, P},
   Title = {Social and object support for early symbolic
             play},
   Journal = {Developmental Science},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {442-455},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00186},
   Abstract = {In this study we sought to determine the degree to which
             2-to 3-year-old children use objects symbolically in the
             relative absence of adult symbolic actions or linguistic
             descriptions, and how the nature of objects influences
             symbolic play. Results revealed a dramatic increase in
             children's creative symbolic productions between 2 and 3
             years of age, with the tendency to produce symbolic actions
             influenced to an equal degree by adult symbolic action
             models and verbal directions. Children of all ages were
             heavily influenced by the nature of the object to be used as
             a symbol, with the youngest children using only replica
             objects as symbols. In a second study, we examined
             children's looks to an adult as they engaged in different
             kinds of activities with objects. The main finding was that
             children looked to the adult immediately after performing a
             symbolic action more often than if they performed an
             instrumental action. We argue for the essentially social
             nature of symbolic play, both in terms of how children learn
             to use objects as symbols and in terms of the reasons they
             do so.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-7687.00186},
   Key = {fds366602}
}

@article{fds325203,
   Author = {Agnetta, B and Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris)
             of different ages do and do not use},
   Journal = {Animal Cognition},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {107-112},
   Publisher = {Springer Nature},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710000070},
   Abstract = {The results of three experiments are reported. In the main
             study, a human experimenter presented domestic dogs (Canis
             familiaris) with a variety of social cues intended to
             indicate the location of hidden food. The novel findings of
             this study were: (1) dogs were able to use successfully
             several totally novel cues in which they watched a human
             place a marker in front of the target location; (2) dogs
             were unable to use the marker by itself with no behavioral
             cues (suggesting that some form of human behavior directed
             to the target location was a necessary part of the cue); and
             (3) there were no significant developments in dogs' skills
             in these tasks across the age range 4 months to 4 years
             (arguing against the necessity of extensive learning
             experiences with humans). In a follow- up study, dogs did
             not follow human gaze into "empty space" outside of the
             simulated foraging context. Finally, in a small pilot study,
             two arctic wolves (Canis lupus) were unable to use human
             cues to locate hidden food. These results suggest the
             possibility that domestic dogs have evolved an adaptive
             specialization for using human-produced directional cues in
             a goal-directed (especially foraging) context. Exactly how
             they understand these cues is still an open question. ©
             Springer-Verlag 2000.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s100710000070},
   Key = {fds325203}
}

@article{fds351947,
   Author = {Call, J and Agnetta, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden
             objects},
   Journal = {Animal Cognition},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {23-34},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710050047},
   Abstract = {Chimpanzees follow conspecific and human gaze direction
             reliably in some situations, but very few chimpanzees
             reliably use gaze direction or other communicative signals
             to locate hidden food in the object-choice task. Three
             studies aimed at exploring factors that affect chimpanzee
             performance in this task are reported. In the first study,
             vocalizations and other noises facilitated the performance
             of some chimpanzees (only a minority). In the second study,
             various behavioral cues were given in which a human
             experimenter either touched, approached, or actually lifted
             and looked under the container where the food was hidden.
             Each of these cues led to enhanced performance for only a
             very few individuals. In the third study - a replication
             with some methodological improvements of a previous
             experiment - chimpanzees were confronted with two
             experimenters giving conflicting cues about the location of
             the hidden food, with one of them (the knower) having
             witnessed the hiding process and the other (the guesser)
             not. In the crucial test in which a third experimenter did
             the hiding, no chimpanzee found the food at above chance
             levels. Overall, in all three studies, by far the best
             performers were two individuals who had been raised in
             infancy by humans. It thus seems that while chimpanzees are
             very good at "behavior reading" of various sorts, including
             gaze following, they do not understand the communicative
             intentions (informative intentions) behind the looking and
             gesturing of others - with the possible exception of
             enculturated chimpanzees, who still do not understand the
             differential significance of looking and gesturing done by
             people who have different knowledge about states of affairs
             in the world. © Springer-Verlag 2000.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s100710050047},
   Key = {fds351947}
}

@article{fds351948,
   Author = {Campbell, AL and Brooks, P and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Factors affecting young children's use of pronouns as
             referring expressions.},
   Journal = {Journal of speech, language, and hearing research :
             JSLHR},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1337-1349},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4306.1337},
   Abstract = {Most studies of children's use of pronouns have focused
             either on the morphology of personal pronouns or on the
             anaphoric use of pronouns by older children. The current two
             studies investigated factors affecting children's choice of
             pronouns as referring expressions-in contrast with their use
             of full nouns and null references. In the first study it was
             found that 2.5- and 3.5-year-old children did not use
             pronouns differentially whether the adult (a) modeled a
             pronoun or a noun for the target object or (b) did or did
             not witness the target event (although there was evidence
             that they did notice and take account of the adult's
             witnessing in other ways). In the second study it was found
             that children of this same age (a) do not use pronouns to
             avoid unfamiliar or difficult nouns but (b) do use pronouns
             differently depending on the immediately preceding discourse
             of the experimenter (whether they were asked a specific
             question such as "What did X do?" or a general question such
             as "What happened?"). In the case of specific questions,
             children prefer to use a null reference but use some
             pronouns as well (almost never using full nouns); in the
             case of the generic questions, children use pronouns even
             more often (and use nouns more as well). This finding was
             corroborated by some new analyses of children's use of
             pronouns in specific discourse situations in previously
             published studies. These findings suggest that children's
             choice of pronouns as referring expressions in early
             language development is influenced more by the immediately
             preceding discourse than other kinds of factors.},
   Doi = {10.1044/jslhr.4306.1337},
   Key = {fds351948}
}

@article{fds351949,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Erratum: (Trends in Cognitive Sciences (April) 4:4
             (156-163))},
   Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {186},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01481-9},
   Doi = {10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01481-9},
   Key = {fds351949}
}

@article{fds351950,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The item-based nature of children's early syntactic
             development},
   Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {156-163},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01462-5},
   Abstract = {Recent research using both naturalistic and experimental
             methods has found that the vast majority of young children's
             early language is organized around concrete, item-based
             linguistic schemas. From this beginning, children then
             construct more abstract and adult-like linguistic
             constructions, but only gradually and in piecemeal fashion.
             These new data present significant problems for nativist
             accounts of children's language development that use
             adult-like linguistic categories, structures and formal
             grammars as analytical tools. Instead, the best account of
             these data is provided by a usage-based model in which
             children imitatively learn concrete linguistic expressions
             from the language they hear around them, and then - using
             their general cognitive and social-cognitive skills -
             categorize, schematize and creatively combine these
             individually learned expressions and structures. Copyright
             (C) 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01462-5},
   Key = {fds351950}
}

@article{fds351951,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do young children have adult syntactic competence?},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {209-253},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00069-4},
   Abstract = {Many developmental psycholinguists assume that young
             children have adult syntactic competence, this assumption
             being operationalized in the use of adult-like grammars to
             describe young children's language. This "continuity
             assumption" has never had strong empirical support, but
             recently a number of new findings have emerged - both from
             systematic analyses of children's spontaneous speech and
             from controlled experiments - that contradict it directly.
             In general, the key finding is that most of children's early
             linguistic competence is item based, and therefore their
             language development proceeds in a piecemeal fashion with
             virtually no evidence of any system-wide syntactic
             categories, schemas, or parameters. For a variety of
             reasons, these findings are not easily explained in terms of
             the development of children's skills of linguistic
             performance, pragmatics, or other "external" factors. The
             framework of an alternative, usage-based theory of child
             language acquisition - relying explicitly on new models from
             Cognitive-Functional Linguistics - is presented.},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00069-4},
   Key = {fds351951}
}

@article{fds325204,
   Author = {Hare, B and Call, J and Agnetta, B and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not
             see},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {59},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {771-785},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1999.1377},
   Abstract = {We report a series of experiments on social problem solving
             in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. In each experiment a
             subordinate and a dominant individual were put into
             competition over two pieces of food. In all experiments
             dominants obtained virtually all of the foods to which they
             had good visual and physical access. However, subordinates
             were successful quite often in three situations in which
             they had better visual access to the food than the dominant,
             for example, when the food was positioned so that only the
             subordinate (and not the dominant) could see it. In some
             cases, the subordinate might have been monitoring the
             behaviour of the dominant directly and simply avoided the
             food that the dominant was moving towards (which just
             happened to be the one it could see). In other cases,
             however, we ruled out this possibility by giving
             subordinates a small headstart and forcing them to make
             their choice (to go to the food that both competitors could
             see, or the food that only they could see) before the
             dominant was released into the area. Together with other
             recent studies, the present investigation suggests that
             chimpanzees know what conspecifics can and cannot see, and,
             furthermore, that they use this knowledge to devise
             effective social-cognitive strategies in naturally occurring
             food competition situations. (C)2000 The Association for the
             Study of Animal Behaviour.},
   Doi = {10.1006/anbe.1999.1377},
   Key = {fds325204}
}

@article{fds351952,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Culture and cognitive development},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {37-40},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00056},
   Abstract = {Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways
             that other primates are not. The difference can be clearly
             seen when the social learning skills of humans and their
             nearest primate relatives are systematically compared. The
             human adaptation for culture begins to make itself manifest
             in human ontogeny at around 1 year of age as human infants
             come to undestrand other persons as intentional agents like
             the self and so engage in joint attentional interactions
             with them. This understanding then enables young children
             (a) to employ some uniquely powerful forms of cultural
             learning to acquire the accumulated wisdom of their
             cultures, especially as embodied in language, and also (b)
             to comprehend their worlds in some uniquely powerful ways
             involving perspectivally based symbolic representations.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-8721.00056},
   Key = {fds351952}
}

@article{fds351953,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Primate cognition: Introduction to the issue},
   Journal = {Cognitive Science},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {351-361},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2403_1},
   Abstract = {I introduce the special issue by: (1) outlining something of
             the relationship between mainstream cognitive science and
             the study of nonhuman primate cognition; (2) providing a
             brief overview of the scientific study of primate cognition
             and how the papers of this special issue fit into that
             scientific paradigm; and (3) explicating my own views about
             the relationship between nonhuman primate cognition and
             human cognition. © 2000 Cognitive Science Society,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1207/s15516709cog2403_1},
   Key = {fds351953}
}

@article{fds351954,
   Author = {Bellagamba, F and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and
             18-month-olds},
   Journal = {Infant Behavior and Development},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {277-282},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00002-8},
   Abstract = {The current study was a replication and extension of a study
             of infant imitative learning by Meltzoff (1995). Unlike the
             18-month-old infants in that study (and other 18-month-olds
             in the current study), the 12-month-olds in this study did
             not frequently imitate unsuccessful goal-directed actions.
             Also, both 12- and 18-month-old infants reproduced actions
             more often when they observed the entire action and its
             result than when they observed the result only. © 1999
             ABLEX Publishing Corporation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00002-8},
   Key = {fds351954}
}

@article{fds351955,
   Author = {Behrens, H and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {And what about the Chinese?},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1014},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99222224},
   Abstract = {We discuss other recent studies on the acquisition of the
             German plural that do not support the dual-mechanism model.
             The attested overgeneralizations are not by default only,
             nor completely random, but predictable from subregularities
             based on the grammatical gender and the phonology of the
             noun. In addition, the dual-mechanism model creates a number
             of problems for acquisition (theory) rather than solving
             existing ones.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X99222224},
   Key = {fds351955}
}

@article{fds351956,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Striano, T and Rochat, P},
   Title = {Do young children use objects as symbols?},
   Journal = {British Journal of Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {563-584},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151099165483},
   Abstract = {Much of young children's symbolic play is heavily scaffolded
             by adult symbolic action models, which children may imitate,
             and by adult verbal scripts. The current studies attempted
             to evaluate 18-35-month-old children's symbolic skills in
             the absence of such scaffolding. In a study of symbol
             comprehension, children were tested for their ability to
             comprehend an adult's use of either a replica object or an
             associated gesture to communicate which object in an array
             she wanted. In a study of symbol production, children were
             given some objects that afforded symbolic manipulations, but
             without adult symbolic action models or verbal scripts. The
             results of the two studies converged to suggest that
             children below 2 years of age have symbolic skills with
             gestures, but not with objects. It was also found that while
             children at 26 months were able to use an object as a symbol
             for another object, they had difficulties when the symbol
             had another conventional use (e.g. a drinking cup used as a
             hat). The findings are discussed in terms of DeLoache's dual
             representation model, and a modification of that model is
             proposed.},
   Doi = {10.1348/026151099165483},
   Key = {fds351956}
}

@article{fds351957,
   Author = {Brooks, PJ and Tomasello, M and Dodson, K and Lewis,
             LB},
   Title = {Young children's overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity
             verbs.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {70},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1325-1337},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00097},
   Abstract = {The present study examined English-speaking children's
             tendency to make argument structure overgeneralization
             errors (e.g., I disappeared it). Children were exposed to
             several English verbs of fixed transitivity (exclusively
             intransitive or exclusively transitive) and then asked
             questions that encouraged them to overgeneralize usage of
             the verbs. Seventy-two children (24 in each of three age
             groups: 3, 4/5, and 8 years of age) experienced four actions
             performed by puppets. Each action had two verbs of similar
             meaning associated with it in the context of the
             experimental action: one more familiar to young children and
             one less familiar. Children at all ages were more likely to
             overgeneralize usage of verbs that were less familiar to
             them, supporting the hypothesis that children's usage of
             verbs in particular construction types becomes entrenched
             over time. As children solidly learn the transitivity status
             of particular verbs, they become more reluctant to use those
             verbs in other argument structure constructions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-8624.00097},
   Key = {fds351957}
}

@article{fds351958,
   Author = {Visalberghi, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Causal understanding in primates in physical and
             psychological domain},
   Journal = {Sistemi Intelligenti},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {307-331},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1422/3515},
   Doi = {10.1422/3515},
   Key = {fds351958}
}

@article{fds351959,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {A nonverbal false belief task: the performance of children
             and great apes.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {70},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {381-395},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00028},
   Abstract = {A nonverbal task of false belief understanding was given to
             4- and 5-year-old children (N = 28) and to two species of
             great ape: chimpanzees and orangutans (N = 7). The task was
             embedded in a series of finding games in which an adult (the
             hider) hid a reward in one of two identical containers, and
             another adult (the communicator) observed the hiding process
             and attempted to help the participant by placing a marker on
             the container that she believed to hold the reward. An
             initial series of control trials ensured that participants
             were able to use the marker to locate the reward, follow the
             reward in both visible and invisible displacements, and
             ignore the marker when they knew it to be incorrect. In the
             crucial false belief trials, the communicator watched the
             hiding process and then left the area, at which time the
             hider switched the locations of the containers. When the
             communicator returned, she marked the container at the
             location where she had seen the reward hidden, which was
             incorrect. The hider then gave the subject the opportunity
             to find the sticker. Successful performance required
             participants to reason as follows: the communicator placed
             the marker where she saw the reward hidden; the container
             that was at that location is now at the other location; so
             the reward is at the other location. Children were also
             given a verbal false belief task in the context of this same
             hiding game. The two main results of the study were: (1)
             children's performance on the verbal and nonverbal false
             belief tasks were highly correlated (and both fit very
             closely with age norms from previous studies), and (2) no
             ape succeeded in the nonverbal false belief task even though
             they succeeded in all of the control trials indicating
             mastery of the general task demands.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-8624.00028},
   Key = {fds351959}
}

@article{fds325205,
   Author = {Itakura, S and Agnetta, B and Hare, B and Tomasello,
             M},
   Title = {Chimpanzee use of human and conspecific social cues to
             locate hidden food},
   Journal = {Developmental Science},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {448-456},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00089},
   Abstract = {Two studies are reported in which chimpanzees attempted to
             use social cues to locate hidden food in one of two possible
             hiding places. In the first study four chimpanzees were
             exposed to a local enhancement cue (the informant approached
             and looked to the location where food was hidden and then
             remained beside it) and a gaze/point cue (the informant
             gazed and manually pointed towards the location where the
             food was hidden). Each cue was given by both a human
             informant and a chimpanzee informant. In the second study 12
             chimpanzees were exposed to a gaze direction cue in
             combination with a vocal cue (the human informant gazed to
             the hiding location and produced one of two different
             vocalizations: a 'food-bark' or a human word-form). The
             results were: (i) all subjects were quite skillful with the
             local enhancement cue, no matter who produced it; (ii) few
             subjects were skillful with the gaze/point cue, no matter
             who produced it (most of these being individuals who had
             been raised in infancy by humans); and (iii) most subjects
             were skillful when the human gazed and vocalized at the
             hiding place, with little difference between the two types
             of vocal cue. Findings are discussed in terms of
             chimpanzees' apparent need for additional cues, over and
             above gaze direction cues, to indicate the presence of
             food.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-7687.00089},
   Key = {fds325205}
}

@article{fds325571,
   Author = {Hare, B and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific
             social cues to locate hidden food},
   Journal = {Journal of Comparative Psychology},
   Volume = {113},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {X173-X177},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0735-7036.113.2.173},
   Abstract = {Ten domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different breeds and
             ages were exposed to 2 different social cues indicating the
             location of hidden food, each provided by both a human
             informant and a conspecific informant (for a total of 4
             different social cues). For the local enhancement cue the
             informant approached the location where food was hidden and
             then stayed beside it. For the gaze and point cue, the
             informant stood equidistant between 2 hiding locations and
             bodily oriented and gazed toward the 1 in which food was
             hidden (the human informant also pointed). Eight of the 10
             subjects, including the one 6-month-old juvenile, were above
             chance with 2 or more cues. Results are discussed in terms
             of the phylogenetic and ontogenetic processes by means of
             which dogs come to use social cues to locate
             food.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0735-7036.113.2.173},
   Key = {fds325571}
}

@article{fds326036,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Hare, B and Agnetta, B},
   Title = {Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction
             geometrically},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {58},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {769-777},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1999.1192},
   Abstract = {Two experiments on chimpanzee gaze following are reported.
             In the first, chimpanzee subjects watched as a human
             experimenter looked around various types of barriers. The
             subjects looked around each of the barriers more when the
             human had done so than in a control condition (in which the
             human looked-in another direction). In the second
             experiment, chimpanzees watched as a human looked towards
             the back of their cage. As they turned to follow the human's
             gaze a distractor-object was presented. The chimpanzees
             looked at the distractor while still following the human's
             gaze to the back of the cage. These two experiments
             effectively disconfirm the low-level model of chimpanzee
             gaze following in which it is claimed that upon seeing
             another animate being's gaze direction chimpanzees simply
             turn in that direction and look around for something
             interesting. Rather, they support the hypothesis that
             chimpanzees follow the gaze direction of other animate
             beings geometrically to specific locations, in much the same
             way as human infants. The degree to which chimpanzees have a
             mentalistic interpretation of the gaze and/or visual
             experience of others is still an open question.},
   Doi = {10.1006/anbe.1999.1192},
   Key = {fds326036}
}

@article{fds351960,
   Author = {Brooks, PJ and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {How children constrain their argument structure
             constructions},
   Journal = {Language},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {720-738},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/417731},
   Abstract = {We tested two hypotheses about how English-speaking children
             learn to avoid making argument structure errors such as
             Don't giggle me. The first is that children base their usage
             of verbs on membership in narrow-range semantic classes
             (Pinker 1989). The second is that children make use of
             indirect negative evidence in the form of alternative
             expressions that preempt tendencies to overgeneralize.
             Ninety-six children (32 each at 2.5, 4.5, and 6/7 years of
             age) were introduced to two nonce verbs, one as a transitive
             verb and one as an intransitive verb. One verb was from a
             semantic class that can be used both transitively and
             intransitively while the other was from a fixed transitivity
             class. Half of the children were given preempting
             alternatives with both verbs; for example, they heard a verb
             in a simple transitive construction (as in Ernie's meeking
             the car) and then they also heard it in a passive
             construction - which enabled them to answer the question
             'What's happening with the car?' with It's getting meeked
             (rather than generalizing to the intransitive construction
             with It's meeking). We found empirical support for the
             constraining role of verb classes and of preemption, but
             only for children 4.5 years of age and older. Results are
             discussed in terms of a model of syntactic development in
             which children begin with lexically specific linguistic
             constructions and only gradually learn to differentiate
             verbs as lexical items from argument structure constructions
             as abstract linguistic entities.},
   Doi = {10.2307/417731},
   Key = {fds351960}
}

@article{fds351961,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The human adaptation for culture},
   Journal = {Annual Review of Anthropology},
   Volume = {28},
   Pages = {509-529},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.509},
   Abstract = {Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways
             that other primates are not, as evidenced most clearly by
             the fact that only human cultural traditions accumulate
             modifications over historical time (the ratchet effect). The
             key adaptation is one that enables individuals to understand
             other individuals as intentional agents like the self. This
             species-unique form of social cognition emerges in human
             ontogeny at approximately 1 year of age, as infants begin to
             engage with other persons in various kinds of joint
             attentional activities involving gaze following, social
             referencing, and gestural communication. Young children's
             joint attentional skills then engender some uniquely
             powerful forms of cultural learning, enabling the
             acquisition of language, discourse skills, tool-use
             practices, and other conventional activities. These novel
             forms of cultural learning allow human beings to, in effect,
             pool their cognitive resources both contemporaneously and
             over historical time in ways that are unique in the animal
             kingdom.},
   Doi = {10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.509},
   Key = {fds351961}
}

@article{fds351962,
   Author = {Brooks, PJ and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children learn to produce passives with nonce
             verbs.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {29-44},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.35.1.29},
   Abstract = {Younger and older children (mean ages = 2 years 11 months
             and 3 years 5 months) learned 2 nonce verbs in a full
             passive or active transitive construction. When asked
             patient-focused questions encouraging passive-voice replies
             (e.g., "What happened to the ball?") or agent-focused
             questions encouraging active-voice replies (e.g., "What did
             Elmo do?"), children used a variety of strategies to meet
             the demands of the questions, usually without changing the
             construction in which the verb occurred. In Study 2 in which
             passive and active constructions were primed, 40% of the
             almost 3-year-old children used an active-introduced verb in
             a passive construction and 35% used a passive-introduced
             verb in an active transitive construction when discourse
             demands encouraged them to do so. Thus, before their 3rd
             birthdays, some children have an understanding of the
             passive and active transitive constructions general enough
             to support productive usages with newly learned
             verbs.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0012-1649.35.1.29},
   Key = {fds351962}
}

@article{fds351963,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Brooks, PJ and Stern, E},
   Title = {Learning to produce passive utterances through
             discourse},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {53},
   Pages = {223-237},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272379801805306},
   Abstract = {Two studies of English-speaking children's acquisition of
             the passive construction are reported. In the first study
             children at 3.0 and 3.5 years of age were taught to produce
             full passive utterances with a nonce verb through rich
             discourse interaction. All the older children learned to
             produce a passive with the nonce verb, whereas only
             two-thirds of the younger children learned to do this - and
             they needed three times as many adult utterances to do so.
             In the second study, also using a nonce verb, some
             3.0-year-old children were given rich discourse interactions
             containing truncated passives, passive questions, and by
             phrases - all of which added up to a full passive - but they
             never heard a full passive utterance as a whole. Other
             children were given only models of full passive utterances
             with no discourse scaffolding. Only children who heard full
             passive utterances produced them. The children who
             participated in rich discourse interactions produced
             truncated passives (as they had heard). These results
             demonstrate that children can learn to produce full passive
             sentences with a nonce verb at 3 years of age, but, in
             accordance with Tomasello's (1992) verb island hypothesis,
             they tend to do so only within the syntactic constructions
             in which they have heard adults using that verb. © Alpha
             Academic.},
   Doi = {10.1177/014272379801805306},
   Key = {fds351963}
}

@article{fds325206,
   Author = {Byrne, RW and Russon, AE},
   Title = {Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach.},
   Journal = {The Behavioral and brain sciences},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {667-684},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x98001745},
   Abstract = {To explain social learning without invoking the cognitively
             complex concept of imitation, many learning mechanisms have
             been proposed. Borrowing an idea used routinely in cognitive
             psychology, we argue that most of these alternatives can be
             subsumed under a single process, priming, in which input
             increases the activation of stored internal representations.
             Imitation itself has generally been seen as a "special
             faculty." This has diverted much research towards the
             all-or-none question of whether an animal can imitate, with
             disappointingly inconclusive results. In the great apes,
             however, voluntary, learned behaviour is organized
             hierarchically. This means that imitation can occur at
             various levels, of which we single out two clearly distinct
             ones: the "action level," a rather detailed and linear
             specification of sequential acts, and the "program level," a
             broader description of subroutine structure and the
             hierarchical layout of a behavioural "program." Program
             level imitation is a high-level, constructive mechanism,
             adapted for the efficient learning of complex skills and
             thus not evident in the simple manipulations used to test
             for imitation in the laboratory. As examples, we describe
             the food-preparation techniques of wild mountain gorillas
             and the imitative behaviour of orangutans undergoing
             "rehabilitation" to the wild. Representing and manipulating
             relations between objects seems to be one basic building
             block in their hierarchical programs. There is evidence that
             great apes suffer from a stricter capacity limit than humans
             in the hierarchical depth of planning. We re-interpret some
             chimpanzee behaviour previously described as "emulation" and
             suggest that all great apes may be able to imitate at the
             program level. Action level imitation is seldom observed in
             great ape skill learning, and may have a largely social
             role, even in humans.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x98001745},
   Key = {fds325206}
}

@article{fds351964,
   Author = {Dodson, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Acquiring the transitive construction in English: the role
             of animacy and pronouns.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {605-622},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000998003535},
   Abstract = {Twenty-four children between 2;5 and 3;1 were taught two
             nonce verbs. Each verb was used multiple times by an adult
             experimenter to refer to a highly transitive action
             involving a mostly animate agent (including the child
             herself) and a patient of varying animacy. One of the verbs
             was modelled in the Two-Participants condition in which the
             experimenter said: 'Look. Big Bird is dopping the boat'. The
             other verb was modelled in the No-Participant condition in
             which the experimenter named the Two-Participants but did
             not use them as arguments of the novel verb: 'Look what Big
             Bird is doing to the boat. It's called keefing'. It was
             found that whereas many children produced transitive
             sentences with the Two-Participants verb, only children
             close to 3;0 produced transitive sentences with the
             No-Participant verb. This age is somewhat younger than
             previous studies in which young children were asked to
             produce transitive sentences with two lexical nouns for the
             two animate participants. Also, re-analyses of previously
             published studies in which children learned novel verbs in
             sentence frames without arguments found that the few
             transitive sentences produced by children under 2;6 involved
             either I or me as subject. One hypothesis is thus that as
             young children in the third year of life begin to construct
             a more abstract and verb-general transitive construction,
             this construction initially contains only certain types of
             participants expressed in only certain kinds of linguistic
             forms.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000998003535},
   Key = {fds351964}
}

@article{fds351965,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in
             orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes),
             and human children (Homo sapiens).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {112},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {192-206},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.192},
   Abstract = {This study investigates the understanding of others'
             intentions in 2- and 3-year-old children, chimpanzees (Pan
             troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). During
             training, subjects learned to use a discriminative cue to
             select a baited box. During testing, the experimenter placed
             a marker on top of the baited box to inform the subject of
             the reward's location. However, the experimenter also
             accidentally dropped the marker on top of an unbaited box,
             so that during any given trial the experimenter marked 2
             boxes, 1 intentionally and 1 accidentally. All 3 species
             preferentially selected the box the experimenter had marked
             intentionally (especially during the initial trials), with
             3-year-old children presenting the most robust results.
             These findings suggest that subjects understood something
             about the experimenter's intentions. The authors speculate
             that understanding of others' intentions may precede the
             understanding of others' beliefs both at the ontogenetic and
             phylogenetic levels.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.192},
   Key = {fds351965}
}

@article{fds351966,
   Author = {Visalberghi, E and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Primate causal understanding in the physical and
             psychological domains},
   Journal = {Behavioural Processes},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {2-3},
   Pages = {189-203},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(97)00076-4},
   Abstract = {Evidence for primates' understanding of causality is
             presented and discussed. Understanding causality requires
             the organism to understand not just that two events are
             associated with one another in space and time, but also that
             there is some 'mediating force' that binds the two events to
             one another which may be used to predict or control those
             events (e.g. a physical force such as gravity or a
             psychological force such as an intention). In the physical
             domain, studies of tool use indicate that capuchin monkeys
             do not have a causal understanding of the functioning of
             tools in terms of the physical forces involved, but rather
             they learn to associate aspects of their own behavior with
             the results it produces. Apes show some possible signs of
             understanding the causal relations involved in tool use in
             the sense that they may employ various forms of foresight in
             approaching novel tasks, perhaps involving an understanding
             of physical forces-although not to the extent of human
             children. In the psychological domain, nonhuman primates
             understand conspecifics as animate beings that generate
             their own behavior and, thus, they appreciate that to
             manipulate conspecifics communicative signals, and not
             physical activities, are required. However, there is very
             little evidence that nonhuman primates of any species
             understand others as psychological beings with intentions
             and other psychological states that mediate their behavioral
             interactions with the world-as human children begin to do
             sometime during their second year of life. More research,
             using a wider range of problem-solving situations, is needed
             if we are to become more precise in our understanding of how
             primates understand the causal structure of the world around
             them.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0376-6357(97)00076-4},
   Key = {fds351966}
}

@article{fds326347,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J and Hare, B},
   Title = {Five primate species follow the visual gaze of
             conspecifics},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1063-1069},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0636},
   Abstract = {Individuals from five primate species were tested
             experimentally for their ability to follow the visual gaze
             of conspecifics to an outside object. Subjects were from
             captive social groups of chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, sooty
             mangabeys, Cercocebus atys torquatus, rhesus macaques,
             Macaca mulatta, stumptail macaques, M. arctoides, and
             pigtail macaques, M. nemestrina. Experimental trials
             consisted of an experimenter inducing one individual to look
             at food being displayed, and then observing the reaction of
             another individual (the subject) that was looking at that
             individual (not the food). Control trials consisted of an
             experimenter displaying the food in an identical manner when
             the subject was alone. Individuals from all species reliably
             followed the gaze of conspecifics, looking to the food about
             80% of the time in experimental trials, compared with about
             20% of the time in control trials. Results are discussed in
             terms of both the proximate mechanisms that might be
             involved and the adaptive functions that might be served by
             gaze-following.},
   Doi = {10.1006/anbe.1997.0636},
   Key = {fds326347}
}

@article{fds351967,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Reference: Intending that others jointly
             attend},
   Journal = {Pragmatics and Cognition},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {1-2},
   Pages = {229-243},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/pc.6.1-2.12tom},
   Abstract = {My approach to reference focuses on naturally occuring
             processes of communication, and in particular on
             children’s earliest referential activities. I begin by
             describing three different kinds of child gesture –
             ritualizations, deictics, and symbolic gestures – and then
             proceed to examine young children’s early word learning.
             The account focuses on the joint attentional situations in
             which young children learn their earliest gestures and
             linguistic symbols and on the social-cognitive and cultural
             learning processes involved in the different cases. © 1998
             John Benjamins Publishing Company.},
   Doi = {10.1075/pc.6.1-2.12tom},
   Key = {fds351967}
}

@article{fds351968,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Brooks, PJ},
   Title = {Young Children'S earliest transitive and intransitive
             constructions},
   Journal = {Cognitive Linguistics},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {379-396},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1998.9.4.379},
   Abstract = {Much of children's early syntactic development can be seen
             as the acquisition of sentence-level constructions that
             correspond to relatively complex events and states of
             affairs. The ctirrent study was an attempt to determine the
             relative concreteness (verb-specificity) or abstractness
             (verb-generality) of such constructions for children just
             beginning to produce large numbers of multi-word utterances.
             Sixteen children at 2.0 years of age and sixteen children at
             2.5 years of age participated (all English speaking). Each
             child was taught two novel verbsfor a highly transitive
             action: one in a transitive construction (Ernie is tamming
             the car) and one in an intransitive construction (with
             patient as subject: The ball is meeking). They were then
             given o p rtunities to use their newly learned verbs, in
             many cases in discourse situations that encouraged use of
             the “opposite” construction (i.e., agentand
             patient-focused questions). Results showed that 2.0-year-old
             children almost never produced an utterance using a novel
             verb in anything other t an the construction in which it had
             been modeled. Children at 2.5 years of age were somewhat
             more productive, but still the large majority of these
             children avoided using the experimental verbs in nonmodeled
             constructions. These results suggest that when
             English-speaking children produce simple transitive and
             intransitive utterances in their spontaneous speech, they
             are doing so on a verb-specific basis (verb Island
             constructions), schematizing more abstract constructions
             only later as they discover patterns that apply across many
             such lexically specific constructions. © 1998, Walter de
             Gruyter. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1515/cogl.1998.9.4.379},
   Key = {fds351968}
}

@article{fds351969,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Response to commentators},
   Journal = {Journal of Child Language},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {485-491},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003511},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0305000998003511},
   Key = {fds351969}
}

@article{fds351970,
   Author = {Ashley, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cooperative problem-solving and teaching in
             preschoolers},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {143-163},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00059},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated the ontogenetic origins of
             children's skills of cooperative problem-solving in a task
             involving two complementary roles. Participants were peer
             dyads of 24, 30, 36, and 42 months of age. Primary dyads
             were initially presented with an instrumental problem whose
             solution required them to cooperate by coordinating two
             complementary actions. To further investigate their
             understanding of the task, these same dyads were then
             presented with the same problem but with roles reversed.
             Finally, after each of these primary participants had
             demonstrated proficiency in both roles, each was separately
             paired with a naive peer and given the opportunity to teach
             the naive partner the task. A clear ontogenetic trend
             emerged. Even with adult assistance, 24-month-old children
             never became independently proficient at the task.
             Thirty-and 36-month-old children became proficient mostly
             independently, but only relatively slowly and without
             demonstrating extensive amounts of behavioral coordination
             or the use of explicitly directive language to facilitate
             coordination. Although they did show evidence of recognizing
             when a peer was new to the task, children of this age
             engaged in little explicit teaching of naive peers. In
             contrast, 42-month-old children mastered the task much more
             quickly than the other children, responded much more quickly
             and accurately when their roles were reversed, coordinated
             both their actions and language in the task to a much
             greater extent, and engaged in more explicit teaching of
             naive peers. Results are discussed in terms of the
             developing social cognitive skills that enable children from
             2 to 4 years of age to understand other persons as mental
             agents with whom they may share mental perspectives.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-9507.00059},
   Key = {fds351970}
}

@article{fds351971,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Akhtar, N and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially
             imitate intentional and accidental actions},
   Journal = {Infant Behavior and Development},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {315-330},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1},
   Abstract = {This study explored infants' ability to discriminate
             between, and their tendency to reproduce, the accidental and
             intentional actions of others. Twenty 14- through
             18-month-olds watched an adult perform a series of two-step
             actions on objects that made interesting results occur. Some
             of the modeled actions were marked vocally as intentional
             ("There!"), some were marked vocally as accidental
             ("Woops!"). Following each demonstration, infants were given
             a chance to make the result occur themselves. Overall,
             infants imitated almost twice as many of the adult's
             intentional actions as her accidental ones. Infants before
             age 18 months thus may understand something about the
             intentions of other persons. This understanding represents
             infants' first step toward adult-like social cognition and
             underlies their acquisition of language and other cultural
             skills. © 1998 Ablex Publishing Corporation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1},
   Key = {fds351971}
}

@article{fds351972,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Uniquely primate, uniquely human},
   Journal = {Developmental Science},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-16},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00002},
   Abstract = {Two hypotheses about primate cognition are proposed. First,
             it is proposed that primates, but not other mammals,
             understand categories of relations among external entities.
             In the physical domain primates have special skills in tasks
             such as oddity, transitivity, and relation matching that
             require facility with relational categories; in the social
             domain primates have special skills in understanding the
             third-party social relationships that hold among other
             individuals in their groups. Second, it is proposed that
             humans, but not other primates, understand the causal and
             intentional relations that hold among external entities. In
             the physical domain only humans understand causal forces as
             mediating the connection between sequentially ordered
             events; in the social domain only humans understand the
             behavior of others as intentionally directed and controlled
             by desired outcomes. Both these uniquely primate and these
             uniquely human cognitive skills are hypothesized to have
             their origins in adaptations for negotiating complex social
             interactions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-7687.00002},
   Key = {fds351972}
}

@article{fds351973,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Nagell, K and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative
             competence from 9 to 15 months of age.},
   Journal = {Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
             Development},
   Volume = {63},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {i-143},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1166214},
   Abstract = {At around 1 year of age, human infants display a number of
             new behaviors that seem to indicate a newly emerging
             understanding of other persons as intentional beings whose
             attention to outside objects may be shared, followed into,
             and directed in various ways. These behaviors have mostly
             been studied separately. In the current study, we
             investigated the most important of these behaviors together
             as they emerged in a single group of 24 infants between 9
             and 15 months of age. At each of seven monthly visits, we
             measured joint attentional engagement, gaze and point
             following, imitation of two different kinds of actions on
             objects, imperative and declarative gestures, and
             comprehension and production of language. We also measured
             several nonsocial-cognitive skills as a point of comparison.
             We report two studies. The focus of the first study was the
             initial emergence of infants' social-cognitive skills and
             how these skills are related to one another developmentally.
             We found a reliable pattern of emergence: Infants progressed
             from sharing to following to directing others' attention and
             behavior. The nonsocial skills did not emerge predictably in
             this developmental sequence. Furthermore, correlational
             analyses showed that the ages of emergence of all pairs of
             the social-cognitive skills or their components were
             inter-related. The focus of the second study was the social
             interaction of infants and their mothers, especially with
             regard to their skills of joint attentional engagement
             (including mothers' use of language to follow into or direct
             infants' attention) and how these skills related to infants'
             early communicative competence. Our measures of
             communicative competence included not only language
             production, as in previous studies, but also language
             comprehension and gesture production. It was found that two
             measures--the amount of time infants spent in joint
             engagement with their mothers and the degree to which
             mothers used language that followed into their infant's
             focus of attention--predicted infants' earliest skills of
             gestural and linguistic communication. Results of the two
             studies are discussed in terms of their implications for
             theories of social-cognitive development, for theories of
             language development, and for theories of the process by
             means of which human children become fully participating
             members of the cultural activities and processes into which
             they are born.},
   Doi = {10.2307/1166214},
   Key = {fds351973}
}

@article{fds366603,
   Author = {Boesch, C and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Chimpanzee and human cultures},
   Journal = {Current Anthropology},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {591-614},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/204785},
   Abstract = {Culture has traditionally been attributed only to human
             beings. Despite growing evidence of behavioral diversity in
             wild chimpanzee populations, most anthropologists and
             psychologists still deny culture to this animal species. We
             argue here that culture is not monolithic but a set of
             processes. These processes show much diversity both in the
             social norms and models that determine which individuals
             will be exposed to particular cultural variants and what
             cultural variants will be present in the population and in
             the social learning mechanisms that determine the fidelity
             of transmission of the variants over time. Recognition of
             the diversity of these processes is important because it
             affects cultural dissemination, cultural evolution, and the
             complexity of cultural artifacts. A comparison of chimpanzee
             and human cultures shows many deep similarities, thus
             suggesting that they share evolutionary roots. Two possible
             differences between the two species are discussed. First,
             thanks to indirect means of transmission such as language,
             cultural dissemination is possible over greater stretches of
             time and space in humans than in chimpanzees. Second, human
             cultures rely more intensively than chimpanzee cultures on
             cumulative cultural evolution through the ratchet effect,
             which allows the accumulation of modifications over time and
             produces more elaborate cultural artifacts. © 1998 by The
             Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1086/204785},
   Key = {fds366603}
}

@article{fds351974,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J and Gluckman, A},
   Title = {Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human
             children.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {68},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1067-1080},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01985.x},
   Abstract = {Forty-eight young children (2.5 and 3.0 years old) and 9
             great apes (6 chimpanzees and 3 orangutans) participated in
             a hiding-finding game. An adult human experimenter (the
             Hider) hid a reward in 1 of 3 opaque containers aligned on a
             wooden plank. Another adult experimenter (the Communicator)
             attempted to help the subject find the reward by giving 1 of
             3 types of communicative sign: (1) Pointing, for which she
             placed her hand directly above the correct container with
             index finger oriented down; (2) Marker, for which she placed
             a small wooden block on top of the correct container; and
             (3) Replica, for which she held up a perceptually identical
             duplicate of the correct container. At both ages, children
             were above chance in this finding game with all 3 types of
             communicative sign, with Pointing being easiest (because
             they knew it prior to the experiment), Marker being next
             easiest, and Replica being most difficult. In contrast, no
             ape was above chance for any of the communicative signs that
             it did not know before the experiment (some had been trained
             in the use of the marker previously, and one knew pointing),
             nor was group performance above chance for any of the signs,
             despite the fact that apes experienced three times as many
             trials as children on each sign. Our explanation of these
             results is that young children understand the communicative
             intentions of other persons--although they may have more
             difficulty comprehending the exact nature of those
             intentions in some cases--whereas apes treat the behavioral
             signs of others as predictive cues only (signals). This may
             be because apes do not perceive and understand the
             communicative intentions of others, at least not in a
             human-like way.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01985.x},
   Key = {fds351974}
}

@article{fds351975,
   Author = {Akhtar, N and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's productivity with word order and verb
             morphology.},
   Journal = {Developmental psychology},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {952-965},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.952},
   Abstract = {Four studies examined English-speaking children's
             productivity with word order and verb morphology. Two- and
             3-year-olds were taught novel transitive verbs with
             experimentally controlled argument structures. The younger
             children neither used nor comprehended word order with these
             verbs; older children comprehended and used word order
             correctly to mark agents and patients of the novel verbs.
             Children as young as 2 years 1 month added -ing but not -ed
             to verb stems; older children were productive with both
             inflections. These studies demonstrate that the present
             progressive inflection is used productively before the
             regular past tense marker and suggest that productivity with
             word order may be independent of developments in verb
             morphology. The findings are discussed in terms of M.
             Tomasello's (1992a) Verb Island hypothesis and M. Rispoli's
             (1991) notion of the mosaic acquisition of grammatical
             relations.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.952},
   Key = {fds351975}
}

@article{fds351976,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Akhtar, N and Dodson, K and Rekau,
             L},
   Title = {Differential productivity in young children's use of nouns
             and verbs.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {373-387},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000997003085},
   Abstract = {A fundamental question of child language acquisition is
             children's productivity with newly learned forms. The
             current study addressed this question experimentally with
             children just beginning to combine words. Ten children
             between 1;6 and 1;11 were taught four new words, two nouns
             and two verbs, over multiple sessions. All four words were
             modelled in minimal syntactic contexts. The experimenter
             gave children multiple opportunities to produce the words
             and made attempts to elicit morphological endings (plural
             for nouns, past tense for verbs). Overall, children combined
             the novel nouns productively with already known words much
             more often than they did the novel verbs-by many orders of
             magnitude. Several children also pluralized a newly learned
             noun, whereas none of them formed a past tense with a newly
             learned verb. A follow-up study using a slightly different
             methodology confirmed the finding of limited syntactic
             productivity with verbs. Hypotheses accounting for this
             asymmetry in the early use of nouns and verbs are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000997003085},
   Key = {fds351976}
}

@article{fds351977,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Camaioni, L},
   Title = {A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human
             infants;},
   Journal = {Human Development},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {7-24},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000278540},
   Abstract = {The naturally occurring gestures of chimpanzees and
             prelinguistic human infants are compared. Considered as
             special cases are apes raised by humans as they gesture to
             humans, and children with autism. Overall, the most
             important differences between the gestures of typically
             developing children and the gestures of individuals from the
             other three groups concern: (1) their predominant use of
             triadic, distal gestures; (2) their extensive use of
             declarative gestures, and (3) their use of imitative
             learning in acquiring some gestures (symbolic or
             referential), which implies that the gestures are understood
             as bi-directional communicative conventions. These
             differences all derive from the uniquely human form of
             social cognition (i.e., knowledge of other minds) that first
             emerges during the 2nd year of life and that enables human
             infants to understand other persons as intentional agents
             with whom they may share experience. Implications for the
             origins and evolution of human culture and language are
             discussed. © 1997 S. Karger AG, Basel.},
   Doi = {10.1159/000278540},
   Key = {fds351977}
}

@article{fds351978,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Strosberg, R and Akhtar, N},
   Title = {Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive
             contexts.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {157-176},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900010138},
   Abstract = {Previous studies have demonstrated that children aged 2;0
             can learn new words in a variety of non-ostensive contexts.
             The current two studies were aimed at seeing if this was
             also true of children just beginning to learn words at 1;6.
             In the first study an adult interacted with 48 children. She
             used a nonce word to announce her intention to find an
             object ('Let's find the gazzer'), picked up and rejected an
             object with obvious disappointment, and then gleefully found
             the target object (using no language). Children learned the
             new word as well in this condition as in a condition in
             which the adult found the object immediately. In the second
             study the adult first played several rounds of a finding
             game with each of 60 children, in which it was first
             established that one of several novel objects was always in
             a very distinctive hiding place (a toy barn). The adult then
             used a nonce word to announce her intention to find an
             object ('Let's find the toma') and then proceeded to the
             barn. In the key condition the barn was mysteriously
             'locked'; the child thus never saw the target object after
             the nonce word was introduced. Children learned the new word
             as well in this condition as in a condition in which the
             adult found the object immediately. The results of these two
             studies suggest that from very early in language acquisition
             children learn words not through passive, associative
             processes, but rather through active attempts to understand
             adult behaviour in a variety of action and discourse
             contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900010138},
   Key = {fds351978}
}

@article{fds351979,
   Author = {Akhtar, N and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and
             actions},
   Journal = {British Journal of Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {79-93},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1996.tb00695.x},
   Abstract = {Two studies of word learning in 24-month-old children are
             reported, one involving an object word (Study 1) and one
             involving an action word (Study 2). In both studies,
             non-verbal scripts of playing with novel objects/actions in
             particular ways were established before the child was
             exposed to any language models. Following this pre-training,
             children heard an experimenter announce her intention to
             either find an object or perform an action. In the referent
             condition, children then saw the intended referent (object
             or action) immediately after hearing the language model.
             Children in the absent referent condition experienced the
             same non-verbal scripts and language models, but never saw
             the referent object or action after hearing the language
             model: at the appropriate juncture in the script they were
             told that the toy barn in which the target object had been
             previously located was 'locked', or that the toy character
             who had previously performed the target action was missing.
             Comparisons with two control conditions indicated that
             children were able to learn words for a novel object and a
             novel action in both the referent and absent referent
             conditions and, moreover, that learning was equivalent in
             these two conditions. These results show quite clearly that
             early lexical acquisition does not depend on temporal
             contiguity between word and referent - or indeed any
             perceptual pairing between word and referent at all - but
             rather it relies on children's active understandings of a
             speaker's referential intentions in particular discourse
             contexts.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.2044-835x.1996.tb00695.x},
   Key = {fds351979}
}

@article{fds351980,
   Author = {Akhtar, N and Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The Role of Discourse Novelty in Early Word
             Learning},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {67},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {635-645},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01756.x},
   Abstract = {2 studies of word learning are reported. In Study 1,
             24-month-old children and 2 adults played with 3 nameless
             objects. These objects were placed in a clear box along with
             a novel nameless object. The adults then displayed
             excitement about the contents of the box and modeled a new
             word. Comparison with a control condition indicated
             significant learning of the new word for the novel object.
             Study 2 followed the same procedure with one difference; the
             children played with the novel object while the adults were
             absent. Thus, at the time of the language model the target
             object was novel only to the adults, not to the children.
             Again subjects displayed significant learning of the new
             word. This last finding suggests that 24-month-old children
             understand that adults use language for things that are
             novel to the discourse context and that this novelty is
             determined from the speaker's point of view.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01756.x},
   Key = {fds351980}
}

@article{fds351981,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The child's contribution to culture: A commentary on
             Toomela},
   Journal = {Culture and Psychology},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {307-318},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1354067X9600200306},
   Abstract = {Toomela (1996) has emphasized the psychological dimensions
             of the process by which human children become participants
             in cultures. I support his arguments with observations of
             chimpanzees, which are similar to humans in some ways but
             still do not live culturally, and of human infants both
             before and after they have the capacity to participate fully
             in cultural activities. Toomela also proposes a new account
             of the process of internalization in which language plays
             the central role. I disagree somewhat with this account,
             arguing that whereas language is the most powerful human
             artifact potentiating internalization, other artifacts -
             both material and symbolic - may serve the same function if
             children are introduced to them in social interactions in
             which others have intentions toward their intentional states
             - and they know this. The central theoretical point of
             Toomela's paper is that a comprehensive account of the human
             species as a cultural species must focus not only on the
             cultural collective, but also on individuals and their
             psychological capacities.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1354067X9600200306},
   Key = {fds351981}
}

@article{fds351982,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Piagetian and Vygotskian Approaches to Language
             Acquisition},
   Journal = {Human Development},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {269-276},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000278478},
   Abstract = {Both Piaget and Vygotsky were centrally concerned with the
             ontogenetic relationships between language, cognition, and
             social life. Recently, researchers have drawn on their
             observations and hypotheses to establish much closer links
             between these phenomena than either theorist ever imagined.
             In investigating the cognitive bases of early language, very
             close links have been established between specific cognitive
             achievements and the acquisition of certain types of early
             words, for example between object permanence development and
             the acquisition of words for disappearance and between means
             ends development and the acquisition of words for
             success/failure. In investigating the social bases of early
             language, close links have been established between the
             quantity and quality of joint attentional social
             interactions in which a child and an adult engage and the
             child’s early word learning skills. Despite their seminal
             contributions to the study of early language development
             along these two lines, neither Piaget nor Vygotsky fully
             appreciated the skills of social cognition that underlie the
             acqusition of language. © 1996 S. Karger AG,
             Basel.},
   Doi = {10.1159/000278478},
   Key = {fds351982}
}

@article{fds351983,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Use of social information in the problem solving of
             orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo
             sapiens).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {109},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {308-320},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.308},
   Abstract = {Fourteen juvenile and adult orangutans and 24 3- and
             4-year-old children participated in 4 studies on imitative
             learning in a problem-solving situation. In all studies a
             simple to operate apparatus was used, but its internal
             mechanism was hidden from subjects to prevent individual
             learning. In the 1st study, orangutans observed a human
             demonstrator perform 1 of 4 actions on the apparatus and
             obtain a reward; they subsequently showed no signs of
             imitative learning. Similar results were obtained in a 2nd
             study in which orangutan demonstrators were used. Similar
             results were also obtained in a 3rd study in which a human
             encouraged imitation from an orangutan that had previously
             been taught to mimic arbitrary human actions. In a 4th
             study, human 3- and 4-year-old children learned the task by
             means of imitation.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.308},
   Key = {fds351983}
}

@article{fds351984,
   Author = {Carpenter, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Joint Attention and Imitative Learning in Children,
             Chimpanzees, and Enculturated Chimpanzees},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {217-237},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1995.tb00063.x},
   Abstract = {In this study we compared the nature of the joint
             attentional interactions that occurred as chimpanzees and
             human children engaged with a human experimenter (E).
             Subjects were three chimpanzees raised mostly with
             conspecifics (mother‐reared), three chimpanzees raised in
             a human‐like cultural environment (encultur‐ated), and
             six 18‐month‐old human children. Of particular interest
             were possible differences between the two groups of
             chimpanzees that might have resulted from their different
             ontogenetic histories. Observations were made as subjects
             participated in an imitative learning task involving a
             number of novel objects. Variables coded were such things as
             subjects' looks to the object, looks to E, the coordination
             of such looks in periods of joint engagement with E, and
             gestural attempts to direct E's attention or behavior
             (declaratives and imperatives). Results showed that
             encultur‐ated chimpanzees were most similar to human
             children in social interactions involv‐ing objects, for
             example, in their attention to the object in compliance with
             E's request, their joint attentional interactions during
             less structured periods, and their use of declarative
             gestures to direct E's attention to objects. They were not
             similar to children, but rather resembled their
             mother‐reared conspecifics, in the duration of their looks
             to E's face. A positive relation between subjects' joint
             attentional skills and their imitative learning skills was
             found for both chimpanzee and human sub‐jects. It is
             concluded that a human‐like sociocultural environment is
             an essential component in the development of human‐like
             social‐cognitive and joint attentional skills for
             chimpanzees, and perhaps for human beings as well Copyright
             © 1995, Wiley Blackwell. All rights reserved},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9507.1995.tb00063.x},
   Key = {fds351984}
}

@article{fds351985,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Commentary},
   Journal = {Human Development},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {46-52},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000278298},
   Doi = {10.1159/000278298},
   Key = {fds351985}
}

@article{fds351986,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Akhtar, N},
   Title = {Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference
             to objects and actions},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {201-224},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(95)90009-8},
   Abstract = {Previous studies have found that children can use
             social-pragmatic cues to determine "which one" of several
             objects or "which one' of several actions an adult intends
             to indicate with a novel word. The current studies attempted
             to determine whether children can also use such cues to
             determine "what kind" of referent, object, or action, an
             adult intends to indicate. In the first study, 27-month-old
             children heard an adult use a nonce word in conjunction with
             a nameless object while it was engaged in a nameless action.
             The discourse situation leading into this naming event was
             manipulated so that in one condition the target action was
             the one new element in the discourse context at the time of
             the naming event, and in another condition the target object
             was the one new element. Results showed that children
             learned the new word for whichever element was new to the
             discourse context. The second study followed this same
             general method, but in this case children in one condition
             watched as an adult engaged in preparatory behaviors that
             indicated her desire that the child perform the action
             before she produced the novel word, whereas children in
             another condition saw no such preparation. Results showed
             that children who saw the action preparation learned the new
             word for the action, whereas children who saw no preparation
             learned the new word for the object. These two studies
             demonstrate the important role of social-pragmatic
             information in early word learning, and suggest that if
             there is a Whole Object assumption in early lexical
             acquisition, it is an assumption that may be very easily
             overridden. © 1995.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0885-2014(95)90009-8},
   Key = {fds351986}
}

@article{fds351987,
   Author = {Byrnl, RW and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Do rats ape?},
   Journal = {Animal Behaviour},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1417-1420},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(95)80056-5},
   Doi = {10.1016/0003-3472(95)80056-5},
   Key = {fds351987}
}

@article{fds351989,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Production and comprehension of referential pointing by
             orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {108},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {307-317},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.108.4.307},
   Abstract = {We report 3 studies of the referential pointing of 2
             orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Chantek was raised in an
             enculturated environment; Puti, raised in a nursery, had a
             more typical captive life. In Experiment 1, flexibility of
             pointing behavior was investigated by requiring subjects to
             point in novel circumstances (for an out-of-sight tool, not
             food). In Experiment 2, we investigated the orangutans'
             comprehension of the significance of a human point in
             helping them to locate food. In Experiment 3, we
             investigated whether these pointing subjects comprehended
             that a human recipient must be looking for the point to
             achieve its attention-directing goal. In all experiments the
             enculturated orangutan showed better understanding of
             pointing than the captive orangutan. This finding is
             consistent with recent studies that have found differences
             in the cognitive and social-cognitive abilities of apes that
             have had different types of experience with
             humans.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.108.4.307},
   Key = {fds351989}
}

@article{fds351990,
   Author = {Call, J and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The social learning of tool use by orangutans (Pongo
             pygmaeus)},
   Journal = {Human Evolution},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {297-313},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02435516},
   Abstract = {Very little is known about the social learning of orangutans
             (Pongo pygmaeus), especially in the context of
             problem-solving situations such as tool use. Sixteen
             orangutans were presented with a rake-like tool and
             desirable but out-of-reach food. Eight subjects observed a
             human demonstrator use the tool in one way, while another
             eight observed the demonstrator use the tool in another way.
             Subjects behaved identically in the two experimental
             conditions, showing no effect of the type of demonstration
             observed. Analysis of individual learning curves suggested
             that a large component of individual trial-and-error
             learning was at work, even for two subjects who received
             additional trials with an orangutan demonstrator. This
             pattern of results suggests that subjects were paying
             attention to the general functional relations in the task
             and to the results obtained by the demonstrator, but not to
             the actual methods of tool use demonstrated. It is concluded
             that subjects in both conditions were employing emulation
             learning, not imitative learning. © 1994 International
             Institute for the Study of Man.},
   Doi = {10.1007/BF02435516},
   Key = {fds351990}
}

@article{fds351991,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Mervis, CB},
   Title = {THE INSTRUMENT IS GREAT, BUT MEASURING COMPREHENSION IS
             STILL A PROBLEM},
   Journal = {Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
             Development},
   Volume = {59},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {174-179},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb00186.x},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb00186.x},
   Key = {fds351991}
}

@article{fds351992,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Kruger, AC and Ratner, HH},
   Title = {The role of emotions in cultural learning},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {782-784},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00037195},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00037195},
   Key = {fds351992}
}

@article{fds351993,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J and Nagell, K and Olguin, R and Carpenter,
             M},
   Title = {The learning and use of gestural signals by young
             chimpanzees: A trans-generational study},
   Journal = {Primates},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {137-154},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02382050},
   Abstract = {Observations of chimpanzee gestural communication are
             reported. The observations represent the third longitudinal
             time point of an ongoing study of the Yerkes Primate Center
             Field Station chimpanzee group. In contrast to observations
             at the first two time points, the current observations are
             of a new generation of infants and juveniles. There were two
             questions. The first concerned how young chimpanzees used
             their gestures, with special focus on the flexibility or
             intentionality displayed. It was found that youngsters quite
             often used the same gesture in different contexts, and
             different gestures in the same context. In addition, they
             sometimes used gestures in combinations in a single social
             encounter, these combinations did not convey intentions that
             could not be conveyed by the component gestures, however. It
             was also found that individuals adjusted their choice of
             signals depending on the attentional state of the recipient.
             The second question was how chimpanzees acquired their
             gestural signals. In general, it was found that there was
             little consistency in the use of gestures among individuals,
             especially for non-play gestures, with much individual
             variability both within and across generations. There were
             also a number of idiosyncratic gestures used by single
             individuals at each time point. It was concluded from these
             results that youngsters were not imitatively learning their
             communicatory gestures from conspecifics, but rather that
             they were individually conventionalizing them with each
             other. Implications of these findings for the understanding
             of chimpanzee communication and social learning are
             discussed. © 1994 Japan Monkey Centre.},
   Doi = {10.1007/BF02382050},
   Key = {fds351993}
}

@article{fds351994,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Barton, M},
   Title = {Learning Words in Nonostensive Contexts},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {639-650},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.5.639},
   Abstract = {Four word learning studies with 24-month-old children are
             reported. In Studies 1 and 2, an adult used a novel word to
             announce her intention to perform an action or to find an
             object. It was found that a knowledge of what action or
             object was impending-established through scripted events
             before the word's introduction-was not necessary for
             children to learn the words. Studies 3 and 4 focused on what
             word learning cues children might be using in these
             contexts. In Study 3, it was found that children learned a
             novel verb for an intentional and not an accidental action.
             In Study 4, it was found that children learned a novel noun
             for an object the adult was searching for, not ones she had
             rejected while searching. Because none of the best-known
             constraints on lexical acquisition could have helped them in
             these contexts, it was concluded that children were relying
             on social-pragmatic cues to learn the new
             words.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.30.5.639},
   Key = {fds351994}
}

@article{fds366604,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Call, J},
   Title = {Social cognition of monkeys and apes},
   Journal = {American Journal of Physical Anthropology},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {19 S},
   Pages = {273-305},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330370610},
   Abstract = {This paper reviews what is known about the social cognition
             of monkeys and great apes. The literature reviewed is
             divided into three main content areas: (1) social
             interaction, including knowledge of individuals, knowledge
             of social relationships, alliance formation, and
             cooperation; (2) communication, including alarm calls, calls
             for recruiting allies, gestures, and the “language”
             skills of human‐raised apes; and (3) social learning,
             including the “cultural transmission” of
             food‐preparation behaviors, the social learning of tool
             use, and the social learning of vocal and gestural
             communication. Contrary to the hypotheses of a number of
             recent investigators, we find no compelling differences in
             the social cognition of monkeys and great apes. It is
             possible that differences in the social behavior of these
             two classes of primate are due to processes of nonsocial
             cognition, and it is possible that the social behavior of
             apes is more strongly influenced by human interaction and
             training.© 1994 Wiley‐Liss, Inc. Copyright © 1994
             Wiley‐Liss, Inc., A Wiley Company},
   Doi = {10.1002/ajpa.1330370610},
   Key = {fds366604}
}

@article{fds351995,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Savage-Rumbaugh, S and Kruger,
             AC},
   Title = {Imitative learning of actions on objects by children,
             chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {64},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1688-1705},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04207.x},
   Abstract = {In this study we compared the abilities of chimpanzees and
             human children to imitatively learn novel actions on
             objects. Of particular interest were possible differences
             between chimpanzees raised mostly with conspecifics
             (mother-reared) and chimpanzees raised in a human-like
             cultural environment (enculturated). Subjects were thus 3
             enculturated and 3 mother-reared chimpanzees, along with 8
             18-month-old and 8 30-month-old human children. Each subject
             was tested over a 2-day period with 16 novel objects. The
             introduction of each object was preceded by a baseline
             period in which the subject's natural proclivities toward
             the object were determined. For 12 objects, a human
             experimenter demonstrated first a simple and then a complex
             novel action, instructing the subject in each case to "Do
             what I do" (chimpanzees were prepared for the task
             behaviorally as well). For the other 4 objects,
             demonstration of a single action took place on the first day
             and the subject's opportunity to imitate was delayed until
             the second day, 48 hours later. Actions that a subject
             produced in baseline were excluded from further analysis.
             For each analyzed action, the subject's behavior was scored
             as to whether it successfully reproduced (1) the end result
             of the demonstrated action, and (2) the behavioral means
             used by the demonstrator. Results showed that in immediate
             imitation the mother-reared chimpanzees were much poorer
             imitators than the enculturated chimpanzees and the human
             children, who did not differ from one another. Surprisingly,
             on the delay trials, the enculturated chimpanzees
             significantly outperformed the other 3 groups. We conclude
             from these results that a human-like sociocultural
             environment is an essential component in the development of
             human-like social-cognitive and imitative learning skills
             for chimpanzees, and perhaps for human beings as
             well.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04207.x},
   Key = {fds351995}
}

@article{fds351996,
   Author = {Nagell, K and Olguin, RS and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees
             (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo
             sapiens).},
   Journal = {Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. :
             1983)},
   Volume = {107},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {174-186},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.107.2.174},
   Abstract = {Common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and 2-year-old human
             children (Homo sapiens) were presented with a rakelike tool
             and a desirable but out-of-reach object. One group of
             subjects observed a human demonstrator use the tool in one
             way, and another group observed a demonstrator use the tool
             in another way. Children in both cases did what the model
             did. Chimpanzee subjects, however, behaved identically in
             the 2 model conditions. Both groups performed better than
             subjects who saw no demonstration. This pattern of results
             suggest that the chimpanzees were paying attention to the
             general functional relations in the task and to the results
             obtained by the demonstrator but not to the actual methods
             of tool use demonstrated. Human children were focused on the
             demonstrator's actual methods of tool use (her behavior).
             The different social learning processes used by the 2
             species have implications for their different forms of
             social organization.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0735-7036.107.2.174},
   Key = {fds351996}
}

@article{fds351997,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {It's imitation, not mimesis},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {771-772},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032921},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00032921},
   Key = {fds351997}
}

@article{fds351998,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Olguin, R},
   Title = {Twenty-three-month-old children have a grammatical category
             of noun},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {451-464},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(05)80004-8},
   Abstract = {This study investigated experimentally the nature and
             development of children's early productivity with nouns,
             both in verb-argument structure and with plural morphology.
             Eight 20- to 26-month-old boys and girls were, in the
             context of playing a game over a several week period,
             exposed to four novel nouns, modeled in experimentally
             controlled ways. The question was whether, when, and in what
             ways the children would become productive with these nouns
             in their spontaneous speech, going beyond the particular
             linguistic forms they had heard. In terms of verb-argument
             structure, 7 of the 8 children used their nouns in
             productive argument roles, that is, in semantic roles they
             had not heard them used in. Five of the 8 children used the
             plural morpheme productively with the novel nouns as well.
             Implications for theories of grammatical category formation
             are discussed. © 1993, Ablex Publishing Corporation, 355
             Chestnut Street, Norwood, New Jersey 07648. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0885-2014(05)80004-8},
   Key = {fds351998}
}

@article{fds351999,
   Author = {Olguin, R and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Twenty-five-month-old children do not have a grammatical
             category of verb},
   Journal = {Cognitive Development},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {245-272},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(93)80001-A},
   Abstract = {This study investigated experimentally the nature and
             development of children's early productivity with
             verb-argument structure and verb morphology. Twenty-two to
             25-month-old boys and girls were, in the context of playing
             a game over a several week period, exposed to eight novel
             verbs modeled with experimentally controlled argument
             structures and verb inflections. The question was whether,
             when, and in what ways the children would become productive
             with these verbs in their spontaneous speech, going beyond
             the particular linguistic forms they had heard. In terms of
             verb-argument structure, the results showed that children
             most often followed the surface structure of the model,
             regardless of the argument they were trying to express.
             Thus, when children had heard an argument expressed for a
             verb, they almost always marked that argument correctly in
             their own utterances; when they had not heard an argument
             expressed for a particular verb, their correct marking
             dropped to chance levels. The children showed no signs of
             productive verb morphology, but they did use the newly
             learned verbs in some creative ways involving noun-like uses
             and the appending of locatives. Results are discussed in
             terms of Tomasello's (1992) Verb Island hypothesis. © 1993
             Ablex Publishing Corporation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0885-2014(93)80001-A},
   Key = {fds351999}
}

@article{fds352000,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Kruger, AC and Ratner, HH},
   Title = {Cultural learning},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {495-552},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0003123x},
   Abstract = {This target article presents a theory of human cultural
             learning. Cultural learning is identified with those
             instances of social learning in which intersubjectivity or
             perspective-taking plays a vital role, both in the original
             learning process and in the resulting cognitive product.
             Cultural learning manifests itself in three forms during
             human ontogeny: imitative learning, instructed learning, and
             collaborative learning - in that order. Evidence is provided
             that this progression arises from the developmental ordering
             of the underlying social-cognitive concepts and processes
             involved. Imitative learning relies on a concept of
             intentional agent and involves simple perspective-taking.
             Instructed learning relies on a concept of mental agent and
             involves alternating/coordinated perspective-taking
             (intersubjectivity). Collaborative learning relies on a
             concept of reflective agent and involves integrated
             perspective-taking reflective intersubjectivity). A
             comparison of normal children, autistic children and wild
             and enculturated chimpanzees provides further evidence for
             these correlations between social cognition and cultural
             learning. Cultural learning is a uniquely human form of
             social learning that allows for a fidelity of transmission
             of behaviors and information among conspecifics not possible
             in other forms of social learning, thereby providing the
             psychological basis for cultural evolution.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0140525x0003123x},
   Key = {fds352000}
}

@article{fds352001,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Kruger, AC},
   Title = {Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and
             non-ostensive contexts.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {311-333},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900011430},
   Abstract = {Two studies of verb learning are reported. The focus of both
             studies was on children in their second year of life
             learning verbs in various pragmatic contexts. Of particular
             interest was the comparison of ostensive contexts--in which
             word and referent were simultaneously present in the child's
             perceptual field--to non-ostensive contexts. In a
             naturalistic study of 24 children at 1;3 and 1;9, it was
             found that mothers modelled verbs for their children most
             often BEFORE the referent action actually occurred. Over 60%
             of maternal verbs were used to refer to actions that mothers
             wished children to perform or that they were anticipating
             their performing (IMPENDING actions). Some verbs were also
             used to refer to current actions (ONGOING actions) or
             actions that had just been completed (COMPLETED actions).
             Children responded with comprehension most often to
             impending models. Impending and completed models, but not
             ongoing models, were correlated with children's verb
             vocabularies at 1;9. The second study was a lexical training
             study of 48 two-year-olds. Children learned to produce a
             novel verb best when it was modelled in the impending
             condition. They learned to comprehend it equally well in the
             impending and completed conditions. Children showed no signs
             of superior learning in the ostensive (ongoing) learning
             context. Results of the two studies are discussed in terms
             of the different learning processes involved in acquiring
             nouns and verbs, and, more broadly, in terms of a
             social-pragmatic view of language acquisition in which the
             ostensive teaching paradigm is but one of many contexts in
             which children learn to establish a joint attentional focus
             with mature language users.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900011430},
   Key = {fds352001}
}

@article{fds352002,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The social bases of language acquisition},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {67-87},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00135.x},
   Abstract = {A language is composed of conventional symbols shaped by
             their social‐communicative functions. Children acquire
             these symbols, both lexical and syntactic, in the context of
             culturally constituted event structures that make salient
             these functions. In the acquisition process children rely on
             cultural learning skills (i.e., imitative learning). These
             skills emanate from their ability to participate
             intersubjectively with adults in cultural activities (i.e.,
             joint attention), which underlies their ability to
             understand the ways adults are using particular pieces of
             language. The development of communicative competence as a
             whole, including not only lexical and syntactic skills but
             also various pragmatic skills, depends largely on feedback
             about communicative efficacy that children receive from
             different interactants. This feedback is used by children to
             make further inferences about the conventional functional
             significance of particular linguistic expressions. This
             social‐pragmatic view of language acquisition obviates the
             need for a priori, specifically linguistic, format
             constraints on the language acquisition process. Copyright
             © 1992, Wiley Blackwell. All rights reserved},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00135.x},
   Key = {fds352002}
}

@article{fds352003,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Author's response: On defining language: Replies to Shatz
             and Ninio},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {159-162},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00121.x},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00121.x},
   Key = {fds352003}
}

@article{fds352004,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cognitive ethology comes of age},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {168-169},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00068163},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00068163},
   Key = {fds352004}
}

@article{fds352005,
   Author = {SECULES, T and HERRON, C and TOMASELLO, M},
   Title = {The Effect of Video Context on Foreign Language
             Learning},
   Journal = {The Modern Language Journal},
   Volume = {76},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {480-490},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1992.tb05396.x},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1540-4781.1992.tb05396.x},
   Key = {fds352005}
}

@article{fds352006,
   Author = {Mannle, S and Barton, M and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Two-year-olds' conversations with their mothers and
             preschool-aged siblings},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {34},
   Pages = {57-71},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272379201203404},
   Abstract = {The pragmatics of sibling-infant and mother-infant
             conversations were compared. Sixteen children, 22 to 26
             months of age, were videotaped for 15 minutes in dyadic
             interaction with their mothers and for 15 minutes in dyadic
             interaction with their preschool-aged siblings.
             Sibling-infant and mother-infant conversations were compared
             on three dimensions: quantitative characteristics,
             conversational style and conversational repair of potential
             breakdowns. Compared with mothers and infants, siblings and
             infants talked less and had shorter conversations. On an
             individual level, siblings asked fewer questions of the
             infants and issued more directives to them than did the
             mothers. Moreover, siblings failed to repair disruptions in
             conversations almost twice as often as mothers. The infants'
             conversational behaviours, however, did not differ when
             interacting with the siblings as opposed to the mothers.
             These results indicate that preschool-aged siblings are not
             yet adept at making the kinds of pragmatic adjustments in
             their speech that scaffold infants in their early
             conversational interactions. It is suggested that the
             experience later- born infants have with less responsive
             siblings may be valuable preparation for interacting with
             strangers, especially peers, who share many characteristics
             with siblings. © 1992, Sage Publications. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1177/014272379201203404},
   Key = {fds352006}
}

@article{fds352007,
   Author = {Barton, ME and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Joint Attention and Conversation in Mother‐Infant‐Sibling
             Triads},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {62},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {517-529},
   Year = {1991},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01548.x},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated the general nature of joint
             attentional and conversational interaction in
             mother‐infant‐sibling triads. 9 19‐month‐old infants
             and 9 24‐month‐old infants were videotaped during 20 min
             of free play with their mothers and preschool‐aged
             siblings around a common activity. Analyses revealed that
             even 19‐month‐old infants were capable of participating
             in triadic interactions and conversations, and that the
             proportional frequency of both these measures increased with
             age. Triadic conversations were nearly 3 times longer and
             elicited nearly twice as many infant turns per conversation
             as dyadic conversations. Infants were more likely to join
             into an ongoing conversational topic than to initiate one
             themselves, and they were more likely to take a turn in
             those conversations if they were in a joint attentional
             state with the speaker. Infants were just as likely to
             respond to a comment or request directed to another person
             as they were to one directed to themselves, indicating
             reliable comprehension of language not addressed to them.
             These results suggest that the mother‐infant‐sibling
             interactive context differs in important ways from the
             mother‐infant dyadic context and that it is a richer
             language learning environment than previously supposed.
             Copyright © 1991, Wiley Blackwell. All rights
             reserved},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01548.x},
   Key = {fds352007}
}

@article{fds352008,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Objects are analogous to words, not phonemes or grammatical
             categories},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {575-576},
   Year = {1991},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00071466},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00071466},
   Key = {fds352008}
}

@article{fds352009,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Herron, C},
   Title = {A Reply to Beck and Eubank},
   Journal = {Studies in Second Language Acquisition},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {513-517},
   Year = {1991},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100010317},
   Abstract = {Beck and Eubank (1991) criticize our recent SSLA article
             (Tomasello & Herron, 1989) on both theoretical and
             methodological grounds. While we appreciate their attempt to
             discuss and clarify important issues—and while they do
             make several sound and very interesting points—in a number
             of cases they seriously misrepresent our study. We will
             attempt to address the criticisms in roughly the order in
             which they were raised. © 1991, Cambridge University Press.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0272263100010317},
   Key = {fds352009}
}

@article{fds352010,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Conti-Ramsden, G and Ewert, B},
   Title = {Young children's conversations with their mothers and
             fathers: differences in breakdown and repair.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {115-130},
   Year = {1990},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900013131},
   Abstract = {In this study we compared the conversations of mothers and
             fathers with their children at 1; 3 and 1; 9, with special
             attention to breakdown-repair sequences. We found that,
             overall, children and secondary caregiver fathers
             experienced more communicative breakdowns than did children
             and primary caregiver mothers. More specifically, fathers
             requested clarification of their children more often than
             did mothers, and they most often used a non-specific query
             (e.g. What?). Mothers used more specific queries (e.g. Put
             it where?) and were involved in more 'looped' sequences
             involving multiple requests for clarification. Fathers also
             failed to acknowledge child utterances more often than did
             mothers. After a father non-acknowledgement, children tended
             not to persist and when they did they often received further
             non-acknowledgements; the dyad did not often return to the
             child's original topic. After a maternal
             non-acknowledgement, on the other hand, children persisted
             and the dyad more often returned to its previous topic. The
             results are interpreted as support for the Bridge Hypothesis
             which claims that fathers present children with
             communicative challenges that help prepare them for
             communication with less familiar adults.},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900013131},
   Key = {fds352010}
}

@article{fds366605,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Gust, DA and Evans, A},
   Title = {Peer interaction in infant chimpanzees.},
   Journal = {Folia primatologica; international journal of
             primatology},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {33-40},
   Year = {1990},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000156495},
   Abstract = {The peer interactions of 6 infant chimpanzees (Pan
             troglodytes) ranging in age from 18 to 50 months were
             observed in a seminatural context. The infants and their
             mothers lived as members of a captive social group at the
             Yerkes Regional Primate Research Center. An analysis of
             contact initiations between infants indicated that the most
             preferred peer interactant was the youngest and the least
             preferred was the oldest infant. Infants also initiated more
             interactions with the offspring of adults that had the
             closest relationships with both themselves and their
             mothers. These results indicate that a number of factors may
             influence the peer affiliations of infant chimpanzees,
             including the age of the infant and the mother's social
             relationships.},
   Doi = {10.1159/000156495},
   Key = {fds366605}
}

@article{fds352011,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Herron, C},
   Title = {Feedback for language transfer errors the garden path
             technique},
   Journal = {Studies in Second Language Acquisition},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {385-395},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100008408},
   Abstract = {In this study we compared two methods for correcting
             language transfer errors in the foreign language classroom.
             Thirty-two English-speaking college students enrolled in two
             sections of an introductory French course served as
             subjects. Eight commonly encountered English-to-French
             transfer errors were identified and randomly assigned to one
             of two teaching conditions for one class section; each error
             was assigned to the opposite condition for the other
             section. In both teaching conditions students began by
             translating English sentences into French. The sentences
             were such that an L1 (first language) transfer strategy
             produced correct translations (e.g., using savoir for some
             uses of “to know”). A sentence for which the transfer
             would not produce an adequate translation (e.g., a sentence
             requiring connaître) was then introduced in one of two
             ways. In one condition—what we have termed the Garden Path
             condition—students were given the new sentence and asked
             to translate as before. Their inevitable transfer error was
             then immediately corrected by the teacher. In the control
             condition students were simply given the correct French form
             and told that it differed from the English pattern (they
             were not given the opportunity to commit a transfer error).
             Student learning of the non-transferable form was assessed
             three times throughout the course of the semester, and at
             all time points performance was better in the Garden Path
             condition. We interpreted this finding as support for a
             cognitive comparison model of second language acquisition.
             © 1989, Cambridge University Press. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0272263100008408},
   Key = {fds352011}
}

@article{fds352012,
   Author = {Snow, CE and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Data on language input: Incomprehensible omission
             indeed!},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {357-358},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049104},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00049104},
   Key = {fds352012}
}

@article{fds352013,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Cognition as cause},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {607-608},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00073738},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00073738},
   Key = {fds352013}
}

@article{fds352014,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Gust, D and Frost, GT},
   Title = {A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in
             young chimpanzees},
   Journal = {Primates},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {35-50},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02381209},
   Abstract = {A longitudinal study of chimpanzee gestural communication is
             reported. Subjects were seven 5- to 8-year-old members of a
             semi-natural group at the Yerkes Field Station. These were
             the same individuals observed by Tomasello et al. (1985)
             four years previously. Nearly identical operational
             definitions and observational procedures were used in the
             two studies. Longitudinal comparisons between the two
             observation periods revealed that the development of
             chimpanzee gestural communication is best characterized as a
             series of ontogenetic adaptations: as particular social
             functions (e.g., nursing, playing, grooming, etc.) arise,
             decline, or change, gestural communication follows suit.
             Most gestures seem to be conventionalized by individuals in
             direct social interaction with conspecifics. Some gestures
             may be learned by "second-person imitation"-an individual
             copying a behavior directed to it by another individual. No
             evidence was found for "third-person imitation"-an
             individual copying a gesture used between two other
             individuals. Implications for the concept of chimpanzee
             "culture" are discussed. © 1989 Japan Monkey
             Centre.},
   Doi = {10.1007/BF02381209},
   Key = {fds352014}
}

@article{fds352015,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Mannle, S and Werdenschlag, L},
   Title = {The effect of previously learned words on the child's
             acquisition of words for similar referents.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {505-515},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900012538},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900012538},
   Key = {fds352015}
}

@article{fds352016,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Herron, C},
   Title = {Down the Garden Path: Inducing and correcting
             overgeneralization errors in the foreign language
             classroom},
   Journal = {Applied Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {237-246},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400007827},
   Abstract = {In this study we compared two methods for teaching
             grammatical exceptions in the foreign language classroom.
             Thirty-nine students in two sections of an introductory
             college French course served as subjects. Eight target
             structures, exemplifying –exceptions to a rule,– were
             randomly assigned to one of two teaching conditions for a
             section taught in the spring; each structure was assigned to
             the opposite teaching condition for a section taught the
             following fall. In one condition we simply taught the
             students the exception as an exception. In the other –
             what we called the Garden Path condition – we presented
             canonical exemplars encouraging students to induce the rule;
             we then asked them to generate the form (which we knew to be
             an exception) and then corrected their resulting
             overgeneralization error. Analysis of subsequent formal
             testing showed that students learned the exception better in
             the Garden Path condition and that this advantage persisted
             throughout the semesterlong course. We hypothesized that
             this technique helped students to focus attention both on
             the rule and on the features of the particular structure
             that marked it as an exception. © 1988, Cambridge
             University Press. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0142716400007827},
   Key = {fds352016}
}

@article{fds352017,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Snow, CE},
   Title = {Well-fed organisms still need feedback},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {475-476},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00058568},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00058568},
   Key = {fds352017}
}

@article{fds352018,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The role of joint attentional processes in early language
             development},
   Journal = {Language Sciences},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {69-88},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0388-0001(88)90006-X},
   Abstract = {In this paper I examine the role of joint attentional
             processes in the child's early lexical acquisition and
             conversational interaction. In both cases I conclude that
             relatively extended periods of adult-child joint attentional
             focus on nonlinguistic entities, perhaps as manifest in
             routines, scaffold the child's early language development.
             On the other hand, adult directiveness - whether of child
             behavior/attention or of the dyad's conversational topic -
             has a negative effect on early language development. For
             both lexical acquisition and conversational interaction some
             findings from experimental studies are available to
             supplement conclusions based on correlational evidence.
             Based on these findings, I propose a developmental sequence
             of joint attentional processes in early language development
             and discuss the role of adults in the child's passage
             through this sequence. © 1988.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0388-0001(88)90006-X},
   Key = {fds352018}
}

@article{fds352019,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Davis-Dasilva, M and Camak, L and Bard,
             K},
   Title = {Observational learning of tool-use by young
             chimpanzees},
   Journal = {Human Evolution},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {175-183},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02436405},
   Abstract = {In the current study two groups of young chimpanzees (4-6
             and 8-9 years old) were given a T-bar and a food item that
             could only be reached by using the T-bar. Experimental
             subjects were given the opportunity to observe an adult
             using the stick as a tool to obtain the food; control
             subjects were exposed to the adult but were given no
             demonstration. Subjects in the older group did not learn to
             use the tool. Subjects in the younger group who were exposed
             to the demonstrator learned to use the stick as a tool much
             more readily than those who were not. None of the subjects
             demonstrated an ability to imitatively copy the
             demonstrator's precise behavioral strategies. More than
             simple stimulus enhancement was involved, however, since
             both groups manipulated the T-bar, but only experimental
             subjects used it in its function as a tool. Our findings
             complement naturalistic observations in suggesting that
             chimpanzee tool-use is in some sense «culturally
             transmitted» - though perhaps not in the same sense as
             social-conventional behaviors for which precise copying of
             conspecifics is crucial. © 1987 Editrice II
             Sedicesimo.},
   Doi = {10.1007/BF02436405},
   Key = {fds352019}
}

@article{fds352507,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Learning to use prepositions: a case study.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {79-98},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900012745},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900012745},
   Key = {fds352507}
}

@article{fds352020,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Why the left hand?},
   Journal = {Behavioral and Brain Sciences},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {286-287},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00047919},
   Doi = {10.1017/S0140525X00047919},
   Key = {fds352020}
}

@article{fds352021,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Farrar, MJ},
   Title = {Joint attention and early language.},
   Journal = {Child development},
   Volume = {57},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1454-1463},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1986.tb00470.x},
   Abstract = {This paper reports 2 studies that explore the role of joint
             attentional processes in the child's acquisition of
             language. In the first study, 24 children were videotaped at
             15 and 21 months of age in naturalistic interaction with
             their mothers. Episodes of joint attentional focus between
             mother and child--for example, joint play with an
             object--were identified. Inside, as opposed to outside,
             these episodes both mothers and children produced more
             utterances, mothers used shorter sentences and more
             comments, and dyads engaged in longer conversations. Inside
             joint episodes maternal references to objects that were
             already the child's focus of attention were positively
             correlated with the child's vocabulary at 21 months, while
             object references that attempted to redirect the child's
             attention were negatively correlated. No measures from
             outside these episodes related to child language. In an
             experimental study, an adult attempted to teach novel words
             to 10 17-month-old children. Words referring to objects on
             which the child's attention was already focused were learned
             better than words presented in an attempt to redirect the
             child's attentional focus.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1986.tb00470.x},
   Key = {fds352021}
}

@article{fds352022,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Farrar, MJ},
   Title = {Object permanence and relational words: a lexical training
             study.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {495-505},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s030500090000684x},
   Doi = {10.1017/s030500090000684x},
   Key = {fds352022}
}

@article{fds352023,
   Author = {Kruger, AC and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Transactive Discussions With Peers and Adults},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {681-685},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.5.681},
   Abstract = {Piaget hypothesized that peer and adult-child discussions of
             moral dilemmas are qualitatively different. He asserted that
             children are more likely to use reasoning when interacting
             with peers. To test this hypothesis, the present study
             compared the interactive styles of child-child and
             adult-child dyads engaged in discussions of moral dilemmas,
             focusing on the use of logical operations (transacts).
             Forty-eight female subjects, ages 7 and 11 years, were
             paired with either a female agemate or their mother.
             Children used transacts in a higher proportion of their
             conversational turns when interacting with peers than when
             interacting with mothers. Subjects produced proportionally
             more transactive responses when interacting with mothers
             because mothers produced proportionally more requests for
             idea clarification than did peer partners. Self-generated
             transacts, on the other hand, were produced proportionally
             more often with peers. Furthermore, when paired with peers,
             children produced transactive statements that operated on
             the partner's logic more often, rather than clarifying their
             own logic. These results support Piaget's contention that
             moral discussions with peers feature a more spontaneous use
             of reasoning than do discussions with adults. © 1986
             American Psychological Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.22.5.681},
   Key = {fds352023}
}

@article{fds352024,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Mannle, S and Kruger, AC},
   Title = {Linguistic Environment of 1- to 2-Year-Old
             Twins},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {169-176},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.2.169},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated differences in the language
             learning environments of singletons and twins, with special
             reference to pragmatic factors that might be expected to
             differ in dyadic and triadic interactive situations. Six
             twin pairs and 12 singleton children (all firstborn) were
             observed in natural interactions with their mothers, once at
             15 months of age and again at 21 months of age. Twins were
             lower than singletons on all measures of language
             development. The language learning environments of the two
             groups differed as well. Although twin mothers spoke and
             interacted with their children as much as singleton mothers
             when twins were analyzed together, when analyzed from the
             point of view of the individual twin child, twin children
             received less speech directed specifically to them,
             participated in fewer and shorter episodes of joint
             attentional focus, and had fewer and shorter conversations
             with their mothers. In addition, twin mothers were more
             directive in their interactional styles. Correlational
             analyses indicated that variation of these language learning
             environment factors for the sample as a whole, as well as
             variation for some of these factors within the twin group
             itself, was related to early language growth. It is proposed
             that both the quantitative and qualitative differences
             observed in the language learning environments of singletons
             and twins derive from the nature of the triadic situation
             and that these differences have important effects on the
             child's early language development. © 1986 American
             Psychological Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.22.2.169},
   Key = {fds352024}
}

@article{fds352025,
   Author = {Anselmi, D and Tomasello, M and Acunzo, M},
   Title = {Young children's responses to neutral and specific
             contingent queries.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {135-144},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900000349},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900000349},
   Key = {fds352025}
}

@article{fds352026,
   Author = {Evans, A and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Evidence for social referencing in young chimpanzees (Pan
             troglodytes).},
   Journal = {Folia primatologica; international journal of
             primatology},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {49-54},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000156263},
   Abstract = {A captive chimpanzee group was observed in order to
             determine the extent to which the social interactions of the
             infants and juveniles (18-50 months) were affected by their
             mothers' relationships with other adult group members. It
             was found that the young chimpanzees initiated more
             interactions with adults who interacted more with their
             mothers. A vast majority of those interactions occurred at
             significant distances from the mother. It is argued that
             these data imply a social-cognitive ability in young
             chimpanzees closely related to the human infant's ability to
             use its mother in 'social referencing'.},
   Doi = {10.1159/000156263},
   Key = {fds352026}
}

@article{fds352027,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and George, BL and Kruger, AC and Jeffrey, M and Farrar, and Evans, A},
   Title = {The development of gestural communication in young
             chimpanzees},
   Journal = {Journal of Human Evolution},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {175-186},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(85)80005-1},
   Abstract = {Plooij (Action, Gesture and Symbol, Academic Press 1978;
             Before Speech, C.U.P. 1979) described some
             intentionally-produced communicatory gestures used by
             one-year-old chimpanzees on the Gombe Stream Reserve. The
             current study investigated the use of this type of gesture
             at later developmental periods. Subjects were five infant
             and juvenile chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) living in a
             semi-natural group at the Yerkes Regional Primate Center
             Field Station. On the basis of naturalistic observations,
             three stages in the development of communicatory gestures
             were determined: (1) One-year-old infants used some
             gestures, but only in an immature form and only with their
             mothers or with peers; (2) Two-year-olds produced more
             gestures which were clearly intentional and conventional
             (they waited for a response), and they directed them to all
             group members; (3) Three-year-olds used a wider variety of
             gestures, and they supplemented them with a
             "gaze-alternation" behavior which indicated even more
             clearly the goal of the communication. Many of the gestures
             used by infants and juveniles were not used by adults, thus
             indicating a significance confined to specific developmental
             periods. This contradicts the commonly-held assumption (e.g.
             Van Lawick-Goodall, 1967) that the developmental process is
             one in which young chimpanzees come gradually to learn a
             pre-existing set of adult communicatory gestures. From this
             and other evidence, it is argued that, while some of the
             gestures are learned observationally, many are learned
             through a process of "direct convention-alization" between
             animals, and others rely on both of these processes. © 1985
             Academic Press Inc. (London) Limited.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0047-2484(85)80005-1},
   Key = {fds352027}
}

@article{fds352028,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Farrar, MJ},
   Title = {Cognitive bases of lexical development: object permanence
             and relational words.},
   Journal = {Journal of child language},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {477-493},
   Year = {1984},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900005900},
   Doi = {10.1017/s0305000900005900},
   Key = {fds352028}
}

@article{fds352029,
   Author = {Tomasello, M and Farrar, MJ and Dines, J},
   Title = {Children's speech revisions for a familiar and an unfamiliar
             adult.},
   Journal = {Journal of speech and hearing research},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {359-363},
   Year = {1984},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.2703.359},
   Abstract = {Initial characterizations of the communicative abilities of
             preschoolers stressed their egocentric nature. Recently,
             however, even 2-year-olds have been observed to adjust their
             speech appropriately in situations in which the listener
             provides feedback by signaling noncomprehension. The current
             study had an adult signal noncomprehension to the requests
             of 2-year-old Stage I and Stage II children. Each child
             interacted with a familiar (mother) and an unfamiliar adult.
             The children repeated their requests about one third of the
             time and revised them about two thirds of the time. Stage I
             children elaborated their requests significantly more often
             than Stage II children. The familiarity of the adult
             listener had no effect on the way Stage II children revised
             their requests, but the Stage I children's revisions
             contained novel lexical items more often when they were
             interacting with the unfamiliar adult. Both of these
             findings may have resulted from the fact that the more
             conversationally skilled Stage II children relied on
             verbal-conversational cues, which were the same for both
             adult interactants in this situation. The Stage I children
             may have been less aware of these conversational cues,
             relying on general social cues such as familiarity of the
             interactant. The results are discussed in terms of the
             potential role of different types of adults in the language
             acquisition process.},
   Doi = {10.1044/jshr.2703.359},
   Key = {fds352029}
}

@article{fds352030,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Young children's coordination of gestural and linguistic
             reference},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {15},
   Pages = {199-209},
   Year = {1984},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272378400501503},
   Abstract = {The current study investigated the relationship between
             young children's linguistic and nonlinguistic communicative
             strategies. Twenty-three children, 20-44 months of age,
             served as subjects. In a naturalistic setting, an adult gave
             signs of noncomprehension (a contingent query) to each of
             the child's object references. The child's original
             linguistic reference and use of gestures were recorded and
             compared to his/her subsequent linguistic and gestural
             responses to the adult query. Results showed that the
             children used gestures more often with pronouns than with
             nouns: either to clarify a linguistic reference from the
             original utterance or to supplement a linguistic response to
             the adult query. This would imply that two- to three-year-
             old children are aware of the communicative principle that
             pronouns 'need' gestures more than nouns, and more
             generally, that they are capable of coordinating their
             linguistic and nonlinguistic communi cative strategies. ©
             1984, Sage Publications. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1177/014272378400501503},
   Key = {fds352030}
}

@article{fds352031,
   Author = {George, BL and Tomasello, M},
   Title = {The effect of variation in sentence length on young
             children's attention and comprehension},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {14},
   Pages = {115-127},
   Year = {1984},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272378400501403},
   Abstract = {The effect of sentence length on children's attention and
             com prehension was studied. Twenty-five two- to
             five-year-olds were placed into three groups, High, Middle,
             and Low, based on their mean length of utterance (MLU).
             Subsequently, each child watched three videotaped stories,
             each having the same number of words but a different MLU.
             The time the child spent gazing at the monitor was measured.
             Comprehension was measured by a picture choice task. Results
             indicated that the High group attended most to the Long
             Level, comprehended obvious content best at the Long Level,
             and comprehended subtle content best at the Medium Level;
             the Low group attended most to the Medium Level and
             comprehended little. It was concluded that both input level
             and child level differentially affect attention and
             comprehension. © 1984, Sage Publications. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1177/014272378400501403},
   Key = {fds352031}
}

@article{fds352032,
   Author = {Tomasello, M},
   Title = {Joint attention and lexical acquisition style},
   Journal = {First Language},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {197-211},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272378300401202},
   Abstract = {Recent research has documented systematic individual
             differences in early lexical development. The current study
             investigated the relation ship of these differences to
             differences in the way mothers and children regulate each
             other's attentional states. Mothers of 6 one-year-olds kept
             diary records and were videotaped with their children at
             monthly intervals as well. Language measures from the diary
             were related to measures of attention manipulation and
             maintenance derived from a coding of the videotaped
             interactions. Results showed that when mothers initiated
             interactions by directing their child's attention, rather
             than by following into it, their child learned fewer object
             labels and more personal-social words. Dyads who maintained
             sustained bouts of joint attentional focus had children with
             larger vocabularies overall. It was concluded that the way
             mothers and children regulate each other's attention is an
             important factor in children's early lexical development. ©
             1983, Sage Publications. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1177/014272378300401202},
   Key = {fds352032}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * BAA * Faculty All * Postdoc Staff * Non-PHD Staff * Staff * Grads * Reload * Login