Philosophy Faculty Database
Philosophy
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > Philosophy > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Dale Purves    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Books   
@book{fds370321,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Why Brains Don't Compute},
   Pages = {1-168},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {May},
   ISBN = {9783030710637},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-71064-4},
   Abstract = {This book examines what seems to be the basic challenge in
             neuroscience today: understanding how experience generated
             by the human brain is related to the physical world we live
             in. The 25 short chapters present the argument and evidence
             that brains address this problem on a wholly trial and error
             basis. The goal is to encourage neuroscientists, computer
             scientists, philosophers, and other interested readers to
             consider this concept of neural function and its
             implications, not least of which is the conclusion that
             brains don't "compute."},
   Doi = {10.1007/978-3-030-71064-4},
   Key = {fds370321}
}


%% Papers Published   
@article{fds345466,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Opinion: What does AI's success playing complex board games
             tell brain scientists?},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {116},
   Number = {30},
   Pages = {14785-14787},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1909565116},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1909565116},
   Key = {fds345466}
}

@article{fds346898,
   Author = {Ng, CJ and Purves, D},
   Title = {An Alternative Theory of Binocularity.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience},
   Volume = {13},
   Pages = {71},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2019.00071},
   Abstract = {The fact that seeing with two eyes is universal among
             vertebrates raises a problem that has long challenged vision
             scientists: how do animals with overlapping visual fields
             combine non-identical right and left eye images to achieve
             fusion and the perception of depth that follows? Most
             theories address this problem in terms of matching
             corresponding images on the right and left retinas. Here we
             suggest an alternative theory of binocular vision based on
             anatomical correspondence that circumvents the
             correspondence problem and provides a rationale for ocular
             dominance.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fncom.2019.00071},
   Key = {fds346898}
}

@article{fds336007,
   Author = {Bowling, DL and Purves, D and Gill, KZ},
   Title = {Reply to Goffinet: In consonance, old ideas die
             hard.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {115},
   Number = {22},
   Pages = {E4958-E4959},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1805570115},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1805570115},
   Key = {fds336007}
}

@article{fds331493,
   Author = {Bowling, DL and Purves, D and Gill, KZ},
   Title = {Vocal similarity predicts the relative attraction of musical
             chords.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {115},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {216-221},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1713206115},
   Abstract = {Musical chords are combinations of two or more tones played
             together. While many different chords are used in music,
             some are heard as more attractive (consonant) than others.
             We have previously suggested that, for reasons of biological
             advantage, human tonal preferences can be understood in
             terms of the spectral similarity of tone combinations to
             harmonic human vocalizations. Using the chromatic scale, we
             tested this theory further by assessing the perceived
             consonance of all possible dyads, triads, and tetrads within
             a single octave. Our results show that the consonance of
             chords is predicted by their relative similarity to voiced
             speech sounds. These observations support the hypothesis
             that the relative attraction of musical tone combinations is
             due, at least in part, to the biological advantages that
             accrue from recognizing and responding to conspecific vocal
             stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1713206115},
   Key = {fds331493}
}

@article{fds348739,
   Author = {Purves, D and Yegappan, C},
   Title = {The Demands of Geometry on Color Vision.},
   Journal = {Vision (Basel, Switzerland)},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {E9},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/vision1010009},
   Abstract = {While studies of human color vision have made enormous
             strides, an overarching rationale for the circular sense of
             color relationships generated by two classes of color
             opponent neurons and three cone types is still lacking. Here
             we suggest that color circularity, color opponency and
             trichromacy may have arisen, at least in part, because of
             the geometrical requirements needed to unambiguously
             distinguish all possible spectrally different regions on a
             plane.},
   Doi = {10.3390/vision1010009},
   Key = {fds348739}
}

@article{fds268347,
   Author = {Bowling, DL and Purves, D},
   Title = {A biological rationale for musical consonance.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {112},
   Number = {36},
   Pages = {11155-11160},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1505768112},
   Abstract = {The basis of musical consonance has been debated for
             centuries without resolution. Three interpretations have
             been considered: (i) that consonance derives from the
             mathematical simplicity of small integer ratios; (ii) that
             consonance derives from the physical absence of interference
             between harmonic spectra; and (iii) that consonance derives
             from the advantages of recognizing biological vocalization
             and human vocalization in particular. Whereas the
             mathematical and physical explanations are at odds with the
             evidence that has now accumulated, biology provides a
             plausible explanation for this central issue in music and
             audition.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1505768112},
   Key = {fds268347}
}

@article{fds359924,
   Author = {Purves, D and Morgenstern, Y and Wojtach, WT},
   Title = {Will understanding vision require a wholly empirical
             paradigm?},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
   Volume = {6},
   Pages = {1072},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01072},
   Abstract = {Based on electrophysiological and anatomical studies, a
             prevalent conception is that the visual system recovers
             features of the world from retinal images to generate
             perceptions and guide behavior. This paradigm, however, is
             unable to explain why visual perceptions differ from
             physical measurements, or how behavior could routinely
             succeed on this basis. An alternative is that vision does
             not recover features of the world, but assigns perceptual
             qualities empirically by associating frequently occurring
             stimulus patterns with useful responses on the basis of
             survival and reproductive success. The purpose of the
             present article is to briefly describe this strategy of
             vision and the evidence for it.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01072},
   Key = {fds359924}
}

@article{fds323313,
   Author = {Purves, D and Morgenstern, Y and Wojtach, WT},
   Title = {Perception and Reality: Why a Wholly Empirical Paradigm is
             Needed to Understand Vision.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience},
   Volume = {9},
   Pages = {156},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00156},
   Abstract = {A central puzzle in vision science is how perceptions that
             are routinely at odds with physical measurements of real
             world properties can arise from neural responses that
             nonetheless lead to effective behaviors. Here we argue that
             the solution depends on: (1) rejecting the assumption that
             the goal of vision is to recover, however imperfectly,
             properties of the world; and (2) replacing it with a
             paradigm in which perceptions reflect biological utility
             based on past experience rather than objective features of
             the environment. Present evidence is consistent with the
             conclusion that conceiving vision in wholly empirical terms
             provides a plausible way to understand what we see and
             why.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fnsys.2015.00156},
   Key = {fds323313}
}

@article{fds268349,
   Author = {Morgenstern, Y and Rostami, M and Purves, D},
   Title = {Properties of artificial networks evolved to contend with
             natural spectra.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {111 Suppl 3},
   Pages = {10868-10872},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1402669111},
   Abstract = {Understanding why spectra that are physically the same
             appear different in different contexts (color contrast),
             whereas spectra that are physically different appear similar
             (color constancy) presents a major challenge in vision
             research. Here, we show that the responses of biologically
             inspired neural networks evolved on the basis of accumulated
             experience with spectral stimuli automatically generate
             contrast and constancy. The results imply that these
             phenomena are signatures of a strategy that biological
             vision uses to circumvent the inverse optics problem as it
             pertains to light spectra, and that double-opponent neurons
             in early-level vision evolve to serve this purpose. This
             strategy provides a way of understanding the peculiar
             relationship between the objective world and subjective
             color experience, as well as rationalizing the relevant
             visual circuitry without invoking feature detection or image
             representation.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1402669111},
   Key = {fds268349}
}

@article{fds268350,
   Author = {Purves, D and Monson, BB and Sundararajan, J and Wojtach,
             WT},
   Title = {How biological vision succeeds in the physical
             world.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {111},
   Number = {13},
   Pages = {4750-4755},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1311309111},
   Abstract = {Biological visual systems cannot measure the properties that
             define the physical world. Nonetheless, visually guided
             behaviors of humans and other animals are routinely
             successful. The purpose of this article is to consider how
             this feat is accomplished. Most concepts of vision propose,
             explicitly or implicitly, that visual behavior depends on
             recovering the sources of stimulus features either directly
             or by a process of statistical inference. Here we argue
             that, given the inability of the visual system to access the
             properties of the world, these conceptual frameworks cannot
             account for the behavioral success of biological vision. The
             alternative we present is that the visual system links the
             frequency of occurrence of biologically determined stimuli
             to useful perceptual and behavioral responses without
             recovering real-world properties. The evidence for this
             interpretation of vision is that the frequency of occurrence
             of stimulus patterns predicts many basic aspects of what we
             actually see. This strategy provides a different way of
             conceiving the relationship between objective reality and
             subjective experience, and offers a way to understand the
             operating principles of visual circuitry without invoking
             feature detection, representation, or probabilistic
             inference.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1311309111},
   Key = {fds268350}
}

@article{fds268348,
   Author = {Morgenstern, Y and Rukmini, DV and Monson, BB and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Properties of artificial neurons that report lightness based
             on accumulated experience with luminance.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience},
   Volume = {8},
   Pages = {134},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2014.00134},
   Abstract = {The responses of visual neurons in experimental animals have
             been extensively characterized. To ask whether these
             responses are consistent with a wholly empirical concept of
             visual perception, we optimized simple neural networks that
             responded according to the cumulative frequency of
             occurrence of local luminance patterns in retinal images.
             Based on this estimation of accumulated experience, the
             neuron responses showed classical center-surround receptive
             fields, luminance gain control and contrast gain control,
             the key properties of early level visual neurons determined
             in animal experiments. These results imply that a major
             purpose of pre-cortical neuronal circuitry is to contend
             with the inherently uncertain significance of luminance
             values in natural stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fncom.2014.00134},
   Key = {fds268348}
}

@article{fds268352,
   Author = {Monson, BB and Han, S and Purves, D},
   Title = {Are auditory percepts determined by experience?},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {e63728},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23667666},
   Abstract = {Audition--what listeners hear--is generally studied in terms
             of the physical properties of sound stimuli and
             physiological properties of the auditory system. Based on
             recent work in vision, we here consider an alternative
             perspective that sensory percepts are based on past
             experience. In this framework, basic auditory qualities
             (e.g., loudness and pitch) are based on the frequency of
             occurrence of stimulus patterns in natural acoustic stimuli.
             To explore this concept of audition, we examined five
             well-documented psychophysical functions. The frequency of
             occurrence of acoustic patterns in a database of natural
             sound stimuli (speech) predicts some qualitative aspects of
             these functions, but with substantial quantitative
             discrepancies. This approach may offer a rationale for
             auditory phenomena that are difficult to explain in terms of
             the physical attributes of the stimuli as
             such.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0063728},
   Key = {fds268352}
}

@article{fds268353,
   Author = {Ng, C and Sundararajan, J and Hogan, M and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Network connections that evolve to circumvent the inverse
             optics problem.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e60490},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23555981},
   Abstract = {A fundamental problem in vision science is how useful
             perceptions and behaviors arise in the absence of
             information about the physical sources of retinal stimuli
             (the inverse optics problem). Psychophysical studies show
             that human observers contend with this problem by using the
             frequency of occurrence of stimulus patterns in cumulative
             experience to generate percepts. To begin to understand the
             neural mechanisms underlying this strategy, we examined the
             connectivity of simple neural networks evolved to respond
             according to the cumulative rank of stimulus luminance
             values. Evolved similarities with the connectivity of early
             level visual neurons suggests that biological visual
             circuitry uses the same mechanisms as a means of creating
             useful perceptions and behaviors without information about
             the real world.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0060490},
   Key = {fds268353}
}

@article{fds268472,
   Author = {Bowling, DL and Sundararajan, J and Han, S and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Expression of emotion in Eastern and Western music mirrors
             vocalization.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {e31942},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22431970},
   Abstract = {In Western music, the major mode is typically used to convey
             excited, happy, bright or martial emotions, whereas the
             minor mode typically conveys subdued, sad or dark emotions.
             Recent studies indicate that the differences between these
             modes parallel differences between the prosodic and spectral
             characteristics of voiced speech sounds uttered in
             corresponding emotional states. Here we ask whether tonality
             and emotion are similarly linked in an Eastern musical
             tradition. The results show that the tonal relationships
             used to express positive/excited and negative/subdued
             emotions in classical South Indian music are much the same
             as those used in Western music. Moreover, tonal variations
             in the prosody of English and Tamil speech uttered in
             different emotional states are parallel to the tonal trends
             in music. These results are consistent with the hypothesis
             that the association between musical tonality and emotion is
             based on universal vocal characteristics of different
             affective states.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0031942},
   Key = {fds268472}
}

@article{fds268471,
   Author = {Purves, D and Wojtach, WT and Lotto, RB},
   Title = {Understanding vision in wholly empirical
             terms.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {108 Suppl 3},
   Pages = {15588-15595},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21383192},
   Abstract = {This article considers visual perception, the nature of the
             information on which perceptions seem to be based, and the
             implications of a wholly empirical concept of perception and
             sensory processing for vision science. Evidence from studies
             of lightness, brightness, color, form, and motion all
             indicate that, because the visual system cannot access the
             physical world by means of retinal light patterns as such,
             what we see cannot and does not represent the actual
             properties of objects or images. The phenomenology of visual
             perceptions can be explained, however, in terms of empirical
             associations that link images whose meanings are inherently
             undetermined to their behavioral significance. Vision in
             these terms requires fundamentally different concepts of
             what we see, why, and how the visual system
             operates.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.1012178108},
   Key = {fds268471}
}

@article{fds268470,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Clarke, R and Corney, D and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Seeing in colour},
   Journal = {Optics & Laser Technology},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {261-269},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0030-3992},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.optlastec.2010.02.006},
   Abstract = {Understanding perception of colour is challenging because
             what we see is not always what is there, which is a
             phenomenon we call illusions. Here we review the nature of
             colour vision, and the problems facing most current models
             and explanations. Focusing on our recent research on humans,
             bees and computers, we describe a new, more ecologically
             based explanation that provides a clear framework for why we
             see what we do. © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.optlastec.2010.02.006},
   Key = {fds268470}
}

@article{fds268469,
   Author = {Han, SE and Sundararajan, J and Bowling, DL and Lake, J and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Co-variation of tonality in the music and speech of
             different cultures.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {e20160},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21637716},
   Abstract = {Whereas the use of discrete pitch intervals is
             characteristic of most musical traditions, the size of the
             intervals and the way in which they are used is culturally
             specific. Here we examine the hypothesis that these
             differences arise because of a link between the tonal
             characteristics of a culture's music and its speech. We
             tested this idea by comparing pitch intervals in the
             traditional music of three tone language cultures (Chinese,
             Thai and Vietnamese) and three non-tone language cultures
             (American, French and German) with pitch intervals between
             voiced speech segments. Changes in pitch direction occur
             more frequently and pitch intervals are larger in the music
             of tone compared to non-tone language cultures. More
             frequent changes in pitch direction and larger pitch
             intervals are also apparent in the speech of tone compared
             to non-tone language cultures. These observations suggest
             that the different tonal preferences apparent in music
             across cultures are closely related to the differences in
             the tonal characteristics of voiced speech.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0020160},
   Key = {fds268469}
}

@article{fds268466,
   Author = {Bowling, DL and Gill, K and Choi, JD and Prinz, J and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Major and minor music compared to excited and subdued
             speech.},
   Journal = {The Journal of the Acoustical Society of
             America},
   Volume = {127},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {491-503},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20058994},
   Abstract = {The affective impact of music arises from a variety of
             factors, including intensity, tempo, rhythm, and tonal
             relationships. The emotional coloring evoked by intensity,
             tempo, and rhythm appears to arise from association with the
             characteristics of human behavior in the corresponding
             condition; however, how and why particular tonal
             relationships in music convey distinct emotional effects are
             not clear. The hypothesis examined here is that major and
             minor tone collections elicit different affective reactions
             because their spectra are similar to the spectra of voiced
             speech uttered in different emotional states. To evaluate
             this possibility the spectra of the intervals that
             distinguish major and minor music were compared to the
             spectra of voiced segments in excited and subdued speech
             using fundamental frequency and frequency ratios as
             measures. Consistent with the hypothesis, the spectra of
             major intervals are more similar to spectra found in excited
             speech, whereas the spectra of particular minor intervals
             are more similar to the spectra of subdued speech. These
             results suggest that the characteristic affective impact of
             major and minor tone collections arises from associations
             routinely made between particular musical intervals and
             voiced speech.},
   Doi = {10.1121/1.3268504},
   Key = {fds268466}
}

@article{fds268465,
   Author = {Gill, KZ and Purves, D},
   Title = {A biological rationale for musical scales.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {e8144},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19997506},
   Abstract = {Scales are collections of tones that divide octaves into
             specific intervals used to create music. Since humans can
             distinguish about 240 different pitches over an octave in
             the mid-range of hearing, in principle a very large number
             of tone combinations could have been used for this purpose.
             Nonetheless, compositions in Western classical, folk and
             popular music as well as in many other musical traditions
             are based on a relatively small number of scales that
             typically comprise only five to seven tones. Why humans
             employ only a few of the enormous number of possible tone
             combinations to create music is not known. Here we show that
             the component intervals of the most widely used scales
             throughout history and across cultures are those with the
             greatest overall spectral similarity to a harmonic series.
             These findings suggest that humans prefer tone combinations
             that reflect the spectral characteristics of conspecific
             vocalizations. The analysis also highlights the spectral
             similarity among the scales used by different
             cultures.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0008144},
   Key = {fds268465}
}

@article{fds268468,
   Author = {Wojtach, WT and Sung, K and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of the speed-distance
             effect.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {e6771},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19707552},
   Abstract = {Understanding motion perception continues to be the subject
             of much debate, a central challenge being to account for why
             the speeds and directions seen accord with neither the
             physical movements of objects nor their projected movements
             on the retina. Here we investigate the varied perceptions of
             speed that occur when stimuli moving across the retina
             traverse different projected distances (the speed-distance
             effect). By analyzing a database of moving objects projected
             onto an image plane we show that this phenomenology can be
             quantitatively accounted for by the frequency of occurrence
             of image speeds generated by perspective transformation.
             These results indicate that speed-distance effects are
             determined empirically from accumulated past experience with
             the relationship between image speeds and moving
             objects.},
   Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0006771},
   Key = {fds268468}
}

@article{fds268351,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Perception of Surfaces and Forms},
   Pages = {513-521},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-008045046-9.00229-1},
   Abstract = {The purpose of this article is to consider the strategy that
             vision uses to generate perceptions of surface qualities
             such as brightness and color, as well as perceptions of
             surface form. The basic challenge that vision must contend
             with in elaborating these subjective experiences is linking
             inherently ambiguous retinal stimuli to their real-world
             sources in a manner that leads to successful visually guided
             behavior. The evidence derived from what people actually see
             indicates that this problem is solved in a fundamentally
             empirical manner - that is, by the accumulation of past
             experience rather than by analytical operations on visual
             stimulus features. © 2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-008045046-9.00229-1},
   Key = {fds268351}
}

@article{fds268467,
   Author = {Sung, K and Wojtach, WT and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of aperture effects.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {106},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {298-303},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19114661},
   Abstract = {The perceived direction of a moving line changes, often
             markedly, when viewed through an aperture. Although several
             explanations of this remarkable effect have been proposed,
             these accounts typically focus on the percepts elicited by a
             particular type of aperture and offer no biological
             rationale. Here, we test the hypothesis that to contend with
             the inherently ambiguous nature of motion stimuli the
             perceived direction of objects moving behind apertures of
             different shapes is determined by a wholly empirical
             strategy of visual processing. An analysis of moving line
             stimuli generated by objects projected through apertures
             shows that the directions of motion subjects report in
             psychophysical testing is accounted for by the frequency of
             occurrence of the 2D directions of stimuli generated by
             simulated 3D sources. The completeness of these predictions
             supports the conclusion that the direction of perceived
             motion is fully determined by accumulated behavioral
             experience with sources whose physical motions cannot be
             conveyed by image sequences as such.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0811702106},
   Key = {fds268467}
}

@article{fds268464,
   Author = {Gill, KZ and Purves, D},
   Title = {A biological rationale for musical scales.},
   Journal = {Plos One},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {e8144},
   Year = {2009},
   ISSN = {1932-6203},
   Abstract = {Scales are collections of tones that divide octaves into
             specific intervals used to create music. Since humans can
             distinguish about 240 different pitches over an octave in
             the mid-range of hearing, in principle a very large number
             of tone combinations could have been used for this purpose.
             Nonetheless, compositions in Western classical, folk and
             popular music as well as in many other musical traditions
             are based on a relatively small number of scales that
             typically comprise only five to seven tones. Why humans
             employ only a few of the enormous number of possible tone
             combinations to create music is not known. Here we show that
             the component intervals of the most widely used scales
             throughout history and across cultures are those with the
             greatest overall spectral similarity to a harmonic series.
             These findings suggest that humans prefer tone combinations
             that reflect the spectral characteristics of conspecific
             vocalizations. The analysis also highlights the spectral
             similarity among the scales used by different
             cultures.},
   Key = {fds268464}
}

@article{fds268462,
   Author = {Wojtach, WT and Sung, K and Truong, S and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of the flash-lag
             effect.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {105},
   Number = {42},
   Pages = {16338-16343},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18852459},
   Abstract = {When a flash of light is presented in physical alignment
             with a moving object, the flash is perceived to lag behind
             the position of the object. This phenomenon, known as the
             flash-lag effect, has been of particular interest to vision
             scientists because of the challenge it presents to
             understanding how the visual system generates perceptions of
             objects in motion. Although various explanations have been
             offered, the significance of this effect remains a matter of
             debate. Here, we show that: (i) contrary to previous reports
             based on limited data, the flash-lag effect is an increasing
             nonlinear function of image speed; and (ii) this function is
             accurately predicted by the frequency of occurrence of image
             speeds generated by the perspective transformation of moving
             objects. These results support the conclusion that
             perceptions of the relative position of a moving object are
             determined by accumulated experience with image speeds, in
             this way allowing for visual behavior in response to
             real-world sources whose speeds and positions cannot be
             perceived directly.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0808916105},
   Key = {fds268462}
}

@article{fds268463,
   Author = {Ross, D and Choi, J and Purves, D},
   Title = {Musical intervals in speech.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {104},
   Number = {23},
   Pages = {9852-9857},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17525146},
   Abstract = {Throughout history and across cultures, humans have created
             music using pitch intervals that divide octaves into the 12
             tones of the chromatic scale. Why these specific intervals
             in music are preferred, however, is not known. In the
             present study, we analyzed a database of individually spoken
             English vowel phones to examine the hypothesis that musical
             intervals arise from the relationships of the formants in
             speech spectra that determine the perceptions of distinct
             vowels. Expressed as ratios, the frequency relationships of
             the first two formants in vowel phones represent all 12
             intervals of the chromatic scale. Were the formants to fall
             outside the ranges found in the human voice, their
             relationships would generate either a less complete or a
             more dilute representation of these specific intervals.
             These results imply that human preference for the intervals
             of the chromatic scale arises from experience with the way
             speech formants modulate laryngeal harmonics to create
             different phonemes.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0703140104},
   Key = {fds268463}
}

@article{fds268461,
   Author = {Boots, B and Nundy, S and Purves, D},
   Title = {Evolution of visually guided behavior in artificial
             agents.},
   Journal = {Network: Computation in Neural Systems},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {11-34},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0954-898X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17454680},
   Abstract = {Recent work on brightness, color, and form has suggested
             that human visual percepts represent the probable sources of
             retinal images rather than stimulus features as such. Here
             we investigate the plausibility of this empirical concept of
             vision by allowing autonomous agents to evolve in virtual
             environments based solely on the relative success of their
             behavior. The responses of evolved agents to visual stimuli
             indicate that fitness improves as the neural network control
             systems gradually incorporate the statistical relationship
             between projected images and behavior appropriate to the
             sources of the inherently ambiguous images. These results:
             (1) demonstrate the merits of a wholly empirical strategy of
             animal vision as a means of contending with the inverse
             optics problem; (2) argue that the information incorporated
             into biological visual processing circuitry is the
             relationship between images and their probable sources; and
             (3) suggest why human percepts do not map neatly onto
             physical reality.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09548980601113254},
   Key = {fds268461}
}

@article{fds268460,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Beau Lotto and R and Purves, D},
   Title = {Comparison of Bayesian and empirical ranking approaches to
             visual perception.},
   Journal = {Journal of Theoretical Biology},
   Volume = {241},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {866-875},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0022-5193},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16537082},
   Abstract = {Much current vision research is predicated on the idea--and
             a rapidly growing body of evidence--that visual percepts are
             generated according to the empirical significance of light
             stimuli rather than their physical characteristics. As a
             result, an increasing number of investigators have asked how
             visual perception can be rationalized in these terms. Here,
             we compare two different theoretical frameworks for
             predicting what observers actually see in response to visual
             stimuli: Bayesian decision theory and empirical ranking
             theory. Deciding which of these approaches has greater merit
             is likely to determine how the statistical operations that
             apparently underlie visual perception are eventually
             understood.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.01.017},
   Key = {fds268460}
}

@article{fds268457,
   Author = {Long, F and Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {Spectral statistics in natural scenes predict hue,
             saturation, and brightness.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {103},
   Number = {15},
   Pages = {6013-6018},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16595630},
   Abstract = {The perceptual color qualities of hue, saturation, and
             brightness do not correspond in any simple way to the
             physical characteristics of retinal stimuli, a fact that
             poses a major obstacle for any explanation of color vision.
             Here we test the hypothesis that these basic color
             attributes are determined by the statistical covariations in
             the spectral stimuli that humans have always experienced in
             typical visual environments. Using a database of 1,600
             natural images, we analyzed the joint probability
             distributions of the physical variables most relevant to
             each of these perceptual qualities. The cumulative density
             functions derived from these distributions predict the major
             colorimetric functions that have been reported in
             psychophysical experiments over the last
             century.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0600890103},
   Key = {fds268457}
}

@article{fds268459,
   Author = {Holcombe, AO and Clifford, CWG and Eagleman, DM and Pakarian,
             P},
   Title = {Illusory motion reversal in tune with motion
             detectors.},
   Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {559-560},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {1364-6613},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.009},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.009},
   Key = {fds268459}
}

@article{fds268458,
   Author = {Andrews, T and Purves, D},
   Title = {The wagon-wheel illusion in continuous light.},
   Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {261-263},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.004},
   Abstract = {The fact that a perceptual experience akin to the familiar
             wagon-wheel illusion in movies and on TV can occur in the
             absence of stroboscopic presentation is intriguing because
             of its relevance to visuo-temporal parsing. The wagon-wheel
             effect in continuous light has also been the source of
             considerable misunderstanding and dispute, as is apparent in
             a series of recent papers. Here we review this potentially
             confusing evidence and suggest how it should be
             interpreted.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.004},
   Key = {fds268458}
}

@article{fds268455,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {The Poggendorff illusion explained by natural scene
             geometry.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {102},
   Number = {21},
   Pages = {7707-7712},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15888555},
   Abstract = {One of the most intriguing of the many discrepancies between
             perceived spatial relationships and the physical structure
             of visual stimuli is the Poggendorff illusion, when an
             obliquely oriented line that is interrupted no longer
             appears collinear. Although many different theories have
             been proposed to explain this effect, there has been no
             consensus about its cause. Here, we use a database of range
             images (i.e., images that include the distance from the
             image plane of every pixel in the scene) to show that the
             probability distribution of the possible locations of line
             segments across an interval in natural environments can
             fully account for all of the behavior of this otherwise
             puzzling phenomenon.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0502893102},
   Key = {fds268455}
}

@article{fds268346,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Perceiving geometry: Geometrical illusions explained by
             natural scene statistics},
   Journal = {Perceiving Geometry: Geometrical Illusions Explained by
             Natural Scene Statistics},
   Pages = {1-126},
   Publisher = {Springer Verlag},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/b135453},
   Abstract = {Understanding vision, whether from a neurobiological,
             psychological or philosophical perspective, represents a
             daunting challenge that has been pursued for millennia.
             During at least the last few centuries, natural
             philosophers, and more recently vision scientists, have
             recognized that a fundamental problem in biological vision
             is that the physical sources underlying sensory stimuli are
             unknowable in any direct sense. In vision, because physical
             qualities are conflated when the 3-D world is projected onto
             the 2-D image plane of the retina, the provenance of light
             reaching the eye at any moment is inevitably uncertain. This
             quandary is referred to as the inverse optics problem. The
             relationship of the real world and the information conveyed
             to the brain by light present a profound problem. Successful
             behavior in a complex and potentially hostile environment
             clearly depends on responding appropriately to the sources
             of visual stimuli rather than to the physical
             characteristics of the stimuli as such. If the retinal
             images generated by light cannot specify the underlying
             reality an observer must deal with, how then does the visual
             system produce behavior that is generally successful?
             Perceiving Geometry considers the evidence that, with
             respect to the perception of geometry, the human visual
             system solves this problem by incorporating past human
             experience of what retinal images have typically
             corresponded to in the real world. This empirical strategy,
             which is documented by extensive analyses of scene geometry,
             explains many otherwise puzzling aspects of what we see
             (i.e., the so-called "geometrical illusions"), providing the
             best indication to date as to how perceptions of the
             geometrical aspects of the world are actually generated by
             the brain.},
   Doi = {10.1007/b135453},
   Key = {fds268346}
}

@article{fds268453,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of
             image-source relationships.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {102},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1234-1239},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15657142},
   Abstract = {The Müller-Lyer effect, the apparent difference in the
             length of a line as the result of its adornment with
             arrowheads or arrow tails, is the best known and most
             controversial of the classical geometrical illusions. By
             sampling a range-image database of natural scenes, we show
             that the perceptual effects elicited by the Müller-Lyer
             stimulus and its major variants are correctly predicted by
             the probability distributions of the possible physical
             sources underlying the relevant retinal images. These
             results support the conclusion that the Müller-Lyer
             illusion is a manifestation of the probabilistic strategy of
             visual processing that has evolved to contend with the
             uncertain provenance of retinal stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0409314102},
   Key = {fds268453}
}

@article{fds268456,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Natural-scene geometry predicts the perception of angles and
             line orientation.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {102},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1228-1233},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15657143},
   Abstract = {Visual stimuli that entail the intersection of two or more
             straight lines elicit a variety of well known perceptual
             anomalies. Preeminent among these anomalies are the
             systematic overestimation of acute angles, the
             underestimation of obtuse angles, and the misperceptions of
             line orientation exemplified in the classical tilt, Zollner,
             and Hering illusions. Here we show that the probability
             distributions of the possible real-world sources of
             projected lines and angles derived from a range-image
             database of natural scenes accurately predict each of these
             perceptual peculiarities. These findings imply that the
             perception of angles and oriented lines is determined by the
             statistical relationship between geometrical stimuli and
             their physical sources in typical visual
             environments.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0409311102},
   Key = {fds268456}
}

@article{fds114100,
   Title = {Lotto RB, Purves D (2005) Understanding the basis of color
             perception. International Review of Neurobiology (In
             press).},
   Year = {2005},
   Key = {fds114100}
}

@article{fds268452,
   Author = {Schwartz, DA and Purves, D},
   Title = {Pitch is determined by naturally occurring periodic
             sounds.},
   Journal = {Hearing Research},
   Volume = {194},
   Number = {1-2},
   Pages = {31-46},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0378-5955},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15276674},
   Abstract = {The phenomenology of pitch has been difficult to rationalize
             and remains the subject of much debate. Here we test the
             hypothesis that audition generates pitch percepts by
             relating inherently ambiguous sound stimuli to their
             probable sources in the human auditory environment. A
             database of speech sounds, the principal source of periodic
             sound energy for human listeners, was compiled and the
             dominant periodicity of each speech sound determined. A set
             of synthetic test stimuli were used to assess whether the
             major pitch phenomena described in the literature could be
             explained by the probabilistic relationship between the
             stimuli and their probable sources (i.e., speech sounds).
             The phenomena tested included the perception of the missing
             fundamental, the pitch-shift of the residue, spectral
             dominance and the perception of pitch strength. In each
             case, the conditional probability distribution of speech
             sound periodicities accurately predicted the pitches
             normally heard in response to the test stimuli. We conclude
             from these findings that pitch entails an auditory process
             that relates inevitably ambiguous sound stimuli to their
             probable natural sources.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.heares.2004.01.019},
   Key = {fds268452}
}

@article{fds268454,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {The statistical structure of natural light patterns
             determines perceived light intensity.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {101},
   Number = {23},
   Pages = {8745-8750},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15152077},
   Abstract = {The same target luminance in different contexts can elicit
             markedly different perceptions of brightness, a fact that
             has long puzzled vision scientists. Here we test the
             proposal that the visual system encodes not luminance as
             such but rather the statistical relationship of a particular
             luminance to all possible luminance values experienced in
             natural contexts during evolution. This statistical
             conception of vision was validated by using a database of
             natural scenes in which we could determine the probability
             distribution functions of co-occurring target and contextual
             luminance values. The distribution functions obtained in
             this way predict target brightness in response to a variety
             of challenging stimuli, thus explaining these otherwise
             puzzling percepts. That brightness is determined by the
             statistics of natural light patterns implies that the
             relevant neural circuitry is specifically organized to
             generate these probabilistic responses.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0402192101},
   Key = {fds268454}
}

@article{fds268447,
   Author = {Purves, D and Williams, SM and Nundy, S and Lotto,
             RB},
   Title = {Perceiving the intensity of light.},
   Journal = {Psychological Review},
   Volume = {111},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {142-158},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0033-295X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14756591},
   Abstract = {The relationship between luminance (i.e., the photometric
             intensity of light) and its perception (i.e., sensations of
             lightness or brightness) has long been a puzzle. In addition
             to the mystery of why these perceptual qualities do not
             scale with luminance in any simple way, "illusions" such as
             simultaneous brightness contrast, Mach bands,
             Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet edge effects, and the
             Chubb-Sperling-Solomon illusion have all generated much
             interest but no generally accepted explanation. The authors
             review evidence that the full range of this perceptual
             phenomenology can be rationalized in terms of an empirical
             theory of vision. The implication of these observations is
             that perceptions of lightness and brightness are generated
             according to the probability distributions of the possible
             sources of luminance values in stimuli that are inevitably
             ambiguous.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0033-295x.111.1.142},
   Key = {fds268447}
}

@article{fds268450,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Size contrast and assimilation explained by the statistics
             of natural scene geometry.},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {90-102},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15006039},
   Abstract = {The term "size contrast and assimilation" refers to a large
             class of geometrical illusions in which the apparent sizes
             of identical visual targets in various contexts are
             different. Here we have examined whether these intriguing
             discrepancies between physical and perceived size can be
             explained by a visual process in which percepts are
             determined by the probability distribution of the possible
             real-world sources of retinal stimuli. To test this idea, we
             acquired a range image database of natural scenes that
             specified the location of every image point in 3-D space. By
             sampling the possible physical sources of various size
             contrast or assimilation stimuli in the database, we
             determined the probability distributions of the size of the
             target in the images generated by these sources. For each of
             the various stimuli tested, these probability distributions
             of target size in different contexts accurately predicted
             the perceptual effects reported in psychophysical studies.
             We conclude that size contrast and assimilation effects are
             a further manifestation of a fundamentally probabilistic
             process of visual perception.},
   Doi = {10.1162/089892904322755584},
   Key = {fds268450}
}

@article{fds268451,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {Perceiving colour},
   Journal = {Review of Progress in Coloration and Related
             Topics},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {12-25},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0557-9325},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1478-4408.2004.tb00149.x},
   Abstract = {Understanding the percepts elicited by spectral
             distributions in visual stimuli (i.e. understanding the
             perception of colour) is made especially challenging by the
             peculiar phenomenology of colour contrast and constancy
             effects. Interestingly as the first systematic account of
             colour contrast was published in 1839 by the French chemist
             Michel Chevreul based on work done while serving as the
             director of dyes for the Royal Manufacturers. In this
             current paper we review the nature of colour vision, the
             problems that the observations of Chevreul and others
             present for colour science, and recent work that suggests a
             solution. © Rev. Prog. Color.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1478-4408.2004.tb00149.x},
   Key = {fds268451}
}

@article{fds114060,
   Title = {Purves, D.,  S.M. Williams, S. Nundy and B.B. Lotto (2003)
             Perceiving the Intensity of Light.  Psychological Rev. Vol
             111: 142-158.},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds114060}
}

@article{fds114066,
   Title = {Howe Q, Purves D (2004) Size contrast and assimilation
             explained by the statistics of scene geometry. J Cog
             Neurosci 16(1): 90-102.},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds114066}
}

@article{fds114099,
   Title = {Purves D, Lotto RB (2004) The Cornsweet effect. Encyclopedia
             of Neuroscience, 3rd edition. G. Adelman and B.H. Smith,
             eds. Elsevier Press 2004.},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds114099}
}

@article{fds114101,
   Title = {Lotto RB, Purves D (2004) Perceiving color. Color Dyers Rev
             (in press).},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds114101}
}

@article{fds114102,
   Title = {Yang Z, Purves D (2004) The statistical structure of natural
             light patterns determines perceived light intensity. Proc
             Natl Acad Sci 101: 8745-8750},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds114102}
}

@article{fds114103,
   Title = {Schwartz D, Purves D (2004) Pitch is determined by naturally
             occurring periodic sounds. Hearing Research 194:
             31-46.},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds114103}
}

@article{fds268442,
   Author = {Long, F and Purves, D},
   Title = {Evidence that color contrast effects have a probabilistic
             foundation},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {314-314},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/3.9.314},
   Abstract = {Surfaces returning identical light spectra to the eye can
             elicit different color percepts when embedded in spectrally
             different surrounds. Although various theories have been put
             forward to rationalize these color contrast effects, there
             is no consensus about their basis. Here we tested the
             hypothesis that color contrast is generated by the
             probability distribution of the possible physical sources of
             spectral stimuli (see Lotto and Purves, PNAS 97:12834,
             2001). The analysis used a database of 41 natural scenes in
             which the radiance spectrum of each point (i.e., the
             projected light spectrum for each pixel in the images) was
             known. The relevant reflectance spectra for corresponding
             points were computed by removing the influence of both the
             illuminant and scene geometry from the radiance spectrum.
             The illumination spectra for each pixel (which include the
             influence of both the illuminant and scene geometry) were
             then determined by dividing the radiance spectrum by
             reflectance spectrum. To facilitate the statistical
             analysis, the radiance spectrum on each pixel was converted
             into RGB tristimulus values. Each image in the database was
             sampled repeatedly with a center/surround template, and the
             probability distributions of the possible combinations of
             reflectance and illumination spectra that could have
             generated the relevant RGB values were determined. The
             probability distributions of the reflectance and
             illumination spectra of the central target varied as a
             function of the RGB values of the surround, indicating that
             the typical physical sources of target spectra differ when
             they are embedded in spectrally different surrounds. The
             color percepts predicted by these distributions were in good
             agreement with the percepts elicited by color contrast
             stimuli. This evidence supports the conclusion that color
             contrast effects are determined by the probabilistic
             relationship between ambiguous spectral stimuli and the
             distribution of their possible sources.},
   Doi = {10.1167/3.9.314},
   Key = {fds268442}
}

@article{fds268443,
   Author = {Purves, D and Howe, CQ and Schwartz, DA},
   Title = {Vision and the perception of music have a common
             denominator},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {518-518},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/3.9.518},
   Abstract = {All human listeners perceive tones in the presence of
             regularly repeating patterns of sound pressure fluctuation
             over a wide range of frequencies. In music, the salient and
             widely-shared features of this aspect of auditory perception
             are: 1) an iterated partitioning of the continuous dimension
             of pitch into octave intervals bounded by tones that are
             musically similar; 2) the division of each octave into the
             12 intervals of the chromatic scale; 3) the preference in
             musical composition and performance for particular subsets
             of these 12 intervals (e.g., the intervals of the pentatonic
             or diatonic scales); and 4) the similar consonance ordering
             of chromatic scale tone combinations produced by listeners
             of all ages, places, and periods. Despite intense interest
             in these perceptual phenomena over several millennia, they
             have no generally accepted explanation in physical,
             psychological or biological terms. A rapidly growing body of
             work in vision has shown that the fundamental qualities that
             characterize visual percepts (lightness/brightness, color,
             geometry and motion) accord with the probability
             distributions of the possible sources of visual stimuli.
             Since the uncertain provenance of sensory stimuli is
             general, this empirical solution to the inverse optics
             problem might be expected to extend to other sensory
             modalities. We therefore examined the hypothesis that
             musical percepts also arise from the statistical
             relationship between sound stimuli and their natural
             sources. An analysis of recorded speech shows that the
             probability distribution of amplitude/frequency combinations
             in human utterances, the principal source of periodic
             stimuli in the human acoustical environment, predicts
             octaves, scales and consonance. These observations suggest
             that the auditory system, like the visual system, generates
             percepts determined by the probability distributions that
             link inherently ambiguous stimuli and their
             sources.},
   Doi = {10.1167/3.9.518},
   Key = {fds268443}
}

@article{fds268444,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Size contrast explained by the statistics of scene
             geometry},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {522-522},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/3.9.522},
   Abstract = {Standard presentations of size contrast stimuli include the
             well-known Ebbinghaus circles and the Delboeuf figures. When
             presented with such stimuli, observers perceive a target
             form surrounded by larger but otherwise similar forms to be
             smaller than the same target surrounded by smaller forms.
             Here we have examined the hypothesis that the anomalous
             perception of these stimuli is a consequence of a wholly
             probabilistic strategy of vision in which percepts accord
             with the probability distribution of the possible sources of
             the stimuli. To test this idea, we used a range image
             database acquired by laser scanning natural scenes to
             determine the probability distribution of the size of the
             real-world sources of the central targets in the size
             contrast stimuli. In good quantitative agreement with a
             large body of psychophysical evidence, the average physical
             size of the sources of a given form embedded in a context of
             larger surrounding forms in the image plane is smaller than
             the sources of the same target surrounded by smaller forms.
             Thus, the reason why the two identical central targets look
             different in size is because their possible physical sources
             are, in fact, different in size. These findings support the
             hypothesis that the size contrast effect is a signature of a
             fundamentally probabilistic process of vision
             perception.},
   Doi = {10.1167/3.9.522},
   Key = {fds268444}
}

@article{fds268445,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {Statistical concatenations of luminance can explain
             lightness/brightness percepts},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {423-423},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/3.9.423},
   Abstract = {A number of recent studies have indicated that perceptions
             of lightness/brightness are determined by the probabilistic
             relationship between the luminances in the retinal stimulus
             and the possible physical sources (reviewed in Purves and
             Lotto, "Why we see what we do" Sinauer, 2002). To date,
             these analyses have relied primarily on qualitative
             paradigms, or limited quantitative arguments to rationalize
             the lightness/brightness percepts elicited by Cornsweet
             edges, stimuli that elicit Mach bands, or the reduced cue
             conditions used in brightness scaling experiments.
             Considering the linkage between stimulus luminances and
             lightness/brightness percepts more broadly, however, the
             visual system must instantiate a more fundamental set of
             underlying statistical relationships to generate
             lightness/brightness percepts in any and all circumstances.
             Given the high dimensionality of real-world effects on the
             probability distribution of lightness/brightness sources, it
             seems inevitable that the relevant statistical
             instantiations entail the conditional probabilities of
             concatenations of luminance values in retinal images with
             respect to the underlying natural sources. Accordingly, we
             have explored whether anomalies of lightness/brightness can
             be explained in these terms by analyzing the distribution of
             luminance in 4200 images of natural visual environments in
             the so-called Netherlands database (hlab.phys.rug.nl). A
             large number of samples were generated using various
             templates in which the pattern of light was similar to the
             basic unit in several well-known lightness/brightness
             stimuli that generate unusual percepts (e.g., the Hermann
             grid, the Wertheimer-Benary pattern, White's stimulus and
             the criss-cross pattern), thus allowing us to compute the
             probability distribution of light in the 'target' area of
             these stimuli. The relative shifts in the probability
             distributions of luminance in these stimuli appear to
             account for the associated perceptions of
             lightness/brightness.},
   Doi = {10.1167/3.9.423},
   Key = {fds268445}
}

@article{fds268449,
   Author = {Long, F and Purves, D},
   Title = {Natural scene statistics as the universal basis of color
             context effects.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {100},
   Number = {25},
   Pages = {15190-15193},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14623975},
   Abstract = {The color context effects referred to as color contrast,
             constancy, and assimilation underscore the fact that color
             percepts do not correspond to the spectral characteristics
             of the generative stimuli. Despite a variety of proposed
             theories, these phenomena have resisted explanation in a
             single principled framework. Using a hyperspectral image
             database of natural scenes, we here show that color
             contrast, constancy, and assimilation are all predicted by
             the statistical organization of spectral returns from
             natural visual environments.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.2036361100},
   Key = {fds268449}
}

@article{fds268446,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {Image/source statistics of surfaces in natural
             scenes.},
   Journal = {Network: Computation in Neural Systems},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {371-390},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0954-898X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12938763},
   Abstract = {Perceiving surfaces in a manner that accords with their
             physical properties is essential for successful behaviour.
             Since, however, a given retinal image can have been
             generated by an infinite variety of natural surfaces with
             different geometrical and/or physical qualities, the
             corresponding percepts cannot be determined by the stimulus
             per se. Rather, resolution of this quandary requires a
             strategy of vision that incorporates the statistical
             relationship of the information in retinal images to its
             sources in representative environments. To examine this
             probabilistic relationship with respect to the features of
             object surfaces, we analysed a database of range images in
             which the distances of all the objects in a series of
             natural scenes were measured with respect to the image plane
             by a laser range scanner. By taking any particular scene
             obtained in this way to be made up of a set of concatenated
             surface patches, we were able to explore the statistics of
             scene roughness, size-distance relationships, surface
             orientation and local curvature, as well as the independent
             components of natural surfaces. The relevance of these
             statistics to both perception and the neuronal organization
             of the underlying visual circuitry is discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1088/0954-898x_14_3_301},
   Key = {fds268446}
}

@article{fds268448,
   Author = {Schwartz, DA and Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {The statistical structure of human speech sounds predicts
             musical universals.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {18},
   Pages = {7160-7168},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12904476},
   Abstract = {The similarity of musical scales and consonance judgments
             across human populations has no generally accepted
             explanation. Here we present evidence that these aspects of
             auditory perception arise from the statistical structure of
             naturally occurring periodic sound stimuli. An analysis of
             speech sounds, the principal source of periodic sound
             stimuli in the human acoustical environment, shows that the
             probability distribution of amplitude-frequency combinations
             in human utterances predicts both the structure of the
             chromatic scale and consonance ordering. These observations
             suggest that what we hear is determined by the statistical
             relationship between acoustical stimuli and their naturally
             occurring sources, rather than by the physical parameters of
             the stimulus per se.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.23-18-07160.2003},
   Key = {fds268448}
}

@article{fds268501,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {A statistical explanation of visual space.},
   Journal = {Nature Neuroscience},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {632-640},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {1097-6256},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12754512},
   Keywords = {Humans • Models, Neurological • Models,
             Statistical* • Photic Stimulation • Psychomotor
             Performance • Space Perception • Spatial Behavior
             • Visual Fields • physiology*},
   Abstract = {The subjective visual space perceived by humans does not
             reflect a simple transformation of objective physical space;
             rather, perceived space has an idiosyncratic relationship
             with the real world. To date, there is no consensus about
             either the genesis of perceived visual space or the
             implications of its peculiar characteristics for visually
             guided behavior. Here we used laser range scanning to
             measure the actual distances from the image plane of all
             unoccluded points in a series of natural scenes. We then
             asked whether the differences between real and apparent
             distances could be explained by the statistical relationship
             of scene geometry and the observer. We were able to predict
             perceived distances in a variety of circumstances from the
             probability distribution of physical distances. This finding
             lends support to the idea that the characteristics of human
             visual space are determined probabilistically.},
   Language = {eng},
   Doi = {10.1038/nn1059},
   Key = {fds268501}
}

@article{fds114062,
   Title = {Yang, Z. and D. Purves (2003) Image/source statistics in
             natural scenes. Network: Computation in Neural Systems 14
             (3): 371-390.},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds114062}
}

@article{fds114063,
   Title = {Schwartz, D., C.Q. Howe and D. Purves (2003) Statistical
             evidence that musical universals derive from the acoustical
             characteristics of human speech. J. Neurosci. 23:
             7160-7168.},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds114063}
}

@article{fds114092,
   Title = {Long, F. and D. Purves (2003) Natural scene statistics as
             the universal basis for color context effects. Proc Natl
             Acad Sci 100 (25): 15190-15193.},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds114092}
}

@article{fds114095,
   Title = {Purves,  D. and R.B. Lotto (2003) Why We See What We Do:
              An Empirical Theory  of Vision. Sinauer Associates:
             Sunderland, MA.},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds114095}
}

@article{fds268438,
   Author = {Purves, D and Yang, Z},
   Title = {The Poggendorff illusion explained by the statistics of
             natural scene geometry},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {201-201},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.7.201},
   Abstract = {One of the most intriguing discrepancies between the
             perception of a visual stimulus and its real-world source is
             the Poggendorff illusion. When an obliquely oriented line is
             occluded by a bar, the continuation of the line across the
             occluder appears to be shifted vertically, despite of the
             collinearity of the separated line segments. A great deal of
             literature on this subject notwithstanding, none of the
             explanations so far provided (the angle theory and the depth
             theory are the two major categories) has satisfactorily
             accounted for all aspects of this effect. Here we have
             tested a wholly empirical explanation of this illusion. To
             this end, we acquired a database of natural scenes
             (including indoor, outdoor and natural scenes) in which the
             distances of all the objects from the image plane were
             determined with a laser range scanner. We found the
             probability distribution of the possible positions of the
             line segments in the database lying on the 'far side' of an
             imagined occluder to be shifted vertically compared to the
             positions obtained by direct extension of the same line
             segments. This shift was apparent in indoor, outdoor and
             fully natural scenes, albeit with different magnitudes and
             variances. Moreover, the magnitude of the shift 1) increased
             with the width of the occluding bar; 2) increased with a
             decrease in the (acute) angle of the intersection of the
             line with the occluder; 3) diminished for the acute angle
             components of the stimulus, but was maintained for the
             obtuse angle components; and 4) diminished when the stimulus
             configuration was rotated. Each of these behaviors has been
             described in the perceptual responses to corresponding
             variations in the presentation of the Poggendorff stimulus.
             We conclude that this otherwise peculiar set of perceptual
             discrepancies is generated by the probabilistic relationship
             between the relevant features in the image plane and the
             probability distribution of the possible underlying sources
             of the stimulus in the real world.},
   Doi = {10.1167/2.7.201},
   Key = {fds268438}
}

@article{fds268439,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {A probabilistic explanation of perceived line length and
             orientation},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {706-706},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.7.706},
   Abstract = {Human perception of the length and the orientation of a
             straight line is systematically biased as a function of the
             2D orientation of the line in the retinal image. Motivated
             by recent evidence that the relationship between the retinal
             image and perception is a wholly probabilistic one, we have
             explored the idea that perceived length and orientation of a
             linear stimulus are determined by the probabilistic
             relationship between the linear projection in the image
             plane and its possible physical sources. To test this
             hypothesis, we collected a database of natural scenes that
             included the range and luminance of every pixel in the
             images. The database thus relates projections in the image
             plane to the arrangement of objects in the physical world.
             Accordingly, we could determine the 3-D orientations of the
             physical sources of all straight-line projections on the
             retina (the image plane), as well as the ratio of the
             physical length of the sources to the length of their
             projections. We found that the probability distributions of
             the tilt, slant and the physical-to-image length ratio of
             straight lines determined in this way change systematically
             as a function of the orientation of the projected line.
             These variations in the probability distributions predict
             the perception of line length and line orientation as a
             function of line orientation. Because the probability
             distributions of the possible sources of oblique projections
             show greater variance than those of the linear projections
             in the cardinal axes, these statistical relationships can
             also rationalize the oblique effect (i.e., the poorer and
             more variable performance of human observers confronted with
             oblique lines compared to performance with lines in the
             cardinal axes).},
   Doi = {10.1167/2.7.706},
   Key = {fds268439}
}

@article{fds268440,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {The probabilistic foundation of visual space},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {715-715},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.7.715},
   Abstract = {An assumption in many studies is that visual space (i.e.,
             the space we perceive) is metrical. For example, perceived
             space has often been considered a Riemann space of constant
             curvature. In such cases, perceived spatial relationships
             should be independent of the context of the visual scene.
             This category of assumptions, however, is inconsistent with
             numerous experimental observations showing that the
             relationship between the perceived and the physical
             parameters of scene geometry is systematically distorted. In
             the absence of a principled account of what this distortion
             of physical space actually means, other investigators have
             assumed that visual space is either affine or subject to
             some other transformation of physical space. Here we have
             explored an alternative hypothesis, namely that visual space
             is generated solely by the statistical properties of the
             physical world. To this end, we acquired and analyzed a
             database of natural scenes in which the distances of all
             object points from the image plane were measured with a
             laser range scanner. The probability distributions of these
             distances are scale invariant, a feature that accords with
             the human perception of distance and location under
             impoverished stimulus conditions. Furthermore, the
             probability distributions of the physical sources of visual
             stimuli (i.e., their distance, depth, size, and surface
             orientation) were found to be systematically influenced by
             the range distribution of the surround. These
             context-dependent probability distributions of physical
             sources generally account for the known distortion in the
             perception of distance, depth, size, and orientation (e.g.,
             the "terrain influence" on distance judgment, and the
             well-known contextual effects that influence the perception
             of orientation). Our results thus suggest that visual
             perceptual space, for reasons of biological advantage, is
             straightforwardly determined by the probability
             distributions of the sources underlying visual
             stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.1167/2.7.715},
   Key = {fds268440}
}

@article{fds268441,
   Author = {Long, F and Purves, D},
   Title = {A probabilistic explanation of simultaneous brightness
             contrast},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {366-366},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.7.366},
   Abstract = {A growing body of evidence suggests that visual perception
             is generated according to the probabilistic relationship
             between the components of retinal images and their possible
             physical sources. We have further explored this idea by
             asking whether simultaneous brightness contrast effects can
             be explained by the statistical relationship between the
             physical sources of the light reaching the retina and the
             corresponding luminance values in the retinal image. To this
             end we first created a database of spectral returns
             (radiances) based on the interaction of average daylight
             (CIE D65) at 500 different levels of light intensity with
             200 achromatic reflectances (interpolated from 6 standard
             achromatic reflectances of Macbeth ColorChecker). A database
             of luminances was then created by converting each of the
             100,000 spectral returns obtained in this way into an RGB
             value using the standard CIE conversion. We then used the
             luminance database to create standard and 'articulated'
             brightness contrast stimuli. An analysis of the probability
             distributions of the possible illumination and reflectance
             values that could have generated the stimuli showed that: 1)
             the illumination of a gray patch with a dark surround is
             likely to be less intense than the illumination of the same
             patch in a lighter surround; 2) the illumination difference
             of gray patches in uniform surrounds is likely to be less
             than the illumination difference of the same patches in
             articulated surrounds. Thus on solely empirical grounds,
             these probability distributions predict that a gray patch
             with a darker surround will look brighter than the same
             patch on a lighter surround, and that the articulated
             version will generate a stronger perceptual effect. The
             significance of this work is to demonstrate that the effects
             of standard brightness contrast stimuli can be rationalized
             on the basis of the probability distributions of the sources
             derived from a contrived but nevertheless plausible database
             of visual 'scenes'.},
   Doi = {10.1167/2.7.366},
   Key = {fds268441}
}

@article{fds268498,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {The empirical basis of color perception.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and Cognition},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {609-629},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {December},
   ISSN = {1053-8100},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12470626},
   Keywords = {Color Perception • Humans • Light • Photic
             Stimulation • Probability* • physiology*},
   Abstract = {Rationalizing the perceptual effects of spectral stimuli has
             been a major challenge in vision science for at least the
             last 200 years. Here we review evidence that this otherwise
             puzzling body of phenomenology is generated by an empirical
             strategy of perception in which the color an observer sees
             is entirely determined by the probability distribution of
             the possible sources of the stimulus. The rationale for this
             strategy in color vision, as in other visual perceptual
             domains, is the inherent ambiguity of the real-world origins
             of any spectral stimulus.},
   Doi = {10.1016/s1053-8100(02)00014-4},
   Key = {fds268498}
}

@article{fds268500,
   Author = {Howe, CQ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Range image statistics can explain the anomalous perception
             of length.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {99},
   Number = {20},
   Pages = {13184-13188},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12237401},
   Keywords = {Humans • Lasers • Retina • Space Perception
             • Vision • Visual Perception* • physiology
             • physiology*},
   Abstract = {A long-standing puzzle in visual perception is that the
             apparent extent of a spatial interval (e.g., the distance
             between two points or the length of a line) does not simply
             accord with the length of the stimulus but varies as a
             function of orientation in the retinal image. Here, we show
             that this anomaly can be explained by the statistical
             relationship between the length of retinal projections and
             the length of their real-world sources. Using a laser range
             scanner, we acquired a database of natural images that
             included the three-dimensional location of every point in
             the scenes. An analysis of these range images showed that
             the average length of a physical interval in
             three-dimensional space changes systematically as a function
             of the orientation of the corresponding interval in the
             projected image, the variation being in good agreement with
             perceived length. This evidence implies that the perception
             of visual space is determined by the probability
             distribution of the possible real-world sources of retinal
             images.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.162474299},
   Key = {fds268500}
}

@article{fds268502,
   Author = {Nundy, S and Purves, D},
   Title = {A probabilistic explanation of brightness
             scaling.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {99},
   Number = {22},
   Pages = {14482-14487},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12388786},
   Keywords = {Computer Simulation* • Humans • Light •
             Models, Neurological* • Models, Statistical* •
             Photic Stimulation • Visual Perception •
             physiology*},
   Abstract = {The perceptions of lightness or brightness elicited by a
             visual target are linked to its luminance by a nonlinear
             function that varies according to the physical
             characteristics of the target and the background on which it
             is presented. Although no generally accepted explanation of
             this scaling relationship exists, it has long been
             considered a byproduct of low- or mid-level visual
             processing. Here we examine the possibility that brightness
             scaling is actually the signature of a biological strategy
             for dealing with inevitably ambiguous visual stimuli, in
             which percepts of lightness/brightness are determined by the
             probabilistic relationship between luminances in the image
             plane and their possible real-world sources.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.172520399},
   Key = {fds268502}
}

@article{fds268499,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {A rationale for the structure of color space.},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {84-88},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11814560},
   Keywords = {Animals • Color • Color Perception • Humans
             • Light • Photic Stimulation •
             physiology*},
   Abstract = {The colors perceived by humans in response to light stimuli
             are generally described in terms of four color categories
             (reds, greens, blues and yellows), the members of which are
             systematically arrayed around gray. This broadly accepted
             description of color sensation differs fundamentally from
             the light that induces it, which is neither 'circular' nor
             categorical. What, then, accounts for these discrepancies
             between the structure of color experience and the physical
             reality that underlies it? We suggest that these differences
             are based on two related requirements for successful color
             vision: (1) that spectra be ordered according to their
             physical similarities and differences; and (2) that this
             ordering be constrained by the four-color map
             problem.},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02059-3},
   Key = {fds268499}
}

@article{fds268497,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Shimpi, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {Perception of objects that are translating and
             rotating.},
   Journal = {Perception},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {925-942},
   Year = {2002},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0301-0066},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12269587},
   Keywords = {Discrimination Learning • Form Perception • Humans
             • Motion • Motion Perception • Psychophysics
             • Rotation • physiology*},
   Abstract = {The motion of objects that are both translating and rotating
             can be decomposed into an infinite number of translational
             and rotational combinations. How, then, do such stimuli
             routinely elicit specific percepts and behavioral responses
             that are usually appropriate? A possible answer is that
             motion percepts are fully determined by the probability
             distributions of all the possible correspondences and
             differences in the stimulus sequence. To test the merits of
             this conceptual framework, we investigated the perceived
             motion elicited by a line that is both translating and
             rotating behind an aperture. When stimuli are presented such
             that a particular sequence of appearance and disappearance
             occurs at the aperture boundary, subjects report that the
             line is rotating only; furthermore, the perceived centers of
             rotation appear to describe a cycloidal trajectory, even
             when one aperture shape is replaced by another. These and
             other perceptual effects elicited by translating and
             rotating stimuli are all accurately predicted by the
             probability distribution of the possible sources of the
             physical movements, supporting the conclusion that motion
             perception is indeed generated by a wholly probabilistic
             strategy.},
   Doi = {10.1068/p3379},
   Key = {fds268497}
}

@article{fds114059,
   Title = {Lotto, R.B and D. Purves (2002) An empirical explanation of
             the Chubb Illusion. J. Cog. Neurosci. 13:
             1-9.},
   Year = {2002},
   Key = {fds114059}
}

@article{fds114094,
   Title = {Purves D., R. B. Lotto and S. Nundy (2002) Why we see what
             we do.  American Scientist 90(3): 236-243.},
   Year = {2002},
   Key = {fds114094}
}

@article{fds268431,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lotto, B},
   Title = {Explanation of some major features of color
             perception},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {60-60},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/1.3.60},
   Abstract = {It has long been known that a particular red, green, blue
             and yellow is seen as being entirely free of any other
             color, and that the four color categories defined in this
             way are arranged in a circular manner. Most textbook
             accounts suppose that these features of color experience are
             an incidental consequence of color opponency. An alternative
             possibility is that these aspects of color experience
             represent the solution of a fundamental problem in topology,
             namely insuring that no two areas separated by a common
             boundary in a 2-dimensional array will appear the same if
             they are actually different (the four color map problem).
             However, unlike the cartographer, whose task is simply to
             distinguish the surfaces in a map as being the 'same' or
             'different', the visual system must distinguish surfaces and
             at the same time maintain the full range of spectral
             relationships. Simply differentiating surfaces in color
             experience would provide little behavioral advantage if the
             relative similarities and differences among different
             spectra were not also preserved in perception. If this
             argument is correct, then structure of subjective color
             space (i.e., the circular organization of the four color
             categories and their unique members), should reflect an
             analogous ordering of spectra. Here we use multidimensional
             scaling of a spectral data set to show that arranging
             spectra according to their relative similarities and
             differences defines a space that is similar to subjective
             color space. These results are consistent with the
             conclusion that the major features of subjective color
             experience represent a simultaneous solution of the four
             color map problem while maintaining the relative
             similarities and differences among the full range of light
             spectra.},
   Doi = {10.1167/1.3.60},
   Key = {fds268431}
}

@article{fds268432,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of the Chubb illusion},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {48-48},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/1.3.48},
   Abstract = {The brightness of any luminant stimulus varies, often quite
             markedly, as a function of the context in which it is
             presented. An especially intriguing example of this
             phenomenon is the illusion described by Chubb and colleagues
             (1989) in which the apparent contrast of a patterned target
             is reduced when it is embedded in a pattern of the same
             spatial frequency but of higher luminance contrast. Illusory
             percepts of brightness, like this one, are usually
             considered epiphenomena of inhibitory interactions between
             neurons tuned to the same attributes of the stimulus, in
             this case between neurons in the primary visual cortex
             similarly tuned to spatial contrast frequency. Here we
             tested a different possibility, namely that the Chubb
             illusion is generated according to the experience of the
             visual system with background textures seen through an
             imperfectly transmitting medium. In agreement with this
             suggestion, making the stimulus more consistent with a
             contribution to the target of imperfect transmittance
             increased the effect for naïve subjects, whereas making the
             stimulus less consistent with this possibility decreased the
             effect. Because the luminance contrasts and spatial
             frequencies of the stimuli were unchanged in these
             experiments, these results are difficult to explain in terms
             of the receptive field properties of neurons early in the
             visual processing stream. Rather, the results suggest that
             the Chubb illusion, like other illusions of brightness (and
             color), are generated empirically according to what the
             sources of the same or similar stimuli have typically turned
             out to be in the experience of both the species and the
             individual.},
   Doi = {10.1167/1.3.48},
   Key = {fds268432}
}

@article{fds268433,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Purves, D},
   Title = {Perception of objects that are both rotating and
             translating},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {325-325},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/1.3.325},
   Abstract = {The velocity field generated by a rigid body can be
             decomposed into a translation and a rotation in an infinite
             number of ways. How, then, can the visual system generate a
             definite perception of such stimuli? The conceptual
             framework we used to examine this problem is that the
             physical motion underlying any image sequence is determined
             by: 1) the identity of image features in any two sequential
             images; 2) the appearance of new features in the second
             image compared to the first; 3) the disappearance of
             features in the second image; and 4) any deformation in the
             interval between the two images. Accordingly, the stochastic
             structure of identity, appearance, disappearance and
             deformation in the image plane in relation to the
             displacement of the source can be used to generate a 4
             dimensional probability distribution of physical movements
             underlying the stimulus. Using this approach to study the
             perceptions elicited by a line translating and rotating
             around a fixed center in different contexts, we found that:
             1) the moving line is perceived to rotate but not translate;
             2) the perceived centers of rotation fall on a cycloid
             defined by all the possible movements underlying the
             stimulus; and 3) contexts such as an aperture have no effect
             on the nature of this cycloid. The fact that the probability
             distribution of the physical displacements underlying the
             stimulus accounts for these remarkable percepts supports the
             conclusion that motion perception is generated on an
             entirely empirical basis.},
   Doi = {10.1167/1.3.325},
   Key = {fds268433}
}

@article{fds268434,
   Author = {Nundy, S and Shimpi, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {The relationship between luminance and brightness},
   Journal = {Journal of Vision},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {426-426},
   Publisher = {Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
             (ARVO)},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/1.3.426},
   Abstract = {How the luminance of a visual stimulus is translated into a
             perceptual value of brightness has been debated since the
             middle of the 19th C. Although it seems intuitively clear
             that the perception of brightness should scale directly with
             the intensity of the light that activates retinal receptors,
             this is not the case. Thus, doubling the luminance of a
             stimulus under the laboratory conditions in which such
             studies are typically done does not double its perceived
             brightness. The exponential relationship between luminance
             and brightness in these circumstances is referred to as the
             Weber-Fechner Law or, alternatively, as Stevens' Power Law.
             Although measurements of the relationship between luminance
             and brightness have become progressively more sophisticated,
             the reason for this relationship and its modification under
             more natural viewing conditions has never been explained.
             Operating with the assumption that the basis of these
             relationships might be based entirely on past experience, we
             analyzed the luminance of a visual stimulus as a function of
             its generative sources, i.e. the reflectance of and
             illumination giving rise to the visual stimulus in both
             restricted and more natural scenes. The results of the study
             show that the form of the luminance/brightness relationship
             changes predictably according to the relative contributions
             of reflectance and illumination that would previously have
             been experienced in the presence of the same or a similar
             stimulus. We conclude, therefore, that the relationship
             between luminance and brightness is determined empirically
             according the success or failure of visually-guided
             behavior.},
   Doi = {10.1167/1.3.426},
   Key = {fds268434}
}

@article{fds268503,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Viktor Hamburger 1900-2001.},
   Journal = {Nature Neuroscience},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {777-778},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {1097-6256},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11477419},
   Keywords = {Animals • History, 20th Century • Humans •
             Neurosciences • Portraits • United States •
             history*},
   Doi = {10.1038/90470},
   Key = {fds268503}
}

@article{fds268437,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of the Chubb illusion.},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {547-555},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11506656},
   Abstract = {The perceived difference in brightness between elements of a
             patterned target is diminished when the target is embedded
             in a similar surround of higher luminance contrast (the
             Chubb illusion). Here we show that this puzzling effect can
             be explained by the degree to which imperfect transmittance
             is likely to have affected the light that reaches the eye.
             These observations indicate that this 'illusion' is yet
             another signature of the fundamentally empirical strategy of
             visual perception, in this case generated by the typical
             influence of transmittance on inherently ambiguous
             stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.1162/089892901750363154},
   Key = {fds268437}
}

@article{fds268436,
   Author = {Yang, Z and Shimpi, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {A wholly empirical explanation of perceived
             motion.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {98},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {5252-5257},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11320255},
   Abstract = {Because the retinal activity generated by a moving object
             cannot specify which of an infinite number of possible
             physical displacements underlies the stimulus, its
             real-world cause is necessarily uncertain. How, then, do
             observers respond successfully to sequences of images whose
             provenance is ambiguous? Here we explore the hypothesis that
             the visual system solves this problem by a probabilistic
             strategy in which perceived motion is generated entirely
             according to the relative frequency of occurrence of the
             physical sources of the stimulus. The merits of this concept
             were tested by comparing the directions and speeds of moving
             lines reported by subjects to the values determined by the
             probability distribution of all the possible physical
             displacements underlying the stimulus. The velocities
             reported by observers in a variety of stimulus contexts can
             be accounted for in this way.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.091095298},
   Key = {fds268436}
}

@article{fds268435,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lotto, RB and Williams, SM and Nundy, S and Yang,
             Z},
   Title = {Why we see things the way we do: evidence for a wholly
             empirical strategy of vision.},
   Journal = {Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological Sciences},
   Volume = {356},
   Number = {1407},
   Pages = {285-297},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0962-8436},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11316481},
   Abstract = {Many otherwise puzzling aspects of the way we see
             brightness, colour, orientation and motion can be understood
             in wholly empirical terms. The evidence reviewed here leads
             to the conclusion that visual percepts are based on patterns
             of reflex neural activity shaped entirely by the past
             success (or failure) of visually guided behaviour in
             response to the same or a similar retinal stimulus. As a
             result, the images we see accord with what the sources of
             the stimuli have typically turned out to be, rather than
             with the physical properties of the relevant objects. If
             vision does indeed depend upon this operational strategy to
             generate optimally useful perceptions of inevitably
             ambiguous stimuli, then the underlying neurobiological
             processes will eventually need to be understood within this
             conceptual framework.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2000.0772},
   Key = {fds268435}
}

@article{fds114090,
   Title = {Purves, D., R.B. Lotto, S.M. Williams, S. Nundy and Z.Yang
             (2001) Why we see things the way we do: Evidence for a
             wholly empirical strategy of vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
             Lond. B, 356: 285-297.},
   Year = {2001},
   Key = {fds114090}
}

@article{fds114091,
   Title = {Yang, Z., A. Shimpi and D. Purves (2001) A wholly empirical
             explanation of perceived motion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 9:
             5252-5257.},
   Year = {2001},
   Key = {fds114091}
}

@article{fds268429,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of color contrast.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {97},
   Number = {23},
   Pages = {12834-12839},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11058148},
   Abstract = {For reasons not well understood, the color of a surface can
             appear quite different when placed in different chromatic
             surrounds. Here we explore the possibility that these color
             contrast effects are generated according to what the same or
             similar stimuli have turned out to signify in the past about
             the physical relationships between reflectance,
             illumination, and the spectral returns they produce. This
             hypothesis was evaluated by (i) comparing the physical
             relationships of reflectances, illuminants, and spectral
             returns with the perceptual phenomenology of color contrast
             and (ii) testing whether perceptions of color contrast are
             predictably changed by altering the probabilities of the
             possible sources of the stimulus. The results we describe
             are consistent with a wholly empirical explanation of color
             contrast effects.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.210369597},
   Key = {fds268429}
}

@article{fds268430,
   Author = {Nundy, S and Lotto, B and Coppola, D and Shimpi, A and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Why are angles misperceived?},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {97},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {5592-5597},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10805814},
   Abstract = {Although it has long been apparent that observers tend to
             overestimate the magnitude of acute angles and underestimate
             obtuse ones, there is no consensus about why such
             distortions are seen. Geometrical modeling combined with
             psychophysical testing of human subjects indicates that
             these misperceptions are the result of an empirical strategy
             that resolves the inherent ambiguity of angular stimuli by
             generating percepts of the past significance of the stimulus
             rather than the geometry of its retinal projection.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.97.10.5592},
   Key = {fds268430}
}

@article{fds268427,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lotto, B and Polger, T},
   Title = {Color vision and the four-color-map problem.},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {233-237},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10771407},
   Abstract = {Four different colors are needed to make maps that avoid
             adjacent countries of the same color. Because the retinal
             image is two dimensional, like a map, four dimensions of
             chromatic experience would also be needed to optimally
             distinguish regions returning spectrally different light to
             the eye. We therefore suggest that the organization of human
             color vision according to four-color classes (reds, greens,
             blues, and yellows) has arisen as a solution to this logical
             requirement in topology.},
   Doi = {10.1162/089892900562011},
   Key = {fds268427}
}

@article{fds268428,
   Author = {Purves, D and Williams, SM and Lotto, RB},
   Title = {The relevance of visual perception to cortical evolution and
             development.},
   Journal = {Novartis Foundation Symposium},
   Volume = {228},
   Pages = {240-254},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {1528-2511},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10929326},
   Abstract = {The quality of brightness--perhaps the simplest visual
             attribute we perceive--appears to be determined
             probabilistically. In this empirical conception of the
             perception of light, the stimulus-induced activity of visual
             cortical neurons does not encode the retinal image or the
             properties of the stimulus per se, but associations
             (percepts) determined by the relative probabilities of the
             possible sources of the stimulus. If this theory is correct,
             the rationale for the prolonged postnatal construction of
             visual circuitry--and the evolution of this visual
             scheme--is to strengthen and/or create by activity-dependent
             feedback the empirically determined association on which
             vision depends.},
   Doi = {10.1002/0470846631.ch16},
   Key = {fds268428}
}

@article{fds114057,
   Title = {Lotto, R.B. and D. Purves (2000) An empirical explanation of
             color contrast. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 97:
             12834-12839.},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds114057}
}

@article{fds114058,
   Title = {Purves, D., B. Lotto and T. Polger (2000) Color vision and
             the four-color-map problem. J. Cog. Neurosci. 12:
             233-237.},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds114058}
}

@article{fds114087,
   Title = {Nundy, S., B. Lotto, D. Coppola, A. Shimpi and D. Purves
             (2000) Why are angles misperceived? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
             97: 5592-5597.},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds114087}
}

@article{fds114088,
   Title = {Purves, D., G.A. Augustine, D. Fitzpatrick, L.C. Katz, A.-S.
             LaMantia and J.O. McNamara (2000) Neuroscience, 2nd edition.
             Sinauer Associates: Sunderland, MA.},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds114088}
}

@article{fds114089,
   Title = {Purves, D., S.M. Williams and R.B. Lotto (2000) The
             relevance of visual perception to cortical evolution and
             development. In: Evolutionary Developmental Biology of the
             Cerebral Cortex (Novartis Foundation Symposium Series, G.R.
             Bock and G. Cardew, eds.) John Wiley & Sons, Vol. 228, pp.
             240-258.},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds114089}
}

@article{fds268425,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Perception as probability.},
   Journal = {Brain Research Bulletin},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {5-6},
   Pages = {321-322},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0361-9230},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10643417},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0361-9230(99)00152-5},
   Key = {fds268425}
}

@article{fds268426,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Purves, D},
   Title = {The effects of color on brightness.},
   Journal = {Nature Neuroscience},
   Volume = {2},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {1010-1014},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {1097-6256},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10526341},
   Abstract = {Observation of human subjects shows that the spectral
             returns of equiluminant colored surrounds govern the
             apparent brightness of achromatic test targets. The
             influence of color on brightness provides further evidence
             that perceptions of luminance are generated according to the
             empirical frequency of the possible sources of visual
             stimuli, and suggests a novel way of understanding color
             contrast and constancy.},
   Doi = {10.1038/14808},
   Key = {fds268426}
}

@article{fds268422,
   Author = {Purves, D and Shimpi, A and Lotto, RB},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of the cornsweet
             effect.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {19},
   Pages = {8542-8551},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10493754},
   Abstract = {A long-standing puzzle in vision is the assignment of
             illusory brightness values to visual territories based on
             the characteristics of their edges (the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet
             effect). Here we show that the perception of the
             equiluminant territories flanking the Cornsweet edge varies
             according to whether these regions are more likely to be
             similarly illuminated surfaces having the same material
             properties or unequally illuminated surfaces with different
             properties. Thus, if the likelihood is increased that these
             territories are surfaces with similar reflectance properties
             under the same illuminant, the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet
             effect is diminished; conversely, if the likelihood is
             increased that the adjoining territories are differently
             reflective surfaces receiving different amounts of
             illumination, the effect is enhanced. These findings
             indicate that the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet effect is
             determined by the relative probabilities of the possible
             sources of the luminance profiles in the
             stimulus.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.19-19-08542.1999},
   Key = {fds268422}
}

@article{fds268421,
   Author = {Halpern, SD and Andrews, TJ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Interindividual variation in human visual
             performance.},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {521-534},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0898-929X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10511641},
   Abstract = {The responses of 20 young adult emmetropes with normal color
             vision were measured on a battery of visual performance
             tasks. Using previously documented tests of known
             reliability, we evaluated orientation discrimination,
             contrast sensitivity, wavelength sensitivity, vernier
             acuity, direction-of-motion detection, velocity
             discrimination, and complex form identification. Performance
             varied markedly between individuals, both on a given test
             and when the scores from all tests were combined to give an
             overall indication of visual performance. Moreover,
             individual performances on tests of contrast sensitivity,
             orientation discrimination, wavelength discrimination, and
             vernier acuity covaried, such that proficiency on one test
             predicted proficiency on the others. These results indicate
             a wide range of visual abilities among normal subjects and
             provide the basis for an overall index of visual proficiency
             that can be used to determine whether the surprisingly large
             and coordinated size differences of the components of the
             human visual system (Andrews, Halpern, & Purves, 1997) are
             reflected in corresponding variations in visual
             performance.},
   Doi = {10.1162/089892999563580},
   Key = {fds268421}
}

@article{fds268423,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Williams, SM and Purves, D},
   Title = {Erratum: An empirical basis for Mach bands (Proceedings of
             the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
             America (April 27, 1999) 96:9 (5239-5244))},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {96},
   Number = {13},
   Pages = {7610},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.13.7610-b},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.96.13.7610-b},
   Key = {fds268423}
}

@article{fds268420,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Williams, SM and Purves, D},
   Title = {Mach bands as empirically derived associations.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {96},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {5245-5250},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10220451},
   Abstract = {If Mach bands arise as an empirical consequence of
             real-world luminance profiles, several predictions follow.
             First, the appearance of Mach bands should accord with the
             appearance of naturally occurring highlights and lowlights.
             Second, altering the slope of an ambiguous luminance
             gradient so that it corresponds more closely to gradients
             that are typically adorned with luminance maxima and minima
             in the position of Mach bands should enhance the illusion.
             Third, altering a luminance gradient so that it corresponds
             more closely to gradients that normally lack luminance
             maxima and minima in the position of Mach bands should
             diminish the salience of the illusion. Fourth, the
             perception of Mach bands elicited by the same luminance
             gradient should be changed by contextual cues that indicate
             whether the gradient is more or less likely to signify a
             curved or a flat surface. Because each of these predictions
             is met, we conclude that Mach bands arise because the
             association elicited by the stimulus (the percept)
             incorporates these features as a result of past
             experience.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.96.9.5245},
   Key = {fds268420}
}

@article{fds268424,
   Author = {Lotto, RB and Williams, SM and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical basis for Mach bands.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {96},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {5239-5244},
   Year = {1999},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10220450},
   Abstract = {Mach bands, the illusory brightness maxima and minima
             perceived at the initiation and termination of luminance
             gradients, respectively, are generally considered a direct
             perceptual manifestation of lateral inhibitory interactions
             among retinal or other lower order visual neurons. Here we
             examine an alternative explanation, namely that Mach bands
             arise as a consequence of real-world luminance gradients. In
             this first of two companion papers, we analyze the natural
             sources of luminance gradients, demonstrating that
             real-world gradients arising from curved surfaces are
             ordinarily adorned by photometric highlights and lowlights
             in the position of the illusory bands. The prevalence of
             such gradients provides an empirical basis for the
             generation of this perceptual phenomenon.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.96.9.5239},
   Key = {fds268424}
}

@article{fds114046,
   Title = {Purves, D., A. Shimpi and R.B. Lotto (1999) An empirical
             explanation of the Cornsweet effect. J. Neurosci. 19:
             8542-8551.},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds114046}
}

@article{fds114056,
   Title = {Lotto, R.B. and D. Purves (1999) The effects of color on
             brightness. Nature Neurosci. 2: 1010-1014.},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds114056}
}

@article{fds114071,
   Title = {Lotto, R.B., S.M. Williams and D. Purves (1999) Mach bands
             as empirically derived associations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
             96: 5245-5250.},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds114071}
}

@article{fds114085,
   Title = {Halpern, S.D., T.J. Andrews and D. Purves (1999)
             Interindividual variation in human visual performance. J.
             Cog. Neurosci. 11: 521-534.},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds114085}
}

@article{fds114086,
   Title = {Lotto, R.B., S.M. Williams and D. Purves (1999a) An
             empirical basis for Mach bands. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 96:
             5239-5244.},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds114086}
}

@article{fds268418,
   Author = {Williams, SM and McCoy, AN and Purves, D},
   Title = {The influence of depicted illumination on
             brightness.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {95},
   Number = {22},
   Pages = {13296-13300},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9789082},
   Abstract = {The striking illusions produced by simultaneous brightness
             contrast generally are attributed to the center-surround
             receptive field organization of lower order neurons in the
             primary visual pathway. Here we show that the apparent
             brightness of test objects can be either increased or
             decreased in a predictable manner depending on how light and
             shadow are portrayed in the scene. This evidence suggests
             that perceptions of brightness are generated empirically by
             experience with luminance relationships, an idea whose
             implications we pursue in the accompanying
             paper.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.95.22.13296},
   Key = {fds268418}
}

@article{fds268419,
   Author = {Williams, SM and McCoy, AN and Purves, D},
   Title = {An empirical explanation of brightness.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {95},
   Number = {22},
   Pages = {13301-13306},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9789083},
   Abstract = {In this second part of our study on the mechanism of
             perceived brightness, we explore the effects of manipulating
             three-dimensional geometry. The additional scenes portrayed
             here demonstrate that the same luminance profile can elicit
             different sensations of brightness as a function of how the
             objects in the scene are arranged in space. This further
             evidence confirms the implication of the scenes presented in
             the accompanying paper, namely that sensations of relative
             brightness-including standard demonstrations of simultaneous
             brightness contrast-cannot arise by computations of local
             contrast. The most plausible explanation of the full range
             of perceptual phenomena we have described is an empirical
             strategy that links the luminance profile in a visual
             stimulus with an association (the percept) that represents
             the profile's most probable real-world source.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.95.22.13301},
   Key = {fds268419}
}

@article{fds268416,
   Author = {Coppola, DM and Purves, HR and McCoy, AN and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {The distribution of oriented contours in the real
             world.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {95},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {4002-4006},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9520482},
   Abstract = {In both humans and experimental animals, the ability to
             perceive contours that are vertically or horizontally
             oriented is superior to the perception of oblique angles.
             There is, however, no consensus about the developmental
             origins or functional basis of this phenomenon. Here, we
             report the analysis of a large library of digitized scenes
             using image processing with orientation-sensitive filters.
             Our results show a prevalence of vertical and horizontal
             orientations in indoor, outdoor, and even entirely natural
             settings. Because visual experience is known to influence
             the development of visual cortical circuitry, we suggest
             that this real world anisotropy is related to the enhanced
             ability of humans and other animals to process contours in
             the cardinal axes, perhaps by stimulating the development of
             a greater amount of visual circuitry devoted to processing
             vertical and horizontal contours.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.95.7.4002},
   Key = {fds268416}
}

@article{fds268417,
   Author = {Coppola, DM and White, LE and Fitzpatrick, D and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Unequal representation of cardinal and oblique contours in
             ferret visual cortex.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {95},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {2621-2623},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9482936},
   Abstract = {We have measured the amount of cortical space activated by
             differently oriented gratings in 25 adult ferrets by optical
             imaging of intrinsic signal. On average, 7% more area of the
             exposed visual cortex was preferentially activated by
             vertical and horizontal contours than by contours at oblique
             angles. This anisotropy may reflect the real-world
             prevalence of contours in the cardinal axes and could
             explain the greater sensitivity of many animals to vertical
             and horizontal stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.95.5.2621},
   Key = {fds268417}
}

@article{fds114055,
   Title = {Williams, S.M.,  A.N. McCoy and D. Purves (1998) The
             influence of depicted illumination on perceived brightness.
              Proc Natl Acad Sci 95: 13296-13300.},
   Year = {1998},
   Key = {fds114055}
}

@article{fds114069,
   Title = {Williams, S.M., A.N. McCoy and D. Purves (1998) An empirical
             explanation of brightness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 95:
             13301-13306.},
   Year = {1998},
   Key = {fds114069}
}

@article{fds114083,
   Title = {Coppola, D.M, L.E. White, D. Fitzpatrick and D. Purves
             (1998)  Unequal representation of cardinal and oblique
             contours in ferret visual cortex.  Proc Natl Acad Sci  95:
             2621-2623.},
   Year = {1998},
   Key = {fds114083}
}

@article{fds114084,
   Title = {Coppola, D.M.,  H.R. Purves, A.N .McCoy and D. Purves
             (1998)  The distribution of oriented contours in the real
             world.  Proc Natl Acad Sci 95: 4002-4006.},
   Year = {1998},
   Key = {fds114084}
}

@article{fds268414,
   Author = {Andrews, TJ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Similarities in normal and binocularly rivalrous
             viewing.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {94},
   Number = {18},
   Pages = {9905-9908},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9275224},
   Abstract = {We report here a series of observations-most of which the
             reader can experience directly-showing that distinct
             components of patterned visual stimuli (orthogonal lines of
             a different hue) vary in perception as sets. Although less
             frequent and often less complete, these perceptual
             fluctuations in normal viewing are otherwise similar to the
             binocular rivalry experienced when incompatible scenes are
             presented dichoptically.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.94.18.9905},
   Key = {fds268414}
}

@article{fds268411,
   Author = {Sporns, O},
   Title = {Variation and selection in neural function.},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {291},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)88843-1},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0166-2236(97)88843-1},
   Key = {fds268411}
}

@article{fds268415,
   Author = {Purves, D and Andrews, TJ},
   Title = {The perception of transparent three-dimensional
             objects.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {94},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {6517-6522},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9177250},
   Abstract = {When the proximal and distal elements of wire-frame cubes
             are conflated, observers perceive illusory structures that
             no longer behave veridically. These phenomena suggest that
             what we normally see depends on visual associations
             generated by experience. The necessity of such learning may
             explain why the mammalian visual system is subject to a
             prolonged period of plasticity in early life, when novel
             circuits are made in enormous numbers.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.94.12.6517},
   Key = {fds268415}
}

@article{fds268412,
   Author = {Andrews, TJ and Halpern, SD and Purves, D},
   Title = {Correlated size variations in human visual cortex, lateral
             geniculate nucleus, and optic tract.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {2859-2868},
   Year = {1997},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9092607},
   Abstract = {We have examined several components of the human visual
             system to determine how the dimensions of the optic tract,
             lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and primary visual cortex
             (V1) vary within the same brain. Measurements were made of
             the cross-sectional area of the optic tract, the volumes of
             the magnocellular and parvocellular layers of the LGN, and
             the surface area and volume of V1 in one or both cerebral
             hemispheres of 15 neurologically normal human brains
             obtained at autopsy. Consistent with previous observations,
             there was a two- to threefold variation in the size of each
             of these visual components among the individuals studied.
             Importantly, this variation was coordinated within the
             visual system of any one individual. That is, a relatively
             large V1 was associated with a commensurately large LGN and
             optic tract, whereas a relatively small V1 was associated
             with a commensurately smaller LGN and optic tract. This
             relationship among the components of the human visual system
             indicates that the development of its different parts is
             interdependent. Such coordinated variation should generate
             substantial differences in visual ability among
             humans.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.17-08-02859.1997},
   Key = {fds268412}
}

@article{fds114052,
   Title = {White, L.E., T. J. Andrews, C. Hulette, A. Richards, M.
             Groelle, J. Paydarfar and D. Purves  (1997)  Structure of
             the Human Sensorimotor System II.  Lateral symmetry.
              Cereb Cortex 7:31-47.},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds114052}
}

@article{fds114053,
   Title = {Purves, D. and T. J.  Andrews (1997)  The perception of
             transparent 3-dimensional objects.  Proc Natl Acad Sci 94:
             6517-6522.},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds114053}
}

@article{fds114054,
   Title = {Andrews T.J., and D. Purves  (1997)  Similarities in
             normal and binocularly rivalrous viewing.  Proc Natl Acad
             Sci  94: 9905-9908.},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds114054}
}

@article{fds114081,
   Title = {White L.E., T. J. Andrews, C. Hulette, A. Richards, M.
             Groelle, J. Paydarfar and D.  Purves  (1997)  Structure
             of the human sensorimotor system I.  Morphology and
             cytoarchitecture of the central sulcus.  Cereb Cortex
             7:18-30.},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds114081}
}

@article{fds114082,
   Title = {Andrews T.J., S. D. Halpern and D. Purves (1997)
              Correlated size variations in human visual cortex, lateral
             geniculate nucleus and optic tract.  J Neurosci 17:
             2859-2868.},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds114082}
}

@article{fds268473,
   Author = {White, LE and Andrews, TJ and Hulette, C and Richards, A and Groelle, M and Paydarfar, J and Purves, D},
   Title = {Structure of the human sensorimotor system. I: Morphology
             and cytoarchitecture of the central sulcus.},
   Journal = {Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991)},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {18-30},
   Year = {1997},
   ISSN = {1047-3211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9023429},
   Abstract = {We have studied the morphology of the central sulcus and the
             cytoarchitecture of the primary sensorimotor cortex in 20
             human brains obtained at autopsy. Although the surface
             appearance of the central sulcus varies greatly from brain
             to brain (and between hemispheres of individual brains), its
             deep structure is remarkably consistent. The fundus of the
             central sulcus is divided into medial and lateral limbs by a
             complex junction midway between the sagittal and Sylvian
             fissures. Based on functional imaging studies, this junction
             appears to be a structural hallmark of the sensorimotor
             representation of the distal upper extremity. We also
             identified and measured area 4 (primary motor cortex) and
             area 3 (primary somatic sensory cortex) in Nissl-stained
             sections cut orthogonal to the course of the central sulcus.
             Although the positions of the cytoarchitectonic boundaries
             in the paracentral lobule showed considerable
             interindividual variation, the locations of the borders of
             areas 4 and 3 along the course of the sulcus were similar
             among the 40 hemispheres examined. In addition to describing
             more thoroughly this portion of the human cerebral cortex,
             these observations provide a basis for evaluating lateral
             symmetry of the human primary sensorimotor
             cortex.},
   Doi = {10.1093/cercor/7.1.18},
   Key = {fds268473}
}

@article{fds268474,
   Author = {White, LE and Andrews, TJ and Hulette, C and Richards, A and Groelle, M and Paydarfar, J and Purves, D},
   Title = {Structure of the human sensorimotor system. II: Lateral
             symmetry.},
   Journal = {Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991)},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {31-47},
   Year = {1997},
   ISSN = {1047-3211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9023430},
   Abstract = {We have evaluated the lateral symmetry of the human central
             sulcus, brainstem and spinal cord using quantitative
             histological and imaging techniques in specimens from 67
             autopsy cases. Our purpose was to determine whether the
             preferred use of the right hand in the majority of humans is
             associated with grossly discernible asymmetries of the
             neural centers devoted to the upper extremities. In the
             accompanying report, we described a consistent set of
             morphological features in the depths of the central sulcus
             that localize the sensorimotor representation of the distal
             upper extremity. Measurements of the cortical surface in
             this region, and indeed throughout the entire central
             sulcus, showed no average lateral asymmetry.
             Cytoarchitectonic measurements of area 4 and area 3
             confirmed this similarity between the left and right
             hemispheres. The medullary pyramids, which contain the
             corticospinal tracts, were also symmetrical, as were the
             cross-sectional areas of white and gray matter in the
             cervical and lumbar enlargements of the spinal cord.
             Finally, we found no lateral difference in the size and
             number of motor neurons in the ventral horns at these levels
             of the cord. Based on these several observations, we
             conclude that the preferred use of the right hand in humans
             occurs without a gross lateral asymmetry of the primary
             sensorimotor system.},
   Doi = {10.1093/cercor/7.1.31},
   Key = {fds268474}
}

@article{fds268413,
   Author = {Purves, D and White, LE and Riddle, DR},
   Title = {Is neural development Darwinian?},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {460-464},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8931267},
   Abstract = {Gradually, and without much debate, the idea that the
             developing nervous system is in some sense darwinian has
             become one of the canons of neurobiology. In fact, there is
             little evidence to support this idea.},
   Doi = {10.1016/s0166-2236(96)20038-4},
   Key = {fds268413}
}

@article{fds268410,
   Author = {Coppola, D and Purves, D},
   Title = {The extraordinarily rapid disappearance of entoptic
             images.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {93},
   Number = {15},
   Pages = {8001-8004},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8755592},
   Abstract = {It has been known for more than 40 years that images fade
             from perception when they are kept at the same position on
             the retina by abrogating eye movements. Although aspects of
             this phenomenon were described earlier, the use of
             close-fitting contact lenses in the 1950s made possible a
             series of detailed observations on eye movements and visual
             continuity. In the intervening decades, many investigators
             have studied the role of image motion on visual perception.
             Although several controversies remain, it is clear that
             images deteriorate and in some cases disappear following
             stabilization; eye movements are, therefore, essential to
             sustained exoptic vision. The time course of image
             degradation has generally been reported to be a few seconds
             to a minute or more, depending upon the conditions. Here we
             show that images of entoptic vascular shadows can disappear
             in less than 80 msec. The rapid vanishing of these images
             implies an active mechanism of image erasure and creation as
             the basis of normal visual processing.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.93.15.8001},
   Key = {fds268410}
}

@article{fds268409,
   Author = {Andrews, TJ and White, LE and Binder, D and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Temporal events in cyclopean vision.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {93},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {3689-3692},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8622998},
   Abstract = {The majority of neurons in the primary visual cortex of
             primates can be activated by stimulation of either eye;
             moreover, the monocular receptive fields of such neurons are
             located in about the same region of visual space. These
             well-known facts imply that binocular convergence in visual
             cortex can explain our cyclopean view of the world. To test
             the adequacy of this assumption, we examined how human
             subjects integrate binocular events in time. Light flashes
             presented synchronously to both eyes were compared to
             flashes presented alternately (asynchronously) to one eye
             and then the other. Subjects perceived very-low-frequency (2
             Hz) asynchronous trains as equivalent to synchronous trains
             flashed at twice the frequency (the prediction based on
             binocular convergence). However, at higher frequencies of
             presentation (4-32 Hz), subjects perceived asynchronous and
             synchronous trains to be increasingly similar. Indeed, at
             the flicker-fusion frequency (approximately 50 Hz), the
             apparent difference between the two conditions was only 2%.
             We suggest that the explanation of these anomalous findings
             is that we parse visual input into sequential
             episodes.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.93.8.3689},
   Key = {fds268409}
}

@article{fds268408,
   Author = {Purves, D and Paydarfar, JA and Andrews, TJ},
   Title = {The wagon wheel illusion in movies and reality.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {93},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {3693-3697},
   Year = {1996},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8622999},
   Abstract = {Wheels turning in the movies or in other forms of
             stroboscopic presentation often appear to be rotating
             backward. Remarkably, a similar illusion is also seen in
             continuous light. The occurrence of this perception in the
             absence of intermittent illumination suggests that we
             normally see motion, as in movies, by processing a series of
             visual episodes.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.93.8.3693},
   Key = {fds268408}
}

@article{fds114050,
   Title = {Andrews, T.J., L.E. White, D. Binder and  D. Purves (1996)
              Temporal events in cyclopean vision.  Proc. Natl. Acad.
             Sci. 93: 3689-3692.},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds114050}
}

@article{fds114051,
   Title = {Purves, D., L.E. White  and D.R. Riddle (1996)  Is neural
             development Darwinian?  Trends Neurosci 19:
             460-464.},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds114051}
}

@article{fds114078,
   Title = {Purves, D., J.E. Paydarfar and  T.J. Andrews (1996)  The
             wagon wheel illusion in movies and reality.  Proc. Natl.
             Acad. Sci. 93: 3693-3697.},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds114078}
}

@article{fds114079,
   Title = {Coppola, D. and D. Purves  (1996)  The extraordinarily
             rapid disappearance of entoptic images.  Proc. Natl. Acad.
             Sci. 93: 8001-8004.},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds114079}
}

@article{fds114080,
   Title = {Purves D., L. White, D. Zheng, T. Andrews and D. Riddle
              (1996)  Brain size, behavior and the allocation of neural
             space.  In:  The Lifespan Development of Individuals:
              Behavioral, Neurobiological, and Psychosocial
             Perspectives, (Magnusson D, ed).  Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
             University Press, Chapter 8, pp. 162-178.},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds114080}
}

@article{fds268405,
   Author = {Riddle, DR and Purves, D},
   Title = {Individual variation and lateral asymmetry of the rat
             primary somatosensory cortex.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {4184-4195},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7790904},
   Abstract = {We have evaluated the interindividual variability and
             lateral symmetry of a major cortical area by comparing the
             primary somatosensory cortex (S1) of adult rats. Our choice
             of the rat was dictated by the accuracy with which one can
             measure S1 and its component representations in the rodent
             brain; the importance of such measurements lies in
             understanding the rules that govern the allocation of
             cortical space and, ultimately, the consequences of
             differential allocation for behavior. With respect to
             interindividual differences, the major somatic
             representations in S1 are surprisingly variable in size. The
             area of the whiskerpad representation, for example, ranged
             from 3.72 to 6.84 mm2 in a sample of 53 rats; other
             components of S1 showed comparable differences among
             animals. With respect to lateral symmetry, the average area
             of each major representation was similar for the right and
             left hemispheres; thus, we found no consistent bias in the
             size of S1 or its elements in the sample as a whole. Within
             individual animals, however, the sizes of the major somatic
             representations were often quite different in the two
             hemispheres. The magnitude of the lateral differences
             averaged 7.9 +/- 0.8% (mean +/- SEM) for the whisker pad
             representation, 11.6 +/- 1.3% for the upper lip, 15.4 +/-
             1.6% for the furry buccal pad, 13.9 +/- 1.4% for the lower
             jaw, and 13.3 +/- 1.2% for the forepaw. These results show
             that the amount of cortical space allocated to corresponding
             functions in individual rats--or in the two hemispheres of a
             particular rat--are often different. Such variations are
             likely to be reflected in somatosensory performance.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.15-06-04184.1995},
   Key = {fds268405}
}

@article{fds268406,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Race plus IQ does not equal science.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {374},
   Number = {6517},
   Pages = {10},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/374010d0},
   Doi = {10.1038/374010d0},
   Key = {fds268406}
}

@article{fds268407,
   Author = {Zheng, D and Purves, D},
   Title = {Effects of increased neural activity on brain
             growth.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {92},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1802-1806},
   Year = {1995},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7892181},
   Abstract = {We have measured the effects of regionally increased
             metabolic activity--and by inference electrical activity--on
             cortical growth in the developing rat brain. Cortical growth
             is significantly and specifically greater in regions of
             chronically increased activity. This effect of activity on
             cortical growth may help explain the permanent storage of
             early experience in the developing nervous
             system.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.92.6.1802},
   Key = {fds268407}
}

@article{fds114049,
   Title = {Riddle, D.R. and  D. Purves (1995)  Individual variation
             and lateral asymmetry of the rat primary somatosensory
             cortex.  J. Neurosci. 15: 4184-4195.},
   Year = {1995},
   Key = {fds114049}
}

@article{fds114075,
   Title = {Zheng, D. and D. Purves  (1995)  The effects of increased
             neural activity on brain growth.  Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
             92: 1802-1806.},
   Year = {1995},
   Key = {fds114075}
}

@article{fds114076,
   Title = {Purves, D. (1995) Race plus IQ does not equal science.
             Nature 374: 10.},
   Year = {1995},
   Key = {fds114076}
}

@article{fds114077,
   Title = {White, L.E., T.J.Andrews,  C. Hulette, A. Richards,  M.
             Groelle, J. Paydarfar and  D. Purves (1995)  Structural
             symmetry of the human sensorimotor system.  Cerebral Cortex
             (submitted).},
   Year = {1995},
   Key = {fds114077}
}

@article{fds268404,
   Author = {Purves, D and White, LE},
   Title = {Monocular preferences in binocular viewing.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {91},
   Number = {18},
   Pages = {8339-8342},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8078884},
   Abstract = {Faced with an unobstructed view, both foveas can be readily
             aligned with a distant visual target. The minor difference
             in the view of the two eyes (which arises from slightly
             different lines of sight) presents no special problem and
             is, indeed, the basis of stereopsis. However, when
             obstructing objects are present in the foreground, the view
             provided by one eye becomes wholly or partially incompatible
             with the view of the other. We have investigated how we cope
             with this everyday situation by having volunteers observe
             distant targets through a fenestrated screen. In this
             circumstance, subjects naturally position themselves to view
             a target of interest with one eye--usually the right eye.
             This monocular habit in normal viewing reinforces other
             evidence for the unorthodox idea that visual perception
             arises from a union in consciousness of monocular images
             that are elaborated independently.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.91.18.8339},
   Key = {fds268404}
}

@article{fds268403,
   Author = {Purves, D and White, LE and Andrews, TJ},
   Title = {Manual asymmetry and handedness.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {91},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {5030-5032},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8197178},
   Abstract = {Volumetric measurements show that right-handed individuals
             have larger right hands than left hands. In contrast, the
             hands of left-handers are much more nearly symmetrical.
             Based on what is known about trophic interactions between
             neurons and targets, these findings predict a corresponding
             asymmetry of the relevant parts of the sensorimotor system
             in right-handers. The lack of an opposite-hand asymmetry
             among left-handers further implies that right- and
             left-handed phenotypes do not arise according to the same
             developmental rules.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.91.11.5030},
   Key = {fds268403}
}

@article{fds268402,
   Author = {White, LE and Lucas, G and Richards, A and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Cerebral asymmetry and handedness.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {368},
   Number = {6468},
   Pages = {197-198},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0028-0836},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8145817},
   Doi = {10.1038/368197a0},
   Key = {fds268402}
}

@article{fds268401,
   Author = {Purves, D and Riddle, DR and White, LE and Gutierrez-Ospina,
             G},
   Title = {Neural activity and the development of the somatic sensory
             system.},
   Journal = {Current Opinion in Neurobiology},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {120-123},
   Year = {1994},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0959-4388},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8173318},
   Abstract = {Present thinking about the role of neural activity in the
             developing brain is based largely upon observations in the
             visual system. Attempts to generalize these findings in the
             somatic sensory system, however, have yielded perplexing
             results. Unlike the visual system, recent evidence suggests
             that activity plays a relatively minor role in establishing
             structural patterns in the primary somatic sensory cortex.
             Activity levels in the primary somatic sensory cortex are
             nonetheless highest in those regions that grow most during
             postnatal development, implying that activity promotes
             differential cortical growth.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0959-4388(94)90041-8},
   Key = {fds268401}
}

@article{fds114047,
   Title = {White, L., G. Lucas, A. Richards and  D. Purves (1994)
             Cerebral asymmetry and handedness.  Nature 368:
             197-198.},
   Year = {1994},
   Key = {fds114047}
}

@article{fds114048,
   Title = {Purves, D. (1994) Neural Activity and the Growth of the
             Brain. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
             Press.},
   Year = {1994},
   Key = {fds114048}
}

@article{fds114072,
   Title = {Purves, D.,  L. White and T. Andrews  (1994)  Manual
             asymmetry and handedness.  Proc Natl Acad Sci 91:
             5030-5032.},
   Year = {1994},
   Key = {fds114072}
}

@article{fds114073,
   Title = {Purves, D.,  D.Riddle,  L. White and  G. Gutierrez (1994)
             Neural activity and the development of the somatic sensory
             system.  Curr Opin Neurobiol 4: 120-123.},
   Year = {1994},
   Key = {fds114073}
}

@article{fds114074,
   Title = {Purves, D. and L. E. White (1994) Monocular preferences in
             binocular viewing.  Proc Natl Acad Sci 91:
             8339-8342.},
   Year = {1994},
   Key = {fds114074}
}

@article{fds268400,
   Author = {Purves, D and Riddle, DR and White, LE and Gutierrez-Ospina, G and LaMantia, AS},
   Title = {Categories of cortical structure.},
   Journal = {Progress in Brain Research},
   Volume = {102},
   Pages = {343-355},
   Year = {1994},
   ISSN = {0079-6123},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7800824},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60551-8},
   Key = {fds268400}
}

@article{fds268397,
   Author = {Riddle, DR and Gutierrez, G and Zheng, D and White, LE and Richards, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {Differential metabolic and electrical activity in the
             somatic sensory cortex of juvenile and adult
             rats.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {4193-4213},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8410183},
   Abstract = {We have examined relative levels of metabolic and electrical
             activity across layer IV in the primary somatic sensory
             cortex (S1) of the rat in relation to regions of
             differential postnatal cortical growth. Each of several
             indices used--mitochondrial enzyme histochemistry,
             microvessel density, Na+/K+ pump activity, action potential
             frequency, and deoxyglucose uptake--indicate regional
             variations of metabolic and electrical activity in this part
             of the brain in both juvenile (1-week-old) and adult
             (10-12-week-old) animals. At both ages, areas of the somatic
             sensory map related to special sensors such as whiskers and
             digital pads showed evidence of the most intense activity.
             Thus, mitochondrial enzyme staining, blood vessel density,
             and Na+/K+ ATPase activity were all greatest in the barrels
             and barrel-like structures within S1, and least in the
             adjacent interbarrel cortex and the cortex surrounding S1.
             Multiunit recordings in and around the posteromedial barrel
             subfield of anesthetized animals also showed that the
             average ratio of evoked to spontaneous activity was greater
             in barrels than in the surrounding, metabolically less
             active cortex. Furthermore, autoradiograms of labeled
             deoxyglucose accumulation in awake behaving animals
             indicated systematic differences in neural activity across
             S1 barrels and barrel-like structures showed more
             deoxyglucose accumulation than interbarrel, nonbarrel, or
             peri-S1 cortex. These regional differences in neural
             activity correspond to regional differences in neocortical
             growth (Riddle et al., 1992). The correlation of greater
             electrical activity, increased metabolism, and enhanced
             cortical growth during postnatal maturation suggests that
             neural activity foments the elaboration of circuitry in the
             developing brain.},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-10-04193.1993},
   Key = {fds268397}
}

@article{fds268396,
   Author = {Purves, D and LaMantia, A},
   Title = {Development of blobs in the visual cortex of
             macaques.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Comparative Neurology},
   Volume = {334},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {169-175},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0021-9967},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8366193},
   Abstract = {We have examined the area of the primary visual cortex and
             the number and size of blobs within it in 10 neonatal and 11
             adult rhesus monkeys. The average area of the primary visual
             cortex (V1) increases from 919 mm2 in newborns to 1,069 mm2
             in adult animals (16%). The number of blobs decreases per
             unit area from an average of 5.2/mm2 at birth to 4.3/mm2 in
             maturity (18%). As a consequence, the number of blobs
             remains approximately the same during maturation, at about
             4,800/hemisphere. These observations correct a preliminary
             report on a subset of the animals studied here (Purves and
             LaMantia: Proc Natl Acad Sci 87:5765, '90), in which it
             appeared that blob number might increase between birth and
             maturity. As in other regions of the developing postnatal
             brain, we found no net loss of modular circuitry.},
   Doi = {10.1002/cne.903340202},
   Key = {fds268396}
}

@article{fds268399,
   Author = {Purves, D and Riddle, D and LaMantia, A},
   Title = {Reply},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {180-181},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1993},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(93)90149-G},
   Doi = {10.1016/0166-2236(93)90149-G},
   Key = {fds268399}
}

@article{fds114070,
   Title = {Riddle, D.R., G. Gutierrez, D. Zheng, L. White, A. Richards
             and D. Purves (1993) Differential metabolic and electrical
             activity in the somatic sensory cortex of juvenile and adult
             rats. J. Neurosci. 13: 4193-4213.},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds114070}
}

@article{fds268398,
   Author = {Hevner, RF and Illing, RB and Purves, D and Riddle, D and LaMantia,
             A},
   Title = {More modules [2]},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {178-180},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds268398}
}

@article{fds268395,
   Author = {Purves, D and Riddle, DR and LaMantia, AS},
   Title = {Iterated patterns of brain circuitry (or how the cortex gets
             its spots)},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {362-368},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1279855},
   Abstract = {The prominence of repeating patterns of circuitry in the
             mammalian brain has led to the general view that iterated
             modular units reflect a fundamental principle of cortical
             function. Here we argue that these intriguing patterns arise
             not because the functional organization of the brain demands
             them, but as an incidental consequence of the rules of
             synapse formation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0166-2236(92)90180-g},
   Key = {fds268395}
}

@article{fds268394,
   Author = {Riddle, D and Richards, A and Zsuppan, F and Purves,
             D},
   Title = {Growth of the rat somatic sensory cortex and its constituent
             parts during postnatal development.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {3509-3524},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1527593},
   Abstract = {We have compared the size and arrangement of the primary
             somatic sensory cortex (SI) and its constituent parts in
             juvenile (1 week old) and mature (10-12 weeks old) rats
             using succinic dehydrogenase histochemistry and digital
             image analysis. Our goal was to determine whether some
             regions of the maturing cortex grow more than others. To
             this end, we examined (1) the growth of barrels and the
             surrounding (interbarrel) cortex, (2) the growth of the
             major somatic representations within SI, and (3) the overall
             growth of SI compared to the neocortex as a whole. With
             respect to the first of these issues, SI barrels and
             barrel-like structures grow more than the intervening
             cortex. The growth of these elements varies according to
             region: barrels in the head representation more than double
             in size, whereas the barrel-like structures in the paw
             representations increase by only about half this amount. The
             growth of the major somatic representations within SI is
             also heterogeneous, the representation of the head enlarging
             to a greater extent than the representations of the paws.
             Thus, the ratio of the total area of head representation to
             the combined paw representation is 15% greater in adults
             than in juveniles. Finally, the primary somatic sensory
             cortex grows to a somewhat greater extent than the neocortex
             as a whole. These observations demonstrate that postnatal
             cortical growth is not uniform; it varies among cortical
             barrels and the immediately surrounding (interbarrel)
             cortex, among the representations of different body parts,
             and between SI and the rest of the neocortex. As an
             explanation of this differential growth, we suggest that the
             neuropil of metabolically (and/or electrically) more active
             cortical regions grows to a greater extent during maturation
             than that of less active regions.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.12-09-03509.1992},
   Key = {fds268394}
}

@article{fds268393,
   Author = {LaMantia, AS and Pomeroy, SL and Purves, D},
   Title = {Vital imaging of glomeruli in the mouse olfactory
             bulb.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {976-988},
   Year = {1992},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1545246},
   Abstract = {We have monitored the pattern of identified glomeruli in the
             olfactory bulbs of newborn, juvenile, and adult mice over
             intervals of several hours to several weeks. Our purpose was
             to assess the development and stability of these complex
             units in the mammalian brain. Glomeruli can be observed by
             vital fluorescent staining and laser-scanning confocal
             microscopy without causing acute or long-term damage to
             brain tissue. Repeated observation of bulbs in the same
             animals between birth and 3 weeks of age showed that this
             region of the brain develops by progressive addition of
             these units to the original population. This increment
             occurs by the genesis of smaller new glomeruli between
             larger existing ones; no elimination of glomeruli was
             observed during this process. Finally, no addition (or loss)
             of glomeruli occurred in adult animals over a 2 week
             interval; once established, the number, size, and pattern of
             glomeruli are evidently stable.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.12-03-00976.1992},
   Key = {fds268393}
}

@article{fds114045,
   Title = {LaMantia, A-S., S. Pomeroy and D. Purves (1992) Vital
             imaging of glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb. J.
             Neurosci. 12: 976-988.},
   Year = {1992},
   Key = {fds114045}
}

@article{fds268392,
   Author = {Zheng, D and LaMantia, AS and Purves, D},
   Title = {Specialized vascularization of the primate visual
             cortex.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {2622-2629},
   Year = {1991},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1714496},
   Abstract = {We have analyzed blood vessel distribution in the primary
             and secondary visual cortices of the squirrel monkey in
             relation to cortical modules, laminae, and cytoarchitectonic
             areas. Measurements of microvessel length in tangential
             sections through the primary visual cortex showed that blobs
             are more richly vascularized than intervening cortical
             regions. Thus, the mean total length of microvessel profiles
             per unit was 42% greater within these cortical modules than
             within adjacent (interblob) areas. Total microvessel length
             per unit area in another class of module, the stripes in the
             secondary visual cortex, was 27% greater than in interstripe
             regions. Microvessel distribution also varied systematically
             from layer to layer in the primary visual cortex, being
             greatest in lamina IVc. Finally, the overall microvessel
             length per unit area in sections of the primary visual
             cortex was 26% greater than that in the secondary visual
             cortex. These observations indicate that the modular,
             laminar, and regional organization of the primate visual
             cortex is reflected in the underlying distribution of
             cortical microvessels. These vascular patterns should be
             discernable in living animals with vascular contrast agents
             and appropriate imaging techniques.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.11-08-02622.1991},
   Key = {fds268392}
}

@article{fds268389,
   Author = {Purves, D and LaMantia, AS},
   Title = {Numbers of "blobs" in the primary visual cortex of neonatal
             and adult monkeys.},
   Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
             United States of America},
   Volume = {87},
   Number = {15},
   Pages = {5764-5767},
   Year = {1990},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0027-8424},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.87.15.5764},
   Abstract = {We have examined the number of "blobs" (cytochrome
             oxidase-positive cortical modules) in the primary visual
             cortex (area 17) of infant and adult rhesus monkeys. The
             density of these iterated circuits--about five per mm2--was
             not significantly different in three newborn and three
             mature animals. Measurement of the surface of area 17 in
             serial sections, however, showed that the total area
             occupied by the primary visual cortex increases by about 50%
             during maturation. Based on these measurements, the number
             of blobs in this species is about 8000 at birth and about
             12,000 in maturity. Evidently, these complex functional
             units are added gradually to the developing primate brain
             over a period that extends into postnatal
             life.},
   Doi = {10.1073/pnas.87.15.5764},
   Key = {fds268389}
}

@article{fds268391,
   Author = {Pomeroy, SL and LaMantia, AS and Purves, D},
   Title = {Postnatal construction of neural circuitry in the mouse
             olfactory bulb.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1952-1966},
   Year = {1990},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.10-06-01952.1990},
   Abstract = {We have undertaken a quantitative analysis of the mouse
             olfactory bulb to address several major questions concerning
             the development of neural circuitry in the postnatal
             mammalian brain. These are: (1) To what degree are new
             elements and circuits added during maturation? (2) How long
             do such processes go on? and (3) Does postnatal development
             involve a net addition of circuits and their constituent
             elements, or is there elimination of some portion of an
             initial surfeit? Using male mice of known age, weight, and
             length, we measured the overall size of the bulb, the
             numbers of processing units (glomeruli) within the bulb, the
             extent and complexity of postsynaptic dendrites within the
             glomeruli, and the number of synapses in different regions
             of the bulb. Between birth and the time mice reach sexual
             maturity at 6-7 weeks of age, the bulb increases in size by
             a factor of 8, the number of glomeruli by a factor of 4-5,
             the length of mitral cell dendritic branches by a factor of
             11, and the number of glomerular and extraglomerular
             synapses by factors of 90 and 170, respectively. Each of
             these parameters increases steadily from birth, in concert
             with the enlargement of the olfactory mucosa, the overall
             growth of the brain, and indeed, of the entire animal. We
             found no evidence of an initial surfeit of processing units,
             dendritic branches, or synapses. Further elaboration of
             neural circuitry by each of these measures is also apparent
             from the time of sexual maturity until the animals reach
             their full adult size at about 10-12 weeks of age. The
             developmental strategy in this part of the mouse brain
             evidently involves prolonged construction that persists
             until the growth of the body is complete. This ongoing
             elaboration of neural circuitry in the postnatal mammalian
             brain may be relevant to understanding a number of
             unexplained developmental phenomena, including critical
             periods, the ability of the juvenile brain to recover from
             injuries that would cause severe and permanent deficits in
             older animals, and the special ability of the maturing brain
             to encode large amounts of new information.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.10-06-01952.1990},
   Key = {fds268391}
}

@article{fds268390,
   Author = {Purves, D and LaMantia, AS},
   Title = {Construction of modular circuits in the mammalian
             brain.},
   Journal = {Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative
             Biology},
   Volume = {55},
   Pages = {445-452},
   Year = {1990},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0091-7451},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2132831},
   Abstract = {Comparison of seemingly different modular units in the
             mammalian brain raises the possibility of a common mechanism
             for their formation: the growth of neuropil mediated by
             trophic interactions. The ongoing postnatal construction of
             modular circuits according to trophic interplay may in turn
             account for the remarkable plasticity of the juvenile brain.
             By the same token, the normal waning of circuit construction
             during postnatal development may explain the end of critical
             periods, the diminished ability to recover from injury in
             older animals, and the decline with increasing age in the
             ability of mammals to learn complex skills.},
   Doi = {10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.044},
   Key = {fds268390}
}

@article{fds268385,
   Author = {LaMantia, AS and Purves, D},
   Title = {Development of glomerular pattern visualized in the
             olfactory bulbs of living mice.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {341},
   Number = {6243},
   Pages = {646-649},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/341646a0},
   Abstract = {Many regions of the mammalian brain are characterized by
             iterated ensembles of nerve cells which can be distinguished
             anatomically and physiologically. A particularly striking
             example is the pattern of glomeruli in the olfactory bulbs;
             other instances are columns and 'blobs' in the visual
             cortex, barrels and columns in the somatosensory cortex, and
             striasomes and cell islands in the neostriatum.
             Understanding the generation of these neuronal ensembles has
             a bearing on a variety of important neurobiological
             problems, including the nature of critical periods, the
             age-dependent response of the nervous system to injury and
             the manner in which neural information is stored. Analysis
             of these issues has usually been restricted to studies of
             the brains of different individuals at various ages. Many
             questions about the formation of such units, however, can
             only be answered by observing the same brain repeatedly in a
             living animal. This strategy would enable a direct
             assessment of how these units are assembled, whether the
             initial ensembles persist and whether units are lost or
             gained as an animal matures. We have succeeded in studying
             the pattern of glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb on two
             separate occasions during postnatal development. Comparison
             of the patterns observed at intervals of up to three weeks
             show that this part of the brain is gradually constructed by
             the addition of new glomeruli to a persisting
             population.},
   Doi = {10.1038/341646a0},
   Key = {fds268385}
}

@article{fds268386,
   Author = {Harris, LW and Purves, D},
   Title = {Rapid remodeling of sensory endings in the corneas of living
             mice.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {2210-2214},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.09-06-02210.1989},
   Abstract = {The terminals of trigeminal neurons were followed over time
             in the corneas of living mice by repeated staining with a
             nontoxic fluorescent dye. The purpose of these observations
             was to evaluate remodeling of sensory nerve endings in an
             adult mammal. Video images of topically stained nerve
             endings within particular corneal regions were recorded
             initially, and then again after intervals ranging from 4 hr
             to 30 d. Comparison of the 2 sets of images showed that
             sensory endings in the corneal epithelium undergo continual
             rearrangement under normal circumstances. Substantial
             changes in terminal configuration occurred over periods as
             brief as a day.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.09-06-02210.1989},
   Key = {fds268386}
}

@article{fds268387,
   Author = {Ivanov, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {Ongoing electrical activity of superior cervical ganglion
             cells in mammals of different size.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Comparative Neurology},
   Volume = {284},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {398-404},
   Year = {1989},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.902840307},
   Abstract = {The ongoing synaptic activity of superior cervical ganglion
             cells in adult mammals was examined in situ by intracellular
             recording in anesthetized mice, hamsters, rats, guinea pigs,
             and rabbits. The proportion of neurons exhibiting
             subthreshold and suprathreshold synaptic activity during a
             standard period of observation was least in a small mammal
             like the mouse (30%), intermediate among neurons of mammals
             of intermediate size such as the hamster and rat (48% and
             45%, respectively), and greatest in the largest animals in
             the series, the guinea pig (89%) and rabbit (91%). Ganglion
             cells in all species fell silent after transection of the
             cervical trunk. The average frequency of synaptic activity
             among tonically active cells also increased with animal
             size, being least in the mouse (1.0/second) and greatest in
             the rabbit (6.4/second). This variation of ongoing synaptic
             activity in sympathetic ganglion cells may reflect the
             demands of progressively larger peripheral targets on
             relatively fixed populations of autonomic
             neurons.},
   Doi = {10.1002/cne.902840307},
   Key = {fds268387}
}

@article{fds268388,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Assessing some dynamic properties of the living nervous
             system},
   Journal = {Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1089-1105},
   Year = {1989},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/expphysiol.1989.sp003335},
   Doi = {10.1113/expphysiol.1989.sp003335},
   Key = {fds268388}
}

@article{fds268384,
   Author = {Purves, D and Snider, WD and Voyvodic, JT},
   Title = {Trophic regulation of nerve cell morphology and innervation
             in the autonomic nervous system.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {336},
   Number = {6195},
   Pages = {123-128},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0028-0836},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3054564},
   Abstract = {A remarkable feature of nerve cells is the complex and
             variable pattern of their axonal and dendritic branches.
             Quantitative studies of a simple part of the nervous system
             in mammals provide evidence that neuronal geometry and
             innervation are regulated by long-term trophic interactions
             between neurons and their targets. This trophic linkage may
             explain how nerve cells adjust their function to the needs
             of bodies that vary markedly in size and
             form.},
   Doi = {10.1038/336123a0},
   Key = {fds268384}
}

@article{fds268383,
   Author = {Pomeroy, SL and Purves, D},
   Title = {Neuron/glia relationships observed over intervals of several
             months in living mice.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Cell Biology},
   Volume = {107},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {1167-1175},
   Year = {1988},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1083/jcb.107.3.1167},
   Abstract = {Identified neurons and glial cells in a parasympathetic
             ganglion were observed in situ with video-enhanced
             microscopy at intervals of up to 130 d in adult mice.
             Whereas the number and position of glial cells associated
             with particular neurons did not change over several hours,
             progressive differences were evident over intervals of weeks
             to months. These changes involved differences in the
             location of glial nuclei on the neuronal surface,
             differences in the apparent number of glial nuclei
             associated with each neuron, and often both. When we
             examined the arrangement of neurons and glial cells in the
             electron microscope, we also found that presynaptic nerve
             terminals are more prevalent in the vicinity of glial nuclei
             than elsewhere on the neuronal surface. The fact that glial
             nuclei are associated with preganglionic endings, together
             with the finding that the position and number of glial
             nuclei associated with identified neurons gradually changes,
             is in accord with the recent observation that synapses on
             these neurons are normally subject to ongoing rearrangement
             (Purves, D., J. T. Voyvodic, L. Magrassi, and H. Yawo. 1987.
             Science (Wash. DC). 238:1122-1126). By the same token, the
             present results suggest that glial cells are involved in
             synaptic remodeling.},
   Doi = {10.1083/jcb.107.3.1167},
   Key = {fds268383}
}

@article{fds114044,
   Title = {Purves, D., W.D. Snider and J.T. Voyvodic (1988) Trophic
             regulation of nerve cell morphology and innervation in the
             autonomic nervous system. Nature 336: 123-128.},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds114044}
}

@article{fds268380,
   Author = {Purves, D and Voyvodic, JT and Magrassi, L and Yawo,
             H},
   Title = {Nerve terminal remodeling visualized in living mice by
             repeated examination of the same neuron.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {238},
   Number = {4830},
   Pages = {1122-1126},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0036-8075},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3685967},
   Abstract = {The distribution of presynaptic endings on the surfaces of
             autonomic ganglion cells was mapped in living mice after
             intravenous administration of a styryl pyridinium dye. The
             staining and imaging techniques did not appear to damage the
             ganglion cells, or the synapses on them; these procedures
             could therefore be repeated after an arbitrary period.
             Observations of the same neurons at intervals of up to 3
             weeks indicate that the pattern of preganglionic terminals
             on many of these nerve cells gradually changes.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.3685967},
   Key = {fds268380}
}

@article{fds268496,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lichtman, JW},
   Title = {Synaptic sites on reinnervated nerve cells visualized at two
             different times in living mice.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1492-1497},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.07-05-01492.1987},
   Abstract = {Synaptic boutons on the surface of identified autonomic
             ganglion cells were visualized by methylene blue staining at
             intervals of 1-2 months following denervation to assess
             whether regenerating axon terminals reoccupy original
             synaptic sites. The distribution of synapses observed on the
             same neuronal cell bodies was almost always different in
             appearance after reinnervation. These results are at odds
             with the conclusions of earlier workers, who have argued
             that mammalian neurons bear a fixed number of synaptic
             sites, which are reoccupied during reinnervation.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.07-05-01492.1987},
   Key = {fds268496}
}

@article{fds268494,
   Author = {Lichtman, JW and Magrassi, L and Purves, D},
   Title = {Visualization of neuromuscular junctions over periods of
             several months in living mice.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1215-1222},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.07-04-01215.1987},
   Abstract = {Identified neuromuscular junctions were followed in the
             sternomastoid muscle of living mice for several months by
             repeated staining with the fluorescent dye
             4-(4-diethylaminostyryl)-N-methylpyridinium iodide
             (4-Di-2-ASP; Magrassi et al., 1987). Overall terminal growth
             occurred at many endplates; however, the branching pattern
             of presynaptic arbors was largely unchanged, even after
             several months. The absence of significant remodeling over
             time was not a result of dye-staining, since sprouting was
             readily induced at residual motor endings by partial
             denervation. We conclude that--apart from overall
             growth--most neuromuscular junctions in the adult mouse are
             stable over intervals that represent a significant fraction
             of the animal's lifetime.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.07-04-01215.1987},
   Key = {fds268494}
}

@article{fds268495,
   Author = {Magrassi, L and Purves, D and Lichtman, JW},
   Title = {Fluorescent probes that stain living nerve
             terminals.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1207-1214},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.07-04-01207.1987},
   Abstract = {We have evaluated the efficacy of 18 cationic mitochondrial
             dyes that, as a class, show some ability to stain living
             nerve terminals. Several of these agents provide excellent
             staining of neuromuscular junctions in a wide range of
             species. More detailed studies of the most effective of
             these dyes--4-(4-diethylaminostyryl)-N-methylpyridinium
             iodide (4-Di-2-ASP)--indicate that it has no lasting effect
             on the structure or function of motor nerve terminals. As
             demonstrated in the accompanying paper (Lichtman et al.,
             1987; see also Lichtman et al., 1986), 4-Di-2-ASP can
             therefore be used to follow the configuration of identified
             motor terminals over arbitrarily long intervals.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.07-04-01207.1987},
   Key = {fds268495}
}

@article{fds268381,
   Author = {Purves, D and Sanes, JR},
   Title = {The 1986 Nobel Prize in physiology or medicine},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {231-235},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(87)90163-9},
   Doi = {10.1016/0166-2236(87)90163-9},
   Key = {fds268381}
}

@article{fds268382,
   Author = {Purves, D and Voyvodic, JT},
   Title = {Imaging mammalian nerve cells and their connections over
             time in living animals},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {398-404},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1987},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(87)90005-1},
   Abstract = {A variety of technical advances have provided a means of
             following individual nerve cells and their connections over
             intervals of weeks or months in living animals. Such
             observations allow an assessment of the stability of pre-
             and postsynaptic elements in several regions of the nervous
             system of adult mammals. © 1987.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0166-2236(87)90005-1},
   Key = {fds268382}
}

@article{fds268379,
   Author = {Purves, D and Hadley, RD and Voyvodic, JT},
   Title = {Dynamic changes in the dendritic geometry of individual
             neurons visualized over periods of up to three months in the
             superior cervical ganglion of living mice.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1051-1060},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {0270-6474},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3701409},
   Abstract = {We describe a means of visualizing the same neuron in the
             superior cervical ganglion of young adult mice over
             intervals of up to 3 months. The dendrites of these neurons
             change during this interval; some branches retract, others
             elongate, and still others appear to form de novo. Thus,
             neuronal dendrites in this part of the nervous system are
             subject to continual change beyond what is usually
             considered the developmental period. The remodeling of
             postsynaptic processes further implies that the synaptic
             connections made onto these cells undergo substantial
             rearrangement well into adulthood.},
   Doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-04-01051.1986},
   Key = {fds268379}
}

@article{fds268492,
   Author = {Purves, D and Rubin, E and Snider, WD and Lichtman,
             J},
   Title = {Relation of animal size to convergence, divergence, and
             neuronal number in peripheral sympathetic
             pathways.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {158-163},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.06-01-00158.1986},
   Abstract = {The enormous range of animal size raises a fundamental
             problem: How do larger animals maintain adequate control of
             peripheral structures that are many times more massive and
             extensive than the homologous structures in smaller animals?
             To explore this question, we have determined neuronal
             number, the number of axons that innervate each neuron
             (convergence) and the number of neurons innervated by each
             axon (divergence), in a peripheral sympathetic pathway of
             several mammals (mouse, hamster, rat, guinea pig, and
             rabbit). The average adult weights of these species vary
             over approximately a 65-fold range. However, the number of
             superior cervical ganglion cells increases by only a factor
             of 4 between the smallest of these animals (mice; about 25
             gm) and the largest (rabbits; about 1700 gm); the number of
             spinal preganglionic neurons that innervate the ganglion
             increases by only a factor of 2. Thus, the number of nerve
             cells in the sympathetic system does not increase in
             proportion to animal size. On the other hand, our results
             indicate that there are systematic differences across these
             species in the number of axons that innervate each ganglion
             cell and in the number of ganglion cells innervated by each
             axon. We suggest that modulation of convergence and
             divergence in sympathetic ganglia allows this part of the
             nervous system to effectively activate homologous peripheral
             targets over a wide range of animal size.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.06-01-00158.1986},
   Key = {fds268492}
}

@article{fds268493,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {The trophic theory of neural concentrations},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {C},
   Pages = {486-489},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1986},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(86)90155-4},
   Doi = {10.1016/0166-2236(86)90155-4},
   Key = {fds268493}
}

@article{fds114068,
   Title = {Purves, D., R.D. Hadley and J. Voyvodic (1986) Dynamic
             changes in the dendritic geometry of individual neurons
             visualized over periods of up to three months in the
             superior cervical ganglion of living mice. J. Neurosci. 6:
             1051-1060.},
   Year = {1986},
   Key = {fds114068}
}

@article{fds268377,
   Author = {Easter, SS and Purves, D and Rakic, P and Spitzer,
             NC},
   Title = {The changing view of neural specificity.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {230},
   Number = {4725},
   Pages = {507-511},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0036-8075},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.4048944},
   Abstract = {The generation of specific patterns of neuronal connections
             has usually been regarded as a central problem in
             neurobiology. The prevailing view for many years has been
             that these connections are established by complementary
             recognition molecules on the pre- and postsynaptic cells
             (the chemoaffinity theory). Experimental results obtained in
             the past decade, however, indicate that the view that axon
             guidance and synaptogenesis proceed according to restrictive
             chemical markers is too narrow. Although a more rigid plan
             may prevail in some invertebrates, the formation of specific
             connections in vertebrates also involves competition between
             axon terminals, trophic feedback between pre- and
             postsynaptic cells, and modification of connections by
             functional activity.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.4048944},
   Key = {fds268377}
}

@article{fds268378,
   Author = {Purves, D and Hadley, RD},
   Title = {Changes in the dendritic branching of adult mammalian
             neurones revealed by repeated imaging in
             situ.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {315},
   Number = {6018},
   Pages = {404-406},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/315404a0},
   Abstract = {A major obstacle to understanding the mechanism of long-term
             change in the vertebrate nervous system has been the
             inability to observe the same nerve cell at different times
             during the life of an animal. The possibility that changes
             in neural connectivity underlie the remarkable flexibility
             of the nervous systems of mammals has therefore not been
             tested by direct observation. Here, we report studies in
             which we have visualized the same neurone in the superior
             cervical ganglion of young adult mice at intervals of up to
             33 days. This collection of nerve cells is particularly
             accessible and therefore well suited to our approach. We
             find that the dendritic branches of the neurones examined
             change appreciably over intervals of 2 weeks or more; some
             branches retract, others elongate and others seem to form de
             novo. The apparent remodelling of these postsynaptic
             elements implies that the synaptic connections of these
             cells normally undergo significant rearrangement beyond what
             is usually considered to be the developmental
             period.},
   Doi = {10.1038/315404a0},
   Key = {fds268378}
}

@article{fds268376,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lichtman, JW},
   Title = {Geometrical differences among homologous neurons in
             mammals.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {228},
   Number = {4697},
   Pages = {298-302},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.3983631},
   Abstract = {The dendritic arbors of sympathetic neurons in different
             species of mammals vary systematically: the superior
             cervical ganglion cells of smaller mammals have fewer and
             less extensive dendrites than the homologous neurons in
             larger animals. This difference in dendritic complexity
             according to body size is reflected in the convergence of
             ganglionic innervation; the ganglion cells of progressively
             larger mammals are innervated by progressively more axons.
             These relations have implications both for the function of
             homologous neural systems in animals of different sizes and
             for the regulation of neuronal geometry during
             development.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.3983631},
   Key = {fds268376}
}

@article{fds114067,
   Title = {Purves, D. and J.W. Lichtman (1985) Geometrical differences
             among homologous neurons in mammals. Science 228:
             298-302.},
   Year = {1985},
   Key = {fds114067}
}

@article{fds268375,
   Author = {Forehand, CJ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Regional innervation of rabbit ciliary ganglion cells by the
             terminals of preganglionic axons.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-12},
   Year = {1984},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.04-01-00001.1984},
   Abstract = {In the rabbit, ciliary ganglion neurons with dendrites
             maintain inputs from several different axons during the
             period of synaptic rearrangement that occurs in early
             postnatal life. Neurons without dendrites, on the other
             hand, lose the majority of their initial inputs and are
             innervated in maturity by the terminals of only one or two
             axons (Purves, D., and R.I. Hume (1981) J. Neurosci. 1:
             441-452; Hume, R.I., and D. Purves (1981) Nature 293:
             469-471). We have explored the basis of this phenomenon by
             individually marking preganglionic axons and the neurons
             they innervate with horseradish peroxidase. In general, the
             innervation of geometrically complex (multiply innervated)
             neurons by individual preganglionic axons is regional. That
             is, the synaptic contacts made by an axon on these neurons
             are limited to a portion of the postsynaptic surface that
             includes some, but not all, of the dendrites. This regional
             innervation of target neurons is consistent with the view
             that dendrites allow multiple innervation to persist by
             providing relatively separate postsynaptic domains for
             individual preganglionic axons. Such regional innervation
             may mitigate competitive interactions between the several
             axons which initially innervate the same
             neuron.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.04-01-00001.1984},
   Key = {fds268375}
}

@article{fds268369,
   Author = {Johnson, DA and Purves, D},
   Title = {Tonic and reflex synaptic activity recorded in ciliary
             ganglion cells of anaesthetized rabbits.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {339},
   Pages = {599-613},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014737},
   Abstract = {We have studied patterns of synaptic activity in rabbit
             ciliary ganglion cells by intracellular recording in vivo,
             and have examined the morphology of functionally
             characterized neurones by intracellular injection of
             horseradish peroxidase. Nearly all of the neurones studied
             (293 of 300) received tonic synaptic input from
             preganglionic neurones. This tonic activity was not
             decreased by darkness or by acute optic nerve section. The
             rate of tonic synaptic activity recorded in the vast
             majority of neurones (94%) changed in response to retinal
             illumination. Most ganglion cells showed an increased rate;
             some cells, however, showed decreased activity during
             illumination. The rate of synaptic activity recorded in
             ciliary neurones tended to be progressively higher in
             neurones with more complex geometries. Neurones with similar
             reflex properties included cells that lacked dendrites and
             cells with complex dendritic arborizations; conversely,
             neurones with similar geometries often had different reflex
             characteristics. The synaptic activity arising from
             different preganglionic axons innervating the same ganglion
             cell was not temporally linked in any obvious way. The
             relevance of these results to the regulation of the number
             of axons that innervate target neurones is
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014737},
   Key = {fds268369}
}

@article{fds268368,
   Author = {Hume, RI and Purves, D},
   Title = {Apportionment of the terminals from single preganglionic
             axons to target neurones in the rabbit ciliary
             ganglion.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {338},
   Pages = {259-275},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014672},
   Abstract = {We have studied the apportionment of terminals from single
             preganglionic axons to target neurones in the ciliary
             ganglion of adult rabbits. Both electrical recording and
             intra-axonal injection of horseradish peroxidase (HRP)
             showed that each preganglionic axon innervates only a small
             fraction of the ganglion cell population (about 10-20 of the
             approximately 400 ganglion cells). Examination of ganglia in
             whole mounts showed that neurones whose cell bodies were
             enveloped by HRP-labelled boutons from a single axon were
             often surrounded by other neurones which received no
             contacts from the labelled fibre. Electron microscopical
             examination of labelled presynaptic terminals on individual
             ganglion cells confirmed that the boutons of single axons
             were sharply confined to particular target cells. This
             suggests that individual target neurones (or portions of
             them) are the unit of innervation during the development of
             these synaptic connexions. Comparison of the amplitudes of
             synaptic responses in singly and multiply innervated
             ganglion cells indicated that, on average, an individual
             axon made a weaker synaptic connexion with a multiply
             innervated neurone than with neurone that received only one
             input. Moreover, neurones innervated by several different
             axons tended to have fewer synapses on their somata than
             neurones innervated by only one or two preganglionic axons.
             Individual post-synaptic profiles were often contacted
             exclusively by labelled terminals when examined in the
             electron microscope. Since many of these neurones are
             multiply innervated, this observation suggests some regional
             separation of the several inputs contacting the same cell.
             For several reasons, however, this inference must be
             regarded as tentative. Taken together, these findings
             provide a possible explanation of the correlation between
             the dendritic geometry of ganglion cells and the number of
             different axons that innervate them (Purves & Hume, 1981).
             The several axons that initially innervate ganglion cells
             without dendrites evidently compete during early life until
             only a single input remains. On ganglion cells with
             dendrites, however, the number of inputs that persists is
             proportional to dendritic complexity. The present results
             suggest that the diminished competition between axons
             innervating neurones with dendrites may result from some
             degree of terminal segregation on dendritic
             arborizations.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014672},
   Key = {fds268368}
}

@article{fds268373,
   Author = {Lichtman, JW and Purves, D},
   Title = {Activity-mediated neural change.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {301},
   Number = {5901},
   Pages = {563},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/301563a0},
   Doi = {10.1038/301563a0},
   Key = {fds268373}
}

@article{fds268370,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Modulation of neuronal competition by postsynaptic geometry
             in autonomic ganglia},
   Journal = {Trends in Neurosciences},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {C},
   Pages = {10-16},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0166-2236},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(83)90007-3},
   Abstract = {The number of axons that innervate autonomic ganglion cells
             is the result of competition in early life between terminals
             contacting the same target cell. In maturity, a correlation
             between the number of inputs a cell receives and dendritic
             complexity implies that geometry modulates neuronal
             competition. These observations suggest a novel view of the
             role that dendrites play in neuronal development. ©
             1983.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0166-2236(83)90007-3},
   Key = {fds268370}
}

@article{fds268371,
   Author = {Purves, D and Wigston, DJ},
   Title = {Neural units in the superior cervical ganglion of the
             guinea-pig.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {334},
   Pages = {169-178},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014487},
   Abstract = {The size and arrangement of the set of neurones innervated
             by individual preganglionic axons (the neural unit) has been
             investigated in the superior cervical ganglion of the
             guinea-pig. 1. Based on the ratio of preganglionic neurones
             to ganglion cells, and the average number of axons
             contacting each ganglion cell, we estimated that individual
             preganglionic axons innervate on the order of 50-200
             superior cervical ganglion cells. 2. Of 562 pairs of
             ganglion cells examined with intracellular recording,
             forty-seven (8.4%) were innervated by one or more common
             axons. 3. Pairs of ganglion cells innervated by the same
             axon were not necessarily near each other. Although nearby
             cells were more likely to share innervation than neurones
             far apart, cells sharing innervation were often found
             several hundred micrometers apart, and were occasionally
             separated by the largest dimension of the ganglion (about
             1-2 mm). 4. The incidence of cell pairs that shared
             innervation from more than one axon was greater than
             expected from the frequency of pairs sharing at least one
             axon. 5. Extracellular recordings from small fascicles of
             the cervical sympathetic trunk showed that preganglionic
             axons from different segmental levels intermingle
             extensively en route to the superior cervical ganglion. 6.
             Taken together, these findings support the view that sets of
             ganglion cells are innervated in common not because of any
             special topographic relationship within the ganglion, but
             because they share one or more properties that make them
             especially attractive to particular preganglionic
             axons.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014487},
   Key = {fds268371}
}

@article{fds268372,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lichtman, JW},
   Title = {Specific connections between nerve cells.},
   Journal = {Annual Review of Physiology},
   Volume = {45},
   Pages = {553-565},
   Year = {1983},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ph.45.030183.003005},
   Doi = {10.1146/annurev.ph.45.030183.003005},
   Key = {fds268372}
}

@article{fds268367,
   Author = {Hume, RI and Purves, D},
   Title = {Geometry of neonatal neurones and the regulation of synapse
             elimination.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {293},
   Number = {5832},
   Pages = {469-471},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/293469a0},
   Abstract = {In the ciliary ganglion of adult rabbits, ganglion cells
             lacking dendrites are generally innervated by a single axon,
             whereas cells with one or more dendrites are innervated by a
             number of different axons that increases in proportion to
             the complexity of their dendritic arbor. We have now
             explored the basis of this correlation by comparing the
             dendritic arborization of cells receiving different numbers
             of axons during and after the period of synapse elimination
             that occurs early in postnatal life. Our results suggest
             that the geometry of neonatal neurones influences the
             competitive interaction between the several axons that
             initially innervate the same cell. This finding in turn
             implies that an important function of dendrites is to
             regulate the number of different axons that ultimately
             innervate each neurone.},
   Doi = {10.1038/293469a0},
   Key = {fds268367}
}

@article{fds268363,
   Author = {Johnson, DA and Purves, D},
   Title = {Post-natal reduction of neural unit size in the rabbit
             ciliary ganglion.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {318},
   Pages = {143-159},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013855},
   Abstract = {We have studied the innervation of adult and neonatal
             ciliary ganglia in the rabbit to determine the average
             number of ganglion cells innervated by each preganglionic
             neurone at different stages of development. 1. The adult
             ciliary ganglion comprises about 380 ganglion cells which
             are innervated by about forty preganglionic neurones. 2.
             Ciliary ganglion cells in adult rabbits are on average
             innervated by 2.2 different axons; in contrast, neonatal
             ganglion cells are on average innervated by 4.6 different
             axons. The transition to the adult pattern of innervation
             occurs gradually during the first few post-natal weeks. 3.
             The numbers of ganglion cells and preganglionic neurones do
             not change appreciably after birth. Accordingly, the loss of
             some innervation to individual neurones during post-natal
             development indicates that each preganglionic axon
             innervates progressively fewer ciliary ganglion cells. 4.
             The number of synaptic boutons found in ganglia at birth,
             however, is less than the number of synaptic boutons found
             in adult ganglia. 5. We conclude that synaptic connexions in
             this ganglion age gradually rearranged in early post-natal
             life such that each preganglionic neurone focuses an
             increasing number of synaptic contacts on a progressively
             smaller subset of the ganglion cell population.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013855},
   Key = {fds268363}
}

@article{fds268366,
   Author = {Purves, D and Hume, RI},
   Title = {The relation of postsynaptic geometry to the number of
             presynaptic axons that innervate autonomic ganglion
             cells.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the
             Society for Neuroscience},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {441-452},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.01-05-00441.1981},
   Abstract = {We have studied the shape of rabbit ciliary ganglion cells
             in relation to the number of axons that innervate each
             neuron. Adult ganglion cells receive synapses from one to
             seven different preganglionic axons. Some neurons lack
             dendrites altogether, whereas others have complex
             arborizations of up to eight primary dendrites. The neurons
             that receive all of their synaptic contacts from a single
             preganglionic axon usually have no dendrites; on the other
             hand, multiply innervated ganglion cells receive synapses
             from a number of different axons that increases in
             proportion to the number of primary dendrites that they
             possess. A possible explanation of these results is that
             individual ciliary ganglion cells comprise a number of
             separate spatial domains, each of which is largely
             constrained to receive innervation from a single
             preganglionic axon.},
   Doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.01-05-00441.1981},
   Key = {fds268366}
}

@article{fds268365,
   Author = {Purves, D and Thompson, W and Yip, JW},
   Title = {Re-innervation of ganglia transplanted to the neck from
             different levels of the guinea-pig sympathetic
             chain.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {313},
   Pages = {49-63},
   Year = {1981},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013650},
   Abstract = {Thoracic and lumbar sympathetic ganglia from donor
             guinea-pigs were transplanted to the bed of an excised
             superior cervical ganglion in host animals. Homotopic
             transplants of superior cervical ganglia served as controls.
             In this way the same set of preganglionic axons (the
             cervical sympathetic trunk) was confronted with ganglia from
             different levels of the sympathetic chain. Re-innervation of
             the transplants was studied after 3-5 months. 1. Neurones in
             ganglia transplanted from different levels of the
             sympathetic chain were re-innervated to about the same
             over-all degree by the preganglionic axons of the host's
             cervical sympathetic trunk. Thus, the mean amplitude of
             post-synaptic potentials, the estimated number of
             innervating axons, and the number of spinal segments
             providing innervation to each neurone were similar in
             transplanted thoracic, lumbar and superior cervical ganglion
             cells. 2. Neurones in transplanted mid-thoracic ganglia,
             however, were re-innervated more frequently, and more
             strongly, by axons arising from more caudal thoracic
             segments than neurones in transplanted superior cervical
             ganglia. Stimulation of axons arising from the fourth
             thoracic spinal segment (T4), for example, elicited
             post-synaptic potentials that on average were twice as large
             in transplanted fifth thoracic ganglion cells as in
             transplanted superior cervical ganglion cells; conversely,
             axons arising from T1 re-innervated neurones in the superior
             cervical ganglion about 2-3 times more effectively than
             fifth thoracic ganglion cells. This difference in the
             re-innervation of the fifth thoracic and the superior
             cervical ganglion is in the same direction as (although less
             pronounced than) the normal difference in the segmental
             innervation of these ganglia. 3. Transplanted lumbar ganglia
             were also re-innervated more effectively by relatively
             caudal segments compared to re-innervated cervical ganglia,
             but this difference was no greater than that observed for
             transplanted thoracic ganglia. 4. We conclude that
             preganglionic axons can distinguish (or be distinguished by)
             ganglia derived from different levels of the sympathetic
             chain. Our findings are consistent with the view that
             ganglion cells have some permanent property that biases the
             innervation they receive.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013650},
   Key = {fds268365}
}

@article{fds340940,
   Author = {Purves, D and Johnson, DA and Hume, RI},
   Title = {Regulation of synaptic connections in the rabbit ciliary
             ganglion.},
   Journal = {Ciba Foundation Symposium},
   Volume = {83},
   Pages = {232-251},
   Year = {1981},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470720653.ch12},
   Abstract = {One of the intriguing questions about the establishment of
             synaptic connections is how appropriate numbers of different
             axons come to innervate each target neuron. A reorganization
             of connections in early postnatal life appears to be an
             important aspect of this process, since many of the axons
             terminals that initially innervate target cells are
             subsequently lost. The rabbit ciliary ganglion is a
             remarkably simple neural ensemble in which to examine this
             rearrangement of developing synaptic connections. Using this
             system we have found that a reduction in the number of axons
             innervating each cell occurs without any change in the
             number of ciliary ganglion cells or preganglionic neurons;
             therefore the rearrangement is not based on cell death. The
             number of different axons that ultimately innervate each
             cell is, however, influenced in some way by the geometry of
             individual target neurons. Thus, mature ganglion cells that
             lack dendrites are generally innervated by a single axon,
             while neurons with increasingly complex dendritic arbors
             receive innervation from a commensurate number of different
             axons. At birth, on the other hand, neurons with or without
             dendritic processes receive about the same number of
             preganglionic inputs. These results suggest that the
             geometry of the target cell influences the competitive
             interaction between different axons innervating the same
             neuron. Indeed, an important function of dendrites may be to
             regulate the number of axons that innervate each nerve
             cell.},
   Doi = {10.1002/9780470720653.ch12},
   Key = {fds340940}
}

@article{fds268361,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lichtman, JW},
   Title = {Elimination of synapses in the developing nervous
             system.},
   Journal = {Science (New York, N.Y.)},
   Volume = {210},
   Number = {4466},
   Pages = {153-157},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0036-8075},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7414326},
   Abstract = {Reduction of the number of axons that contact target cells
             may be a general feature of neural development. This process
             may underlie the progressively restricted malleability of
             the maturing nervous system.},
   Doi = {10.1126/science.7414326},
   Key = {fds268361}
}

@article{fds268362,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Neuronal competition.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {287},
   Number = {5783},
   Pages = {585-586},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0028-0836},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/287585a0},
   Doi = {10.1038/287585a0},
   Key = {fds268362}
}

@article{fds268359,
   Author = {Rubin, E and Purves, D},
   Title = {Segmental organization of sympathetic preganglionic neurons
             in the mammalian spinal cord.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Comparative Neurology},
   Volume = {192},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {163-174},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.901920111},
   Abstract = {We have used retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase
             to determine the distribution of the preganglionic cell
             bodies whose axons join particular rami of the thoracic
             spinal cord in a series of guinea pigs, and in a small
             number of hamsters and cats. In contrast to other recent
             studies, our results show that the neurons sending axons to
             a ramus are confined to a single segment at the
             corresponding spinal level. This segmental organization
             supports the idea that the rostro-caudal position of
             preganglionic cell bodies is one determinant of selective
             synapse formation between preganglionic axons and
             sympathetic ganglion cells.},
   Doi = {10.1002/cne.901920111},
   Key = {fds268359}
}

@article{fds268360,
   Author = {Lichtman, JW and Purves, D},
   Title = {The elimination of redundant preganglionic innervation to
             hamster sympathetic ganglion cells in early post-natal
             life.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {301},
   Pages = {213-228},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013200},
   Abstract = {The superior cervical ganglion of adult and neonated
             hamsters has been studied with intracellular recording. 1.
             Neurones in adult hamster ganglia are innervated by an
             average of 6-7 preganglionic axons. During the first week of
             post-natal life, however, these cells are innervated by at
             least eleven to twelve axons. Ganglion cells in animals 2-3
             weeks old are innervated to an intermediate degree,
             indicating that these neurones lose a substantial portion of
             their initial synaptic contacts during the first weeks after
             birth. 2. The over-all innervation of the superior cervical
             ganglion in adult hamsters arises from thoracic segments
             T1-T5; no additional segments contribute significantly to
             the innervation of neonatal ganglia. 3. The average number
             of segments innervating each adult ganglion cell is 2 . 8
             compared to 3 . 7 segments innervating neonatal neurones.
             Throughout post-natal development the innervation of
             individual neurones arises from a contiguous subset of the
             spinal segments that innervate the ganglion as a whole. 4.
             We conclude that the elimination of redundant innervatin in
             early life is not limited to those nerve and muscle cells
             contacted by a sigle axon in maturity, but also occurs in
             sympathetic ganglia where adult neurones remain multiply
             innervated. Moreover, the loss of some synaptic contacts
             during development refines the selective innervation of
             individual neurones.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013200},
   Key = {fds268360}
}

@article{fds268358,
   Author = {Lichtman, JW and Purves, D and Yip, JW},
   Title = {Innervation of sympathetic neurones in the guinea-pig
             thoracic chain.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {298},
   Pages = {285-299},
   Year = {1980},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013081},
   Abstract = {We have investigated the organization of the guinea-pig
             thoracic chain by studying the innervation of the stellate
             and fifth thoracic sympathetic ganglia with intracellular
             recording. 1. These ganglia receive preganglionic
             innervation from different but overlapping sets of spinal
             cord segments: the stellate ganglion is innervated by
             preganglionic axons from spinal segments more rostral than
             those supplying the fifth thoracic ganglion, but somewhat
             more caudal than those innervating the superior cervical
             ganglion. 2. Individual thoracic ganglion cells are
             innervated by only some of the spinal segments that supply
             each ganglion as a whole. In general, the subset of spinal
             segments innervating a ganglion cell is contiguous; one of
             these segments provides the strongest innervation, with
             progressively weaker innervation arising from spinal levels
             adjacent to the dominant one. This selective pattern of
             innervation is similar to that in the superior cervical
             ganglion (Njå & Purves, 1977 a). 3. Preganglionic axons
             frequently innervate neurones in more than one ganglion. 4.
             Although neurones innervated by the same spinal cord
             segments are found in both the stellate and the fifth
             thoracic ganglion, as well as in the superior cervical, the
             number of ganglion cells receiving innervation from
             particular spinal segments is different in each ganglion.
             Moreover, neurones dominated by the same segment but located
             in different ganglia receive somewhat different average
             innervation from adjacent segments as a function of the
             ganglion in which they reside. 5. These results indicate
             that neurones in the thoracic chain ganglia, as those in the
             superior cervical ganglion, are selectively innervated by
             particular spinal cord segments. We suggest that the
             different average innervation of sympathetic ganglia
             reflects at least two related factors: the selective
             qualities of their constituent neurones, and the
             availability of different preganglionic axons to each
             ganglion.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013081},
   Key = {fds268358}
}

@article{fds268488,
   Author = {Lichtman, JW and Purves, D and Yip, JW},
   Title = {On the purpose of selective innervation of guinea-pig
             superior cervical ganglion cells.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {292},
   Pages = {69-84},
   Year = {1979},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012839},
   Abstract = {Preganglionic axons arising from different levels of the
             mammalian spinal cord make preferential connexions with
             different classes of superior cervical ganglion cells
             (Langley, 1892, 1900; Njå & Purves, 1977a). For example,
             preganglionic axons from the first thoracic segment (T1)
             make relatively strong connexions with ganglion cells
             activating end-organs of the eye; conversely, axons arising
             from T4 selectively innervate ganglion cells projecting to
             the ear. In the present work we have asked whether this
             selectivity reflects the function of the pre- and
             post-synaptic cells, and aspect of their respective
             positions, or some other criterion. 1. End-organs with
             different functions at the same locus (the eye) respond to
             stimulation of the same ventral roots; end-organs of a
             single modality (hairs or blood vessels) at different
             positions, however, tend to be activated by different spinal
             segments. Thus the segmental innervation of ganglion cells
             is correlated with the position rather than the function of
             post-ganglionic targets. 2. The role of target position in
             ganglion cell innervation was examined directly by recording
             from neurones sending axons to different destinations.
             Superior cervical ganglion cells running dorso-medially in a
             spinal nerve receive, on average, innervation from more
             caudal segments than cells projecting ventro-laterally. 3.
             These selective connexions do not depend on intraganglionic
             cell position: neurones located at different points along
             the major axes of the superior cervical ganglion receive, on
             average, the same segmental innervation. In accord with this
             observation, retrogradely labelled neurones innervating a
             particular target such as the eye or ear are widely and
             randomly distributed within a large portion of the ganglion.
             Thus the importance of post-ganglionic target position in
             ganglion cell innervation is not simply a reflexion of
             ganglionic topography. 4. We conclude that one purpose of
             the selective connexions in the superior cervical ganglion
             is to bring together preganglionic axons arising from
             different levels of the spinal cord and ganglion cells whose
             axons innervate particular regions of the superior cervical
             territory.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012839},
   Key = {fds268488}
}

@article{fds268491,
   Author = {Purves, D and Thompson, W},
   Title = {The effects of post-ganglionic axotomy on selective synaptic
             connexions in the superior cervical ganglion of the
             guinea-pig.},
   Journal = {Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {297},
   Pages = {95-110},
   Year = {1979},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp013029},
   Abstract = {Stimulation of preganglionic axons arising from different
             levels of the thoracic spinal cord causes different effects
             on end-organs supplied by the superior cervical ganglion
             (Langley, 1892; Nja & Purves, 1977a; Lichtman, Purves
             & Yip, 1979). For example, stimulation of the first
             thoracic ventral root (T1) causes pupillary dilatation and
             widening of the palpebral fissure; stimulation of T4, on the
             other hand, has little effect on the eye, even though axons
             arising from this level innervate about as many superior
             cervical ganglion cells as those from T1. Thus ganglion cell
             innervation is selective. (1) Three months after crushing
             the major post-ganglionic branches of the superior cervical
             ganglion this differential effectiveness is lost: T1 and T4
             stimulation have approximately equal effects on the
             end-organs of the eye. (2) In normal animals, the cellular
             counterpart of selective end-organ effects is the
             innervation of each ganglion cell by a contiguous subset of
             the spinal segments that innervate the ganglion as a whole.
             One of these segments is usually dominant, the strength of
             innervation from adjacent segments falling off as a function
             of distance from the dominant one (Nja & Purves, 1977a).
             Intracellular recordings from ganglion cells 3 months after
             post-ganglionic axotomy showed that this selective pattern
             is re-established. (3) Since the innervation of ganglion
             cells appears normal, the abnormal end-organ responses after
             post-ganglionic axotomy suggest that ganglion cell axons are
             not limited to their original targets during peripheral
             re-innervation. This suggestion is supported by the finding
             that ganglion cells sending axons to different peripheral
             destinations via the second and third cervical spinal nerves
             were no longer distinguishable on the basis of their
             segmented inputs 3 months after post-ganglionic axotomy. (4)
             Similar results were obtained when the preganglionic
             cervical trunk was cut at the same time as the
             post-ganglionic axons were crushed; the pattern of end-organ
             responses was abnormal, whereas individual ganglion cells
             were re-innervated according to the rules of contiguity and
             segmental dominance. (5) These results indicate that
             ganglion cells do not undergo a compensatory change in the
             segmental innervation they receive when their axons
             regenerate to targets different from, or in addition to
             those they originally innervated, even when an entirely new
             set of ganglionic connexions is formed. This suggests that
             ganglion cells, or some aspect of their immediate
             environment, possess a permanent label that determines the
             segmental innervation they receive.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp013029},
   Key = {fds268491}
}

@article{fds268489,
   Author = {Purves, D and Lichtman, JW},
   Title = {Formation and maintenance of synaptic connections in
             autonomic ganglia.},
   Journal = {Physiological Reviews},
   Volume = {58},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {821-862},
   Year = {1978},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/physrev.1978.58.4.821},
   Abstract = {The purpose is to review results that shed some light on the
             way in which specific patterns of synaptic connections are
             established and maintained in autonomic ganglia and, by
             analogy, perhaps in other parts of the nervous
             system.},
   Doi = {10.1152/physrev.1978.58.4.821},
   Key = {fds268489}
}

@article{fds268490,
   Author = {Njå, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {Specificity of initial synaptic contacts made on guinea-pig
             superior cervical ganglion cells during regeneration of the
             cervical sympathetic trunk.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {281},
   Pages = {45-62},
   Year = {1978},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012408},
   Abstract = {1. Largely appropriate synaptic connexions are formed with
             neurones in the superior cervical ganglion at long intervals
             after interruption of the preganglionic nerve. In the
             present study we have assessed the accuracy of connexions
             during the early stages of re-innervation by observing
             end-organ responses to ventral root stimulation in vivo, and
             by recording intracellularly from ganglion cells during
             ventral root stimulation in isolated preparations. 2.
             Appropriate, but weak, end-organ responses were elicited by
             stimulation of the first and fourth thoracic ventral roots
             (T1 and T4) 15--30 days after freezing the cervical
             sympathetic trunk. 3. Intracellular recordings from ganglion
             cells during stimulation of the ventral roots C8--T7 in
             vitro showed that synaptic contacts are first re-established
             8--11 days after freezing the preganglionic nerve. The
             proportion of re-innervated cells, and the strength of
             innervation of individual neurones, increased rapidly for up
             to about 3 months after nerve injury, but showed little
             change thereafter. Innervation remained weaker than normal
             even after 6 months. 4. Patterns of segmental innervation
             recorded intracellularly during the early stages of
             regeneration were similar to, but more restricted than
             normal. Even 13--19 days after interruption of the
             preganglionic nerve, neurones re-innervated by more than one
             spinal cord segment tended to be innervated by a contiguous
             subset of the spinal segments which contribute innervation
             to the ganglion. The incidence of neurones receiving
             innervation from a discontinuous segmental subset was about
             the same at early and late stages or re-innervation. 5.
             Throughout the course of nerve regeneration, re-innervated
             neurones tended to receive dominant synaptic input from
             axons arising at a particular spinal level, as do normal
             cells, with adjacent segments contributing a synaptic
             influence that diminished as a function of distance from the
             dominant segment. 6. The results of these experiments argue
             against the initial formation of imprecise connexions with
             subsequent retention of appropriate contacts and a loss of
             inappropriate ones. Rather our findings suggest that the
             re-innervation of ganglion cells proceeds by a gradual
             accumulation of synaptic connexions which are, from the
             outset, appropriate.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012408},
   Key = {fds268490}
}

@article{fds268487,
   Author = {Njå, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {The effects of nerve growth factor and its antiserum on
             synapses in the superior cervical ganglion of the
             guinea-pig.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {277},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {53-75},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {1978},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012260},
   Abstract = {1. The effects of nerve growth factor (NGF) and its
             antiserum on synapses in the superior cervical ganglion of
             the guinea-pig have been examined by intracellular recording
             and electron microscopy. 2. Exogenous NGF, supplied locally
             from a silicone rubber pellet implanted near ganglia for 4-7
             days, had little effect on either the function or the number
             of ganglionic synapses. 3. However, the depression of
             synaptic transmission and loss of synaptic contacts on
             ganglion cells which follow post-ganglionic axotomy were
             diminished by about 50% in the presence of exogenous NGF. 4.
             Other post-axotomy changes such as the development of
             subthreshold regenerative responses in neuronal processes,
             the appearance of ultrastructurally abnormal neuronal
             profiles suggesting rapid membrane turnover, and the
             cytoplasmic and nuclear changes characteristic of
             "chromatolysis", were also largely prevented by exogenous
             NGF. 5. Systemic treatment of neonatal and young adult
             guinea-pigs with antiserum to NGF for 4-5 days caused
             depression of intracellularly recorded synaptic responses
             within 5-8 days of the end of antiserum administration.
             Synapse counts in electron microscopical sections from these
             ganglia showed only about half as many contacts as in
             control ganglia from animals receiving normal rabbit serum.
             6. These findings suggest that the loss of synapses from
             sympathetic neurones which follows axotomy results from a
             reduction in the amount of NGF supplied to ganglion cells. A
             corollary is that, among other biological roles, NGF is
             required by peripheral sympathetic neurones to maintain the
             synapses they receive.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012260},
   Key = {fds268487}
}

@article{fds268486,
   Author = {Nja, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {Re-innervation of guinea-pig superior cervical ganglion
             cells by preganglionic fibres arising from different levels
             of the spinal cord.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {272},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {633-651},
   Year = {1977},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp012064},
   Abstract = {1. The ability of preganglionic axons to re-establish their
             normal pattern of synaptic connexions with superior cervical
             ganglion cells has been studied after section of the
             cervical sympathetic trunk.2. In vivo stimulation of the
             last cervical (C8) and the first seven thoracic ventral
             roots (T1-T7) 3-4 months after section of the trunk produced
             end-organ responses similar to those observed in normal
             animals.3. The pattern of innervation of individual
             neurones, determined by intracellular recording of synaptic
             potentials 4-9 months after cutting the sympathetic trunk,
             was also similar to that observed in normal neurones. Both
             normal and re-innervated ganglion cells were contacted by
             pre-ganglionic axons arising from C8 to T7, and each neurone
             was usually innervated by a contiguous subset of these
             segments.4. Re-innervated neurones, as normal cells, were
             typically dominated by the innervation from a particular
             spinal cord segment, with the adjacent segments contributing
             a synaptic influence that decreased as a function of
             distance from the dominant segment. This was true whether
             the amplitude of the post-synaptic potential, or the
             estimated number of contributing axons, was used as the
             criterion of segmental dominance.5. Re-innervated neurones,
             however, showed some abnormalities. The average number of
             ventral roots contributing innervation to each neurone was
             reduced from 4.1 to 3.0, and discontinuities in the sequence
             of innervating segments were more frequent than in normal
             neurones. Moreover, fewer preganglionic axons contacted each
             neurone after regeneration.6. A further difference between
             normal and re-innervated neurones during the period covered
             by these experiments was that axons from the more caudal
             spinal cord segments were less successful in re-establishing
             contacts with ganglion cells than those from the rostral
             segments. The more caudal the position of the preganglionic
             neurones, the more pronounced was this relative
             deficiency.7. Although anomalies of ganglion cell
             innervation were apparent, the basis for the restoration of
             normal functional effects appears to be the re-establishment
             of a pattern of innervation of individual neurones similar
             to that observed in normal ganglia.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp012064},
   Key = {fds268486}
}

@article{fds268485,
   Author = {Njå, A and Purves, D},
   Title = {Specific innervation of guinea-pig superior cervical
             ganglion cells by preganglionic fibres arising from
             different levels of the spinal cord.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {264},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {565-583},
   Year = {1977},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011683},
   Abstract = {1. The synaptic contribution of preganglionic nerve fibres
             arising from the last cervical (C8) and the first seven
             thoracic spinal cord segments (T1-T7) to neurones of the
             guinea-pig superior cervical ganglion has been studied by
             means of intracellular recording during ventral root
             stimulation in vitro. 2. The majority of neurones received
             innervation from the middle segments (T2 and T3) of the
             length of spinal cord from which preganglionic fibres
             derive; an intermediate number of ganglion cells were
             innervated by fibres from the segments adjacent to these
             (T1, T4, and T5), and relatively few neurones by fibres from
             the most rostral and caudal segments supplying innervation
             to the ganglion (C8, T6 and T7). 3. Each neurone received
             preganglionic terminals from multiple thoracic segments
             (range 1-7, mean = 4-0). The estimated minimum number of
             preganglionic fibres contacting each neurone was 10, on
             average. 4. As a rule, the spinal segments innervating a
             neurone were contiguous. Thus we rarely encountered neurones
             innervated by segments located both rostrally and caudally
             to a segment which failed to provide innervation. 5.
             Neurones tended to be innervated predominantly by axons
             arising from a single spinal segment, with adjacent segments
             contributing a synaptic influence that diminished as a
             function of their distance from the dominant segment. All
             segments provided dominant innervation to at least some
             neurones. 6. Stimulating the ventral roots of C8-T7 in vivo
             showed that the axons arising from each segment produced a
             characteristic pattern of peripheral effects. Thus different
             populations of neurones in the superior cervical ganglion of
             the guinea-pig are innervated by preganglionic axons from
             different levels of the spinal cord, as originally suggested
             by Langley (1892) for the cat, dog, and rabbit. 7. On the
             basis of our in vitro studies we conclude that underlying
             the specificity of innervation of neurones of the superior
             cervical ganglion that can be inferred from in vivo
             experiments is a tendency for individual neurones to be
             innervated in a systematically graded fashion by a
             contiguous subset of the eight spinal segments which provide
             innervation to the ganglion.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011683},
   Key = {fds268485}
}

@article{fds268482,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Competitive and non-competitive re-innervation of mammalian
             sympathetic neurones by native and foreign
             fibres.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {261},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {453-475},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011568},
   Abstract = {The ability of native (sympathetic preganglionic) and
             foreign (vagal) nerve fibres to re-innervate neurones of the
             guinea-pig superior cervical ganglion, either alone or in
             competition with each other, has been studied by means of
             intracellular recording and electron microscopy. 1. Native
             fibres make synaptic contacts with nearly all ganglion cells
             within one month of cervical trunk section; within 6 months
             the degree of innervation, judged by measurement of
             excitatory post-synaptic potential (e.p.s.p.) amplitude and
             electron microscopical synapse counts, approaches normal.
             However, even after 15 months innervation was weaker than in
             normal control ganglia. 2. Vagal fibres are less successful
             during re-innervation. Although a similar number of foreign
             fibres grown into denervated ganglia and make contact with
             nearly all ganglion cells within a month, after 6-12 months
             e.p.s.p. amplitudes in response to foreign nerve stimulation
             remain relatively small, and counts of synapses are only
             about 60% as great as in ganglia re-innervated with the
             native nerve. 3. When both native and foreign fibres are
             allowed to re-innervate ganglion cells simultaneously, about
             half the neurones in the ganglion receive synapses from both
             sources after 1 month. The proportion of dually invervated
             cells remains roughly constant for at least 14 months.
             Neither set of preganglionic fibres dominates or displaces
             the other, although neurones generally are re-innervated
             more effectively by native than foreign fibres, as is true
             during non-competitive re-innervation. 4. Thus during
             re-innervation of mammalian sympathetic neurones native
             fibres are preferred to foreign ones only in the sense that
             roughly the same number of native fibres form many more
             synapses on ganglion cells than do vagal axons. A foreign
             synapse, once formed, is as stable as a native one, and
             shows no tendency to be replaced by native terminals. These
             findings are discussed in relation to other evidence which
             has suggested specificity and selectivity during
             re-innervation of mammalian autonomic neurones.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011568},
   Key = {fds268482}
}

@article{fds268481,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Functional and structural changes in mammalian sympathetic
             neurones following colchicine application to post-ganglionic
             nerves.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {259},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {159-175},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011459},
   Abstract = {1. The effects of post-ganglionic colchicine application on
             neurones of the guinea-pig superior cervical ganglion were
             studied with intracellular recording and electron
             microscopy. 2. Local colchicine application for 30 min to
             one of the major post-ganglionic nerves caused several
             electrophysiological changes after 4-7 days in many neurones
             whose axons run in this nerve. These changes include: (a) a
             reduction in the amplitude of synaptic potentials elicited
             by supramaximal preganglionic stimulation; (b) a decrease in
             the number of preganglionic fibres innervating individual
             neurones; (c) the development of regenerative responses in
             dendrites; and (d) the failure of antidromic action
             potentials to fully invade the neuronal soma. These
             functional changes occurred in the absence of impaired
             impulse conduction or axon degeneration, and were not
             observed in nearby neurones whose axons ran in an untreated
             post-ganglionic nerve. The effects of colchicine are similar
             to the changes produced by axotomy. 3. Counts of synapses in
             thin sections from the region of the ganglion where the
             affected neurones were located showed a reduction, compared
             to the number of synapses in other regions of the colchicine
             treated ganglia, or normal control ganglia. This finding
             indicates that synaptic depression after colchicine
             treatment, like that after axotomy, is due primarily to a
             loss of synaptic contacts from the dendrites of affected
             nerve cells. Unusual profiles containing numerous vesicular
             and tubular organelles frequently seen after interruption of
             the axons were also observed in thin sections after
             colchicine treatment. 4. The similarity of the
             electrophysiological and ultrastructural effects of
             colchicine treatment and axon interruption offers further
             support for the view that synaptic contacts on sympathetic
             neurones are normally regulated by an interaction of the
             neuronal soma with its axonal extension to the
             periphery.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011459},
   Key = {fds268481}
}

@article{fds268484,
   Author = {Purves, D and Njå, A},
   Title = {Effect of nerve growth factor on synaptic depression after
             axotomy.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {260},
   Number = {5551},
   Pages = {535-536},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/260535a0},
   Abstract = {The authors report that exogenous NGF can, to a large
             extent, prevent the synaptic depression seen in adult
             sympathetic ganglion cells after interruption of their
             axons. Ten adult guinea pigs were used.},
   Doi = {10.1038/260535a0},
   Key = {fds268484}
}

@article{fds268477,
   Author = {Roper, S and Purves, D and McMahan, UJ},
   Title = {Synaptic organization and acetylcholine sensitivity of
             multiply innervated autonomic ganglion cells.},
   Journal = {Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative
             Biology},
   Volume = {40},
   Pages = {283-295},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/sqb.1976.040.01.029},
   Abstract = {The principal cells of the mudpuppy cardiac ganglion receive
             synapses from three sources: vagal axons, interneurons and
             axon collaterals from other principal cells. The simplicity
             of the structural organization and the visual clarity in the
             living preparation provide favorable conditions for
             examining the function of these synapses and how different
             classes of synapses on the same cell influence its function.
             We have studied the sensitivity of the principal cells to
             iontophoretically applied acetylcholine--the transmitter at
             synapses made by the vagal axons and by postganglionic axon
             collaterals from other principal cells. In normal ganglia,
             the ACh sensitivity on the cell surface is highest at the
             region of synapses. Partial denervation, produced by
             severing the vagus nerves, results in an increased ACh
             sensitivity in nonsynaptic areas but does not appear to
             affect synaptic transmission at the remaining
             synapses.},
   Doi = {10.1101/sqb.1976.040.01.029},
   Key = {fds268477}
}

@article{fds268479,
   Author = {McMahan, UJ and Purves, D},
   Title = {Visual identification of two kinds of nerve cells and their
             synaptic contacts in a living autonomic ganglion of the
             mudpuppy (Necturus maculosus).},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {254},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {405-425},
   Year = {1976},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011238},
   Abstract = {1. Many of the nerve cells comprising the cardiac
             parasympathetic ganglion of the mudpuppy are spread out in a
             thin, transparent sheet of tissue, enabling one to see
             cellular details in living preparations with differential
             interference contrast optics. The aim of this study was
             twofold: to establish the morphology of the nerve cells and
             their synaptic connections by light and electron microscopy,
             and to determine which aspects of the ganglion's structure
             could be reliably identified in the living tissue. 2. There
             are two types of neurones in the ganglion: (a) principal
             cells that send post-ganglionic axons to cardiac muscle
             fibres, and (b) interneurones whose processes are confined
             to the ganglion. 3. Interneurones are distinguished from
             principal cells by the presence of numerous granular
             vesicles seen with the electron microscope, and by intense
             formaldehyde-induced fluorescence. The interneurones are
             thus similar to catecholamine-containing interneurones in
             autonomic ganglia of other vertebrates. 4. Principal cells
             are innervated by processes that terminate mainly on the
             cell body, forming up to forty-five synaptic boutons and
             covering, on the average, 5% of the perikaryal surface. The
             synaptic terminals are derived from three sources: (a) axons
             from the vagus nerves, (b) interneurones and (c) other
             principal cells. Vagal terminals contacting principal cells
             contain agranular vesicles typical of preganglionic
             cholinergic endings. At regions of contact between processes
             of interneurones and principal cells, the interneurones have
             granular vesicles focused at membrane specializations; in
             addition there are small areas of close plasma membrane
             apposition, probably gap junctions. Some of the contacts
             between principal cells are characterized by gap junctions;
             others are structurally similar to vagal endings but persist
             after vagal degeneration. 6. Interneurones are innervated by
             axons that make contact mainly with their processes. The
             axon terminals on processes of interneurones contain
             agranular vesicles similar to vagal terminals on principal
             cells. 7. In live preparations principal cells are
             distinguished from interneurones by their size and the
             appearance of their organelles. Synaptic contacts on
             principal cells could often be identified and, in some
             cases, large contacts from interneurones or those from other
             nearby principal cells could be traced back to their cell
             bodies of origin. The validity of these identifications was
             confirmed by subsequent electron microscopic examination of
             the same cells.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011238},
   Key = {fds268479}
}

@article{fds268478,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Functional and structural changes in mammalian sympathetic
             neurones following interruption of their
             axons.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {252},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {429-463},
   Year = {1975},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011151},
   Abstract = {The effects of interrupting the axons of principal neurones
             in the superior cervical ganglion of adult guinea-pigs were
             studied by means of intracellular recording, and light and
             electron microscopy. 1. Within 72 hr of axon interruption,
             the amplitude of exitatory postsynaptic potentials
             potentials (e.p.s.p.s) recorded in principal neurons in
             response to maximal preganglionic stimulation declined.
             E.p.s.p.s were maximally reduced (by more than 70% on
             average) 4-7 days following interruption, and failed to
             bring many cells to threshold. E.p.s.p.s. recorded in nearby
             neurones whose axons remained intact were unaffected. 2. In
             ganglia in which axon interruption was achieved by means of
             nerve crush (thus allowing prompt regeneration), mean
             e.p.s.p. amplitudes began to increase again after about 1-2
             weeks. One month after the initial injury many neurones had
             e.p.s.p.s of normal amplitude, and by 2 months affected
             neurones were indistinguishable from control cells.
             Functional peripheral connexions were re-established during
             the period of synaptic recovery. 3. The mean number of
             synapses identified electron microscopically in ganglia in
             which all the major efferent branches had been crushed
             decreased by 65-70% in parallel with synaptic depression
             measured by intracellular recording. However synapse counts
             did not return to normal levels even after 3 months. 4.
             During the period of maximum synaptic depression, numerous
             abnormal profiles which contained accumulations of vesicular
             and tubular organelles, vesicles, and mitochondria were
             observed in electron microscopic sections. Injection of
             horseradish peroxidase into affected neurones demonstrated
             dendritic swelling which probably correspond to these
             profiles. 5. Little or no difference was found in the
             electrical properties of normal neurones and neurones whose
             axons had been interrupted 4-7 days previously. However, the
             mean amplitude of spontaneously occurring synaptic
             potentials was reduced, and the amplitude distribution was
             shifted. This abnormality of the synapses which remain on
             affected neurones also contributes to synaptic depression.
             6. Counts of neurones in normal and experimental ganglia
             showed that approximately half the principal cells died 1-5
             weeks after crushing the major efferent brances. This
             finding presumably explains the failure of synapse counts to
             return to control levels after recovery. 7. If axons were
             prevented from growing back to their target organ by chronic
             ligation, surviving neurones whose axons were enclosed by
             the ligature did not generally recover normal synaptic
             function. Following ligation, most affected cells died
             within a month. 8. Thus the integrity of a principal cell's
             axon is necessary for the maintenance of preganglionic
             synaptic contacts, and ultimately for neuronal survival. The
             basis of neuronal recovery from the effects of axon
             interruption appears to be some aspect of regeneration to
             the peripheral target.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011151},
   Key = {fds268478}
}

@article{fds268480,
   Author = {Purves, D},
   Title = {Persistent innervation of mammalian sympathetic neurones by
             native and foreign fibres.},
   Journal = {Nature},
   Volume = {256},
   Number = {5518},
   Pages = {589-590},
   Year = {1975},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/256589a0},
   Doi = {10.1038/256589a0},
   Key = {fds268480}
}

@article{fds268483,
   Author = {Roper, S and Purves, D and McMahan, UJ},
   Title = {Synaptic organization and acetylcholine sensitivity of
             multiply innervated autonomic ganglion cells},
   Journal = {Symposia on Quantitative Biology},
   Volume = {Vol. 40},
   Pages = {283-295},
   Year = {1975},
   Month = {January},
   Abstract = {The principal cells of the mudpuppy cardiac ganglion receive
             synapses from three sources: vagal axons, interneurons and
             axon collaterals from other principal cells. The simplicity
             of the structural organization and the visual clarity in the
             living preparation provide favorable conditions for
             examining the function of these synapses and how different
             classes of synapses on the same cell influence its function.
             We have studied the sensitivity of the principal cells to
             iontophoretically applied acetylcholine, the transmitter at
             synapses made by the vagal axons and by postganglionic axon
             collaterals from other principal cells. In normal ganglia,
             the ACh sensitivity on the cell surface is highest at the
             region of synapses. Partial denervation, produced by
             severing the vagus nerves, results in an increased ACh
             sensitivity in monosynaptic areas but does not appear to
             affect synaptic transmission at the remaining
             synapses.},
   Key = {fds268483}
}

@article{fds268357,
   Author = {Purves, D and Sakmann, B},
   Title = {Membrane properties underlying spontaneous activity of
             denervated muscle fibres.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {239},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {125-153},
   Year = {1974},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0022-3751},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010559},
   Abstract = {We have examined the events underlying the initiation of
             spontaneous action potentials (fibrillation) in fibres of
             previously denervated rat diaphragm maintained in organ
             culture for up to 10 days.1. Based on discharge pattern, two
             classes of spontaneously active fibres were found:
             rhythmically discharging fibres, and fibres in which action
             potentials occur at irregular intervals.2. Sites of action
             potentials initiation were located by exploration along the
             fibre length with two independent extracellular recording
             electrodes. The majority of sites of origin in both regular
             and irregular fibres were at the former end-plate zone;
             however, there was no region along the length that could
             not, at least in some fibres, be a site of origin.3.
             Intracellular recording at or near sites of origin of action
             potential discharge showed two types of initiating events.
             Irregularly discharging fibres were brought to threshold by
             discrete depolarizations of up to 15 mV in amplitude, while
             regularly occurring action potentials were associated with
             oscillations of the membrane potential.4. Discrete
             depolarizations (called fibrillatory origin potentials or
             f.o.p.s) at sites of origin in irregularly discharging
             fibres have the following properties: (a) random occurrence
             and nearly constant amplitude outside a refractory period
             during which both amplitude and probability of a second
             f.o.p. are reduced; (b) associated inward current flow which
             is localized to about 100 mum or less along the fibre
             length, and (c) dependence of amplitude and frequency on
             membrane potential.5. Oscillation of membrane potential
             found at sites of origin of action potential discharge in
             regular fibres also occurred locally along the fibre length
             and was sensitive to changes in membrane potential.6. Both
             f.o.p.s and oscillations of membrane potential were
             reversibly abolished by low Na(+)-Ringer fluid or
             tetrodotoxin.7. Neither type of initiating event was
             appreciably affected by concentrations of D-tubocurarine
             which blocked extrajunctional sensitivity to
             acetylcholine.8. We conclude that spontaneous action
             potentials under these conditions arise from a localized
             Na(+)-conductance change in the membrane of the active
             fibre; this conductance change is distinct from the
             increased Na(+)-conductance which follows the interaction of
             acetylcholine with its receptor. Spontaneous activity in
             single, denervated muscle fibres is cyclical and
             self-inhibiting (Purves & Sakmann, 1974); thus the
             Na(+)-conductance change underlying the initiation of
             spontaneous action potentials is affected by muscle fibre
             activity.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010559},
   Key = {fds268357}
}

@article{fds268356,
   Author = {Purves, D and Sakmann, B},
   Title = {The effect of contractile activity on fibrillation and
             extrajunctional acetylcholine-sensitivity in rat muscle
             maintained in organ culture.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {237},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {157-182},
   Year = {1974},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0022-3751},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010475},
   Abstract = {1. The effect of contractile activity on the initiation of
             spontaneous action potentials (fibrillation) and on
             extrajunctional acetylcholine-sensitivity has been studied
             in single fibres in strips of previously denervated rat
             diaphragm maintained in organ culture for up to 10 days.2.
             Following removal of the diaphragm from the animal,
             fibrillation slowed and usually stopped altogether for about
             24-36 hr. Thereafter, spontaneously active fibres were found
             in all cultured muscle strips.3. At any one time, about
             (1/4) to (1/3) of fibres impaled with micro-electrodes were
             active (defined as more than one action potential/10 sec),
             with a mean discharge frequency of 4.5/sec (range
             0.1-24/sec).4. The duration of continuous activity in single
             fibres was, on average, 21-22 hr; a period of activity was
             followed by a longer inactive interval. Thus activity in
             single fibres is cyclical.5. Direct stimulation of
             fibrillating strips for 24 hr at 10/sec suppressed
             spontaneous activity for 1-3 days.6. Conversely, blockade of
             spontaneous activity with tetrodotoxin for 72 hr led to a
             two- to threefold increase in the number of fibrillating
             fibres when the drug was washed out; in some strips nearly
             all fibres became spontaneously active.7. The mean rate of
             activity of diaphragm fibres during normal breathing,
             determined by recording single units from the phrenic nerve
             in lightly anaesthetized animals, is about 18/sec.8. Direct
             stimulation of cultured diaphragm strips in a pattern
             similar to breathing for 7-8 days at an average rate of
             10-12/sec (or 5/sec in some experiments), resulted in a
             marked reduction (about 95% in experiments at 10/sec) in
             extrajunctional sensitivity to ionophoretically applied
             ACh.9. Direct stimulation for 24 hr at 10/sec (comparable to
             a period of spontaneous activity) caused only a small
             reduction in extrajunctional ACh-sensitivity.10. We conclude
             that spontaneous activity in single fibres under these
             conditions occurs cyclically because activity, over a period
             of hours, inhibits the ability of the fibrillating fibre to
             initiate further action potentials. Repeated self-inhibition
             of spontaneous activity probably explains why denervated
             muscle fibres remain highly sensitive to extrajunctionally
             applied ACh.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010475},
   Key = {fds268356}
}

@article{fds268355,
   Author = {Purves, D and McMahan, UJ},
   Title = {The distribution of synapses on a physiologically identified
             motor neuron in the central nervous system of the leech. An
             electron microscope study after the injection of the
             fluorescent dye procion yellow.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Cell Biology},
   Volume = {55},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {205-220},
   Year = {1972},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1083/jcb.55.1.205},
   Abstract = {The fine structure of a physiologically identified motor
             neuron in the segmental ganglion of the leech central
             nervous system and the morphology of synapses on it were
             studied after injection of the fluorescent dye Procion
             yellow as a marker. The injected cell and its processes
             within the neuropil were located in thick or thin sections
             with fluorescence optics after initial fixation with
             glutaraldehyde and brief treatment with osmium tetroxide.
             The same or adjacent thin sections could then be examined in
             the electron microscope. Comparison with uninjected cells
             showed that the general features of the injected cell are
             retained although some organelles are distorted. The main
             features of the geometry of this neuron are the same from
             animal to animal: a single large process runs from the soma
             through the neuropil to bifurcate and enter the
             contralateral roots. Within the neuropil the main process
             gives off long branches (up to 150 micro), but these are
             greatly outnumbered by short branches and spines, one or a
             few microns in length, which were not appreciated in
             previous light microscope studies after injection of Procion
             yellow. Serial thin sections of selected areas along the
             main process within the neuropil showed that there are
             synapses on most of the shorter branches and spines;
             occasional synaptic contacts were also made on the main
             process itself and on longer branches. At least two
             morphologically distinct types of synapse could be
             recognized. A minimum estimate of the total number of
             synapses on the motor cell is 300, based on their occurrence
             in reconstructed segments.},
   Doi = {10.1083/jcb.55.1.205},
   Key = {fds268355}
}

@article{fds268354,
   Author = {Nicholls, JG and Purves, D},
   Title = {A comparison of chemical and electrical synaptic
             transmission between single sensory cells and a motoneurone
             in the central nervous system of the leech.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {225},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {637-656},
   Year = {1972},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0022-3751},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp009961},
   Abstract = {In leech ganglia, three sensory cells of different modality
             converge on a motoneurone, where they form chemical and
             electrical synapses. Each of these synapses behaves in a
             characteristic manner and the nature of the transmission
             mechanism has significant functional consequences for the
             operation of the reflexes. An analysis has been made of the
             effects of trains of impulses on synaptic transmission
             through these pathways, using frequencies that correspond to
             natural firing.1. At the chemical synapse between the
             nociceptive sensory cell and the motoneurone, two opposing
             events occur: facilitation and depression. Thus, with trains
             of impulses, the synaptic potentials first increase in
             amplitude and then decrease. The two processes could be
             separated by altering the Mg and Ca content of the bathing
             fluid. In concentrations of Mg that reduced the amplitude of
             a single control chemical synaptic potential, pure
             facilitation occurred during a train. Depression
             predominated during brief trains in raised concentrations of
             Ca, although synaptic potentials were initially larger.
             These results suggest that changes in the amount of
             transmitter released by each presynaptic action potential
             can account for the changes observed in chemical synaptic
             transmission.2. In contrast, electrical transmission between
             the sensory cell responding to touch and the same
             motoneurone did not show facilitation or depression. The
             electrical coupling potential in the motoneurone was
             relatively constant when the touch cell fired at high or low
             frequencies in normal Ringer fluid, high Mg, or high Ca
             fluid.3. Further differences between chemical and electrical
             synapses were apparent when the preparation was cooled to 4
             degrees C. In the cold the latency of chemically evoked
             synaptic potentials in the motoneurone increased and their
             amplitude declined drastically with repetitive stimulation,
             while electrical coupling potentials were unaffected.4. A
             brief hyperpolarization of the presynaptic cell by injected
             current produced a marked and prolonged increase in
             chemically evoked synaptic potentials, but did not influence
             electrical synaptic transmission.5. The synapses of the
             sensory cell responding to pressure, which are both chemical
             and electrical, behaved as expected: the chemical synaptic
             potentials showed facilitation and depression while
             electrical transmission remained relatively constant.6.
             These experiments emphasize the different functional
             consequences of electrical or chemical synapses in reflex
             pathways for the transmission of signals that arise as a
             result of natural sensory stimuli.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp009961},
   Key = {fds268354}
}

@article{fds268475,
   Author = {McMahan, UJ and Purves, D},
   Title = {An electron-microscopic study of a physiologically
             identified motoneurone in the leech C.N.S. after injection
             of the fluorescent dye Procion yellow.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {222},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {64P-66P},
   Year = {1972},
   Month = {April},
   Key = {fds268475}
}

@article{fds268476,
   Author = {Nicholls, JG and Purves, D},
   Title = {Monosynaptic chemical and electrical connexions between
             sensory and motor cells in the central nervous system of the
             leech.},
   Journal = {The Journal of Physiology},
   Volume = {209},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {647-667},
   Year = {1970},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009184},
   Abstract = {The synaptic connexions that underlie three different
             segmental shortening reflexes have been traced by recording
             intracellularly from individual sensory and motor nerve
             cells in the C.N.S. of the leech. The fourteen sensory cells
             involved in these reflexes respond specifically to one of
             three modalities: touch, pressure, or noxious stimuli
             applied to the skin. All three types of sensory neurone give
             rise to excitatory synaptic potentials in two large
             motoneurones. Each of these motor cells provides excitatory
             innervation to the longitudinal muscle fibres of the
             opposite side of the segment. The mechanism of synaptic
             transmission is, however, different for each type of sensory
             cell.1. An impulse in a sensory cell that responds to touch
             gives rise to a short-latency depolarizing potential in the
             large longitudinal motoneurones by way of an electrical
             synapse. This junction rectifies so that excitation can
             spread in only one direction (from the sensory to the motor
             cell), whereas a hyperpolarizing potential can pass only in
             the opposite direction.2. The synaptic potential evoked in
             the motoneurone by an action potential in a sensory cell
             responding to noxious stimuli can be attributed to the
             action of a chemical transmitter agent and has different
             properties: the post-synaptic potential arises after a delay
             of about 2-4 msec, is abolished by high concentrations of
             Mg, and enhanced by high concentrations of Ca. Several lines
             of evidence show that this connexion is monosynaptic.3. The
             synaptic potential following an impulse in a pressure cell
             is produced by both chemical and electrical synaptic
             mechanisms. Rectification, similar to that described for the
             touch cell, also occurs at this electrical synapse.4. One or
             more impulses in any one of the fourteen mechanoreceptor
             cells in the ganglion can initiate impulses in the large
             longitudinal motoneurones to produce a shortening of the
             segment. The contraction is abolished by blocking impulse
             initiation in the motoneurones.5. The arborizations of the
             sensory cells and the motoneurone within the neuropile have
             been studied histologically after injecting a fluorescent
             dye. Their processes are intertwined in a highly complex
             manner so that the sites of the synaptic junctions cannot be
             determined with the resolutions so far achieved.
             Nevertheless, taken together the histological and the
             electrical results support the idea that individual cells
             are connected in a stereotyped pattern and operate by
             distinctive mechanisms.6. These findings provide a basis for
             studying the functional role of chemical and electrical
             synaptic mechanisms in these pathways.},
   Doi = {10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009184},
   Key = {fds268476}
}


%% Articles and Chapters   
@article{fds323314,
   Author = {Bowling, D and Purves, D},
   Title = {A biological basis for musical tonality},
   Pages = {205-214},
   Booktitle = {Sensory Perception: Mind and Matter},
   Publisher = {Springer Vienna},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9783211997505},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-211-99751-2_12},
   Abstract = {Like other sensory qualities, the human ability to perceive
             tonal sound stimuli has presumably evolved because of its
             utility. Although a variety of tonal sounds are present in
             the human auditory environment, the vocalizations of other
             humans are the most biologically relevant and the most
             frequently experienced. It is thus reasonable to assume that
             our appreciation of tonal sounds has arisen primarily for
             the benefits that accrue from this conspecific information.
             It follows that the structure and function of the tonal
             sounds produced by the human vocal apparatus may provide the
             key to understanding how and why we perceive tonality in
             music the way that we do. Here we consider recent evidence
             that bears on this idea.},
   Doi = {10.1007/978-3-211-99751-2_12},
   Key = {fds323314}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Philosophy * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Reload * Login