Psychology and Neuroscience Faculty Database
Psychology and Neuroscience
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > pn > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Steven R. Asher    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Journal Articles   
@article{fds376111,
   Author = {Cerda-Smith, J and Yust, PKS and Weeks, MS and Asher, SR and Mulvey,
             KL},
   Title = {A Novel Approach for Evaluating a Schoolwide Antiracist
             Curriculum Intervention},
   Journal = {AERA Open},
   Volume = {10},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/23328584231223476},
   Abstract = {This manuscript describes our effort to apply a novel
             approach to understanding student outcomes associated with a
             schoolwide antiracist intervention. We report a multimethod
             quantitative approach to evaluate a 10-week antiracist
             intervention designed and implemented by school staff by
             examining patterns of student intervention engagement and
             measures of key constructs that connect to antiracism,
             psychological well-being, and school connectedness. Our
             novel approach combines schoolwide surveys with smaller
             samples of daily diary participants, documenting variation
             in intervention engagement and examining postintervention
             outcomes. Our findings are limited by high attrition rates,
             small sample size, and data collection during the COVID-19
             pandemic; however, our methods offer a promising
             transferable approach to evaluate school-based antiracist
             interventions by examining patterns and predictors of
             intervention engagement, as well as daily fluctuations in
             student experience throughout the intervention
             period.},
   Doi = {10.1177/23328584231223476},
   Key = {fds376111}
}

@article{fds371504,
   Author = {Yust, PKS and Weeks, MS and Williams, GA and Asher,
             SR},
   Title = {Social relationship provisions and loneliness in school:
             Child- and classroom-level effects.},
   Journal = {Journal of school psychology},
   Volume = {99},
   Pages = {101218},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2023.05.001},
   Abstract = {Building on social needs theory (Weiss, 1974), this study
             introduces the construct of classroom provision richness and
             examines the association between the exchange of social
             provisions among children in classrooms and children's
             feelings of loneliness in school. We examined the receipt of
             provisions from reciprocally nominated friends versus
             unilateral (one-sided) and non-friend classmates and
             examined associations between social provisions and
             loneliness at the child and classroom levels. Participants
             were 998 third- through fifth-grade children (468 girls, 530
             boys; 88.5% White) in 38 classrooms who indicated which
             classmates they played with, helped, validated, and provided
             opportunities for self-disclosure. In addition to the social
             provisions nomination measure, children responded to (a) a
             measure of loneliness that avoided content overlapping with
             social provisions, (b) a rating-scale sociometric measure of
             peer acceptance, and (c) a measure that asked them to
             indicate which classmates engaged in prosocial, aggressive,
             or withdrawn-type behaviors. Multilevel analyses indicated
             that social provisions received from reciprocal friends and
             from unilateral-received friends were associated with
             children's feelings of loneliness in school. Furthermore, a
             measure of the provision richness of classrooms moderated
             the association between child-level provisions received and
             feelings of loneliness, such that children who received
             fewer provisions were less lonely in classrooms that were
             more provision-rich. Classroom provision richness was also
             associated with the general level of prosocial behavior and
             peer acceptance in the classroom. Together, findings suggest
             that efforts to foster the exchange of social provisions in
             classrooms could reduce loneliness and facilitate a more
             caring classroom environment.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jsp.2023.05.001},
   Key = {fds371504}
}

@article{fds372819,
   Author = {Hoyle, RH and Weeks, MS and Stutts, LA and Asher, SR and Leary, MR and Strauman, TJ and Blomquist, KIK and Pontari, BA and Stetler, CA and Terrell, DF},
   Title = {Correction to: The Student Resilience and Well-Being
             Project: Opportunities, Challenges, and Lessons Learned
             (International Journal of Community Well-Being, (2021), 4,
             4, (669-690), 10.1007/s42413-021-00138-2)},
   Journal = {International Journal of Community Well-Being},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {691},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s42413-021-00143-5},
   Abstract = {In this article The Student Resilience and Well-Being
             Project Research Group3 members are (in alphabetical order
             by institution and last name) Lauren A. Stutts (Department
             of Health and Human Values, Davidson College); Steven R.
             Asher, Rick H. Hoyle, Mark R. Leary, Timothy J. Strauman,
             and Molly S. Weeks (Department of Psychology & Neuroscience,
             Duke University); Kerstin K. Blomquist, Beth A. Pontari, and
             Cinnamon A. Stetler (Department of Psychology, Furman
             University); and Debra F. Terrell (Department of Social and
             Behavioral Sciences, Johnson C. Smith University) The
             original article has been updated.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s42413-021-00143-5},
   Key = {fds372819}
}

@article{fds363889,
   Author = {Bagwell, CL and Bowker, JC and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Back to the Dyad: Future Directions for Friendship
             Research},
   Journal = {Merrill-Palmer Quarterly},
   Volume = {67},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {457-484},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   Abstract = {Friendship is a developmentally significant relationship in
             childhood and adolescence that contributes to
             socioemotional, social-cognitive, and psychological
             development and well-being. It is a dyadic relationship
             based on mutual affec-tion, with both friends thinking of
             each other as friends. Despite this definitional
             understanding of the dyadic nature of friendship, it is
             common to study friendships individually, for example, by
             investigating how one child’s perception of the quality of
             a friendship is associated with that child’s psychological
             function-ing. Although this research approach yields
             important information about friendships and their effects on
             youth, we suggest that putting the dyad back into friendship
             research, by conceptualizing the dyad as the unit of
             analysis or by including characteristics or perceptions of
             both members of the dyad in analy-ses, will generate
             valuable new knowledge about friendships and their
             developmental significance. We focus on three key areas of
             study about children’s and adolescents’ friendships that
             would benefit from a dyadic perspective: (a) features and
             processes in friendships, (b) temporal and contextual
             approaches to the study of friendship, and (c) friendship
             tasks and social-cognitive perspectives on
             intervention.},
   Key = {fds363889}
}

@article{fds332880,
   Author = {McDonald, KL and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Pacifists and Revenge-Seekers in Response to Unambiguous
             Peer Provocation.},
   Journal = {Journal of youth and adolescence},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1907-1925},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0767-4},
   Abstract = {In order to better understand why some children retaliate
             when they feel provoked and others do not, the present study
             identified "pacifistically-oriented" children who made
             negative interpretations in response to unambiguous
             provocations, yet did not endorse revenge goals, and
             compared them to "revenge-seeking" children who also made
             negative interpretations but did endorse revenge goals.
             Groups were identified based on seventh graders'
             (N = 367; 54.77% male; 22.89% racial/ethnic minority)
             responses to hypothetical situations in which a peer
             excluded and insulted them. Comparing these groups revealed
             that Pacifists endorsed relationship-maintaining goals and
             emotion regulation goals more highly than Revenge-Seekers.
             Revenge-Seekers reported more anger and endorsed beliefs
             about negative reciprocity and aggression being legitimate
             more highly than Pacifists. Additionally, Revenge-Seekers
             were more disrespect sensitive than were Pacifists, based on
             a measure of vigilance for signs of disrespect and
             expectations that others would disrespect them. Together
             these findings point to social-cognitive and emotion-related
             processes that may inhibit revenge-seeking in unambiguous
             provocation situations, even when children interpret the
             peer's behavior quite negatively.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10964-017-0767-4},
   Key = {fds332880}
}

@article{fds322939,
   Author = {Rose, AJ and Asher, SR},
   Title = {The Social Tasks of Friendship: Do Boys and Girls Excel in
             Different Tasks?},
   Journal = {Child Development Perspectives},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {3-8},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12214},
   Abstract = {Research documents the strengths of girls’ friendships
             compared to boys’ friendships leading to the inference
             that boys are not very skilled as friends. In this article,
             we use a friendship tasks framework to propose that this
             inference is premature and should be reconciled with
             evidence that boys are as satisfied as girls with their
             friendships and that their friendships are as stable over
             time. We also propose that the inference arises partly
             because the friendship tasks that girls handle well have
             been studied extensively, whereas certain friendship tasks
             boys handle as well as or more successfully than girls are
             understudied. These tasks include being a fun and enjoyable
             companion, coping when a friend violates a core expectation
             of friendship, and sustaining friendships in the broader
             social context of a friend having other friends. Finally, we
             suggest that girls and boys who develop skills to respond to
             a range of friendship tasks will benefit in the long
             term.},
   Doi = {10.1111/cdep.12214},
   Key = {fds322939}
}

@article{fds250582,
   Author = {Vanhalst, J and Soenens, B and Luyckx, K and Petegem, SV and Weeks, MS and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Why do the chronically lonely stay lonely? Chronically
             lonely children and adolescents attributions and emotions in
             situations of social inclusion and exclusion.},
   Journal = {Journal of Personality and Social Psychology},
   Volume = {109},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {932-948},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {1939-1315},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000051},
   Abstract = {The goal of this study was to identify mechanisms associated
             with chronic loneliness by examining the effect of
             adolescents' accumulated history of loneliness on responses
             to new social situations. Specifically, this study
             investigated whether attributions and emotions in situations
             of social inclusion and exclusion differ between chronically
             lonely adolescents and adolescents with a different
             loneliness history. A total of 730 adolescents (Mage at Wave
             1 = 15.43 years) participated in a 4-wave longitudinal study
             with annual loneliness assessments. A chronic loneliness
             trajectory was identified, in addition to low-stable,
             moderate-stable, moderate-increasing, and high-decreasing
             loneliness trajectories. At Wave 4, vignettes depicting
             social inclusion and exclusion were presented, and
             participants rated a set of attributions and emotions
             following each vignette. Compared with individuals following
             other trajectories, chronically lonely adolescents were
             characterized by hypersensitivity to social exclusion (i.e.,
             higher levels of negative emotions) and hyposensitivity to
             social inclusion (i.e., lower levels of enthusiasm).
             Further, chronically lonely adolescents had a stronger
             tendency to attribute social inclusion to circumstantial
             factors and social exclusion to internal and stable
             characteristics. This maladaptive attribution style
             partially mediated their emotional experiences. Together,
             results indicate that chronically lonely individuals respond
             to social situations in ways that may perpetuate rather than
             reduce their loneliness.},
   Doi = {10.1037/pspp0000051},
   Key = {fds250582}
}

@article{fds250586,
   Author = {Mcdonald, KL and Asher, SR},
   Title = {College students' revenge goals across friend, romantic
             partner, and roommate contexts: The role of interpretations
             and emotions},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {499-521},
   Year = {2013},
   ISSN = {0961-205X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00650.x},
   Abstract = {Residential college environments provide young people with
             distinctive relationship opportunities and challenges. A
             major purpose of the present study was to learn whether
             college students respond differently to conflict-of-interest
             vignettes in three different relationship contexts. Students
             were more likely to make negative interpretations about
             their romantic partner's behavior than they did about their
             friend's or roommate's behavior. They were also more likely
             to feel angry and hurt and to endorse hostile goals and
             strategies with romantic partners. A second major purpose
             was to learn about the types of interpretations and emotions
             associated with revenge goals in conflict-of-interest
             situations. Results indicated that interpreting the other
             person's actions as disrespectful and as rejecting was
             related to revenge goals and also predicted to revenge goals
             beyond the contributions of anger and hurt feelings. © 2012
             John Wiley & Sons Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00650.x},
   Key = {fds250586}
}

@article{fds290487,
   Author = {Simon, TR and Ikeda, RM and Smith, EP and Reese, LE and Rabiner, DL and Miller, S and Winn, D-M and Dodge, KA and Asher, SR and Horne, AM and Orpinas, P and Martin, R and Quinn, WH and Tolan, PH and Gorman-Smith,
             D and Henry, DB and Gay, FN and Schoeny, M and Farrell, AD and Meyer, AL and Sullivan, TN and Allison, KW and Proj, MVP},
   Title = {Mediators of Effects of a Selective Family-Focused Violence
             Prevention Approach for Middle School Students},
   Journal = {PREVENTION SCIENCE},
   Volume = {13},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-14},
   Publisher = {Springer Nature},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {1389-4986},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000300663600001&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {This study examined how parenting and family characteristics
             targeted in a selective prevention program mediated effects
             on key youth proximal outcomes related to violence
             perpetration. The selective intervention was evaluated
             within the context of a multi-site trial involving random
             assignment of 37 schools to four conditions: a universal
             intervention composed of a student social-cognitive
             curriculum and teacher training, a selective family-focused
             intervention with a subset of high-risk students, a
             condition combining these two interventions, and a
             no-intervention control condition. Two cohorts of
             sixth-grade students (total N = 1,062) exhibiting high
             levels of aggression and social influence were the sample
             for this study. Analyses of pre-post change compared to
             controls using intent-to-treat analyses found no significant
             effects. However, estimates incorporating participation of
             those assigned to the intervention and predicted
             participation among those not assigned revealed significant
             positive effects on student aggression, use of aggressive
             strategies for conflict management, and parental estimation
             of student's valuing of achievement. Findings also indicated
             intervention effects on two targeted family processes:
             discipline practices and family cohesion. Mediation analyses
             found evidence that change in these processes mediated
             effects on some outcomes, notably aggressive behavior and
             valuing of school achievement. Results support the notion
             that changing parenting practices and the quality of family
             relationships can prevent the escalation in aggression and
             maintain positive school engagement for high-risk
             youth.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11121-011-0245-2},
   Key = {fds290487}
}

@article{fds250591,
   Author = {Weeks, MS and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Loneliness in childhood: toward the next generation of
             assessment and research.},
   Journal = {Adv Child Dev Behav},
   Volume = {42},
   Pages = {1-39},
   Booktitle = {Advances in Child Development and Behavior},
   Publisher = {Academic Press},
   Address = {San Diego, CA},
   Editor = {Benson, JB},
   Year = {2012},
   ISSN = {0065-2407},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22675902},
   Abstract = {Loneliness is a sad, even painful emotional experience that
             is thought to result from deficiencies in the quantity or
             quality of one's social relationships. Assessments of
             loneliness have evolved to typically include diverse item
             content that assesses the causes of loneliness as well as
             the emotional experience of loneliness itself. This
             embedding of hypothesized causes in the assessment of
             loneliness creates pervasive problems in drawing valid
             conclusions from empirical research on loneliness. In this
             chapter, we review major historical and contemporary
             perspectives on loneliness and then describe how widely used
             assessments have led to the development of a literature in
             which key questions about the causes of loneliness, the
             influence of gender on the experience of loneliness, and the
             developmental course of loneliness remain unanswered. We
             argue for the use of loneliness assessments that focus
             purely on the emotional experience of loneliness without
             including hypothesized causes.},
   Doi = {10.1016/b978-0-12-394388-0.00001-0},
   Key = {fds250591}
}

@article{fds250592,
   Author = {Rose, AJ and Schwartz-Mette, RA and Smith, RL and Asher, SR and Swenson,
             LP and Carlson, W and Waller, EM},
   Title = {How girls and boys expect disclosure about problems will
             make them feel: implications for friendships},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {844-863},
   Year = {2012},
   ISSN = {0009-3920},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01734.x},
   Abstract = {Although girls disclose to friends about problems more than
             boys, little is known about processes underlying this sex
             difference. Four studies (Ns=526, 567, 769, 154) tested
             whether middle childhood to mid-adolescent girls and boys
             (ranging from 8 to 17 years old) differ in how they expect
             that talking about problems would make them feel. Girls
             endorsed positive expectations (e.g., expecting to feel
             cared for, understood) more strongly than boys. Despite
             common perceptions, boys did not endorse negative
             expectations such as feeling embarrassed or worried about
             being made fun of more than girls. Instead, boys were more
             likely than girls to expect to feel "weird" and like they
             were wasting time. Sex differences in outcome expectations
             did help to account for girls' greater disclosure to
             friends. © 2012 The Authors. Child Development © 2012
             Society for Research in Child Development,
             Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01734.x},
   Key = {fds250592}
}

@article{fds250593,
   Author = {MacEvoy, JP and Asher, SR},
   Title = {When friends disappoint: Boys’ and girls’ responses to
             transgressions of friendship expectations},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {104-119},
   Year = {2012},
   ISSN = {0009-3920},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01685.x},
   Abstract = {In this study, the prevailing view that girls are
             pervasively more skilled in their friendships than boys was
             challenged by examining whether girls respond more
             negatively than boys when a friend violates core friendship
             expectations. Fourth- and fifth-grade children (n = 267)
             responded to vignettes depicting transgressions involving a
             friend's betrayal, unreliability, or failure to provide
             support or help. Results indicated that girls were more
             troubled by the transgressions, more strongly endorsed
             various types of negative relationship interpretations of
             the friend's actions, and reported more anger and sadness
             than did boys. Girls also endorsed revenge goals and
             aggressive strategies just as much as boys. These findings
             lead to a more complex view of boys' and girls' friendship
             competencies.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01685.x},
   Key = {fds250593}
}

@article{fds250585,
   Author = {MacEvoy, JP and Weeks, MS and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Loneliness},
   Volume = {2},
   Pages = {178-187},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of adolescence,},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-373951-3.00116-2},
   Abstract = {This article reviews theory and empirical research on
             loneliness with an emphasis on late childhood through late
             adolescence and early adulthood. The authors first discuss
             theoretical perspectives on loneliness from various
             intellectual traditions - with a particular focus on social
             needs perspectives on loneliness - and then briefly review
             different loneliness assessments that may be used with
             children and adolescents. Next, the authors present an
             overview of the developmental and contextual factors that
             may influence the degree of loneliness that is experienced
             during adolescence. Finally, the authors review research on
             the link between loneliness and peer and family
             relationships, and examine what is known about the
             behavioral and cognitive styles of lonely youth. © 2011
             Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-373951-3.00116-2},
   Key = {fds250585}
}

@article{fds250595,
   Author = {Dodge, TMVPPKA and member},
   Title = {The ecological effects of universal and selective violence
             prevention programs for middle school students: A randomized
             trial},
   Journal = {Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology},
   Volume = {77},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {526.-542.},
   Year = {2009},
   ISSN = {0022-006X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014395},
   Abstract = {This study reports the findings of a multisite randomized
             trial evaluating the separate and combined effects of 2
             school-based approaches to reduce violence among early
             adolescents. A total of 37 schools at 4 sites were
             randomized to 4 conditions: (1) a universal intervention
             that involved implementing a student curriculum and teacher
             training with 6th-grade students and teachers, (2) a
             selective intervention in which a family intervention was
             implemented with a subset of 6th-grade students exhibiting
             high levels of aggression and social influence, (3) a
             combined intervention condition, and (4) a no-intervention
             control condition. Analyses of multiple waves of data from 2
             cohorts of students at each school (N = 5,581) within the
             grade targeted by the interventions revealed a complex
             pattern. There was some evidence to suggest that the
             universal intervention was associated with increases in
             aggression and reductions in victimization; however, these
             effects were moderated by preintervention risk. In contrast,
             the selective intervention was associated with decreases in
             aggression but no changes in victimization. These findings
             have important implications for efforts to develop effective
             violence prevention programs.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0014395},
   Key = {fds250595}
}

@article{fds250594,
   Author = {Dodge, MVPPKA and member},
   Title = {The multisite violence prevention project: Impact of a
             universal school-based violence prevention program on
             social-cognitive outcomes},
   Journal = {Prevention Science},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {231-244},
   Year = {2008},
   ISSN = {1389-4986},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11121-008-0101-1},
   Abstract = {This study evaluated the impact of a universal school-based
             violence prevention program on social-cognitive factors
             associated with aggression and nonviolent behavior in early
             adolescence. The effects of the universal intervention were
             evaluated within the context of a design in which two
             cohorts of students at 37 schools from four sites (N=5,581)
             were randomized to four conditions: (a) a universal
             intervention that involved implementing a student curriculum
             and teacher training with sixth grade students and teachers;
             (b) a selective intervention in which a family intervention
             was implemented with a subset of sixth grade students
             exhibiting high levels of aggression and social influence;
             (c) a combined intervention condition; and (d) a
             no-intervention control condition. Short-term and long-term
             (i.e., 2-year post-intervention) universal intervention
             effects on social-cognitive factors targeted by the
             intervention varied as a function of students'
             pre-intervention level of risk. High-risk students benefited
             from the intervention in terms of decreases in beliefs and
             attitudes supporting aggression, and increases in
             self-efficacy, beliefs and attitudes supporting nonviolent
             behavior. Effects on low-risk students were in the opposite
             direction. The differential pattern of intervention effects
             for low- and high-risk students may account for the absence
             of main effects in many previous evaluations of universal
             interventions for middle school youth. These findings have
             important research and policy implications for efforts to
             develop effective violence prevention programs. © 2008
             Society for Prevention Research.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11121-008-0101-1},
   Key = {fds250594}
}

@article{fds250604,
   Author = {Troop Gordon and W and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Modifications in children's goals when encountering
             obstacles to conflict resolution},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {76},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {568-582},
   Year = {2005},
   ISSN = {0009-3920},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00864.x},
   Abstract = {Previous studies have demonstrated that children's goals are
             associated with their success in peer relationships. The
             current study extends earlier findings by examining changes
             in children's goals during hypothetical conflicts.
             Participants were 252 children ages 9 to 12 years old (133
             boys, 119 girls). As predicted, children's goals changed
             significantly when they encountered obstacles to conflict
             resolution, and these changes were predictive of their
             subsequent strategy choices. Both aggressive- and
             submissive-rejected children were more likely to evidence
             antisocial changes in their goals, including an increased
             desire to retaliate. They also showed reluctance to forego
             instrumental objectives. Other findings highlighted the need
             to investigate the combinations of goals children pursue as
             predictors of their strategies and the quality of their peer
             relationships.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00864.x},
   Key = {fds250604}
}

@article{fds250605,
   Author = {Guerra, VS and Asher, SR and DeRosier, ME},
   Title = {Effect of children’s perceived rejection on physical
             aggression},
   Journal = {Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {551-563},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0091-0627},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15500033},
   Abstract = {This study investigated whether the perception of self as
             socially rejected might contribute to increased physical
             aggression among elementary-school children. It was
             hypothesized that physically aggressive children would
             become more physically aggressive over time if they
             perceived that they were rejected and tended to blame peers
             for social failure experiences. Third-grade boys and girls
             (n = 941) were assessed in the Fall and Spring of the school
             year. Peer-report data on physical aggression and social
             preference were collected, along with self-report data on
             perceived rejection and attributions for social failure
             experiences. Results for boys were consistent with
             hypotheses, whereas the results for girls revealed a
             different pattern of relations. These results constitute
             prospective evidence that children's self-perceptions of
             social rejection can uniquely influence externalizing
             behavior. Results are discussed in terms of mechanisms that
             might mediate the relation between perceived rejection and
             physical aggression.},
   Doi = {10.1023/b:jacp.0000037783.88097.69},
   Key = {fds250605}
}

@article{fds250596,
   Author = {Ikeda, RM and Simon, TR and Smith, EP and Reese, LRE and Rabiner, DL and Miller-Johnson, S and Winn, DM and Asher, SR and Dodge, KA and Horne,
             AM and Orpinas, P and Quinn, WH and Huberty, CH and Tolan, PH and Gorman-Smith, D and Henry, DB and Gay, FN and Farrell, AD and Meyer, AL and Sullivan, TN and Allison, KW and Proj, MVP},
   Title = {The Multisite Violence Prevention Project: Background and
             overview},
   Journal = {American Journal of Preventive Medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1 Suppl},
   Pages = {3-11},
   Publisher = {ELSEVIER SCIENCE INC},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0749-3797},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000187880000002&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {This paper provides an overview of the Multisite Violence
             Prevention Project, a 5-year project to compare the effects
             of a universal intervention (all students and teachers) and
             a targeted intervention (family program for high-risk
             children) on reducing aggression and violence among sixth
             graders. First, the paper describes the role of the Centers
             for Disease Control and Prevention in developing the
             project. Second, it details the background of researchers at
             the four participating universities (Duke University, The
             University of Georgia, University of Illinois at Chicago,
             and Virginia Commonwealth University) and examines the
             characteristics of the selected schools (n=37). Finally, the
             paper summarizes the theoretical perspectives guiding the
             work, the development of interventions based on promising
             strategies, the decision to intervene at the school level,
             the research questions guiding the project, the research
             design, and the measurement process for evaluating the
             results of the program.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.amepre.2003.09.017},
   Key = {fds250596}
}

@article{fds250597,
   Author = {Ikeda, RM and Simon, TR and Smith, EP and Reese, LRE and Rabiner, DL and Miller-Johnson, S and Winn, DM and Asher, SR and Dodge, KA and Horne,
             AM and Orpinas, P and Quinn, WH and Huberty, CJ and Tolan, PH and Gorman-Smith, D and Henry, DB and Gay, FN and Farrell, AD and Meyer, AL and Sullivan, TN and Allison, KW and Proj, MVP},
   Title = {Lessons learned in the Multisite Violence Prevention Project
             collaboration: Big questions require large
             efforts},
   Journal = {American Journal of Preventive Medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1 Suppl},
   Pages = {62-71},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0749-3797},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000187880000008&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {This paper summarizes some organizational, scientific, and
             policy lessons that have emerged in the formation and
             conducting of the collaboration of the Multisite Violence
             Prevention Project. We contend that these lessons are
             valuable for other collaborations and are important for
             furthering the utility of scientific efforts. A central
             contention is that large-scale efforts such as this
             collaboration are underused but are essential for efficient
             advancement of knowledge about preventing youth
             violence.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.amepre.2003.09.025},
   Key = {fds250597}
}

@article{fds250598,
   Author = {Smith, EP and Gorman-Smith, D and Quinn, WH and Rabiner, DL and Tolan,
             PH and Winn, D-M and Multisite Violence Prevention
             Project},
   Title = {Community-Based multiple family groups to prevent and reduce
             violent and aggressive behavior: the GREAT Families
             Program.},
   Journal = {American journal of preventive medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {1 Suppl},
   Pages = {39-47},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2003.09.018},
   Abstract = {This paper describes the targeted intervention component of
             GREAT Schools and Families. The intervention-GREAT
             Families-is composed of 15 weekly multiple family group
             meetings (e.g., 4-6 families per group) and addresses
             parenting practices (discipline, monitoring), family
             relationship characteristics (communication, support,
             cohesion), parental involvement and investment in their
             child's schooling, parent and school relationship building,
             and planning for the future. High-risk youth and their
             families-students identified by teachers as aggressive and
             socially influential among their peers-were targeted for
             inclusion in the intervention. The paper describes the
             theoretical model and development of the intervention.
             Approaches to recruitment, engagement, staff training, and
             sociocultural sensitivity in work with families in
             predominantly poor and challenging settings are described.
             The data being collected throughout the program will aid in
             examining the theoretical and program processes that can
             potentially mediate and moderate effects on families. This
             work can inform us about necessary approaches and procedures
             to engage and support families in efforts to reduce
             individual and school grade-level violence and
             aggression.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.amepre.2003.09.018},
   Key = {fds250598}
}

@article{fds250599,
   Author = {Orpinas, and P, and Horne, and M, A and Dodge, TMVPPKA and member},
   Title = {A teacher-focused approach to prevent and reduce students'
             aggressive behavior: The GREAT Teacher Program},
   Journal = {American Journal of Preventive Medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Pages = {29-38},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds250599}
}

@article{fds250600,
   Author = {Meyer, and L, A and Allison, and W, K and Reese, and E, L and Gay, and N, F and Dodge, TMVPPKA and member},
   Title = {Choosing to be violence free in middle school: The student
             component of the GREAT Schools and Families Universal
             Program.},
   Journal = {American Journal of Preventive Medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Pages = {20-28},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds250600}
}

@article{fds250601,
   Author = {Henry, and B, D and Farrell, and D, A and Project,
             MVP},
   Title = {The study designed by a committee: Design of the
             MVPP},
   Journal = {American Journal of Preventive Medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Pages = {12-19},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds250601}
}

@article{fds250602,
   Author = {Miller-Johnson, and S, and Sullivan, and T, and Simon, and Project,
             M-SVP},
   Title = {Evaluation of a multisite violence prevention study:
             Measures, procedures and baseline data},
   Journal = {American Journal of Preventive Medicine},
   Volume = {26},
   Pages = {48-61},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds250602}
}

@article{fds250645,
   Author = {Rose, AJ and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's strategies and goals in response to help-giving
             and help-seeking tasks within a friendship},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {75},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {749-763},
   Year = {2004},
   ISSN = {0009-3920},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00704.x},
   Abstract = {The present research tested whether children's responses to
             help-giving and help-seeking friendship tasks predicted how
             many friends they had and the quality of their best
             friendship. Fifth-grade children (N=511; typically 10 or 11
             years old) responded to vignettes in which they could either
             give help to a friend or seek help from a friend. Children's
             strategies and goals in both contexts were significantly
             correlated with the number of friends children had.
             Responses in the help-giving context but not in the
             help-seeking context were significantly associated with
             friendship quality. Although gender differences in
             strategies and goals were found, strategies and goals were
             related to the number of friends and friendship quality for
             both boys and girls.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00704.x},
   Key = {fds250645}
}

@article{fds250644,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Paquette, JA},
   Title = {Loneliness and peer relations in childhood},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {75-78},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2003},
   ISSN = {0963-7214},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01233},
   Abstract = {Although loneliness is a normative experience, there is
             reason to be concerned about children who are chronically
             lonely in school Research indicates that children have a
             fundamental understanding of what it means, to be lonely,
             and that loneliness can be reliably measured in children.
             Most of the research on loneliness in children has focused
             on the contributions of children's peer relations to their
             feelings of well-being at school. Loneliness in children is
             influenced by how well accepted they are by peers, whether
             they are overtly victimized, whether they have friends, and
             the durability and quality of their best friendships.
             Findings from this emerging area of research provide a
             differentiated picture of how children's peer experiences
             come to influence their emotional well-being.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-8721.01233},
   Key = {fds250644}
}

@article{fds250606,
   Author = {Erdley, CA and Asher, SR},
   Title = {A social goals perspective on children's social
             competence},
   Journal = {Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {156-167},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1999},
   ISSN = {1063-4266},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/106342669900700304},
   Abstract = {Developing a more comprehensive understanding of social
             competence and the reasons for social relationship
             difficulties requires attention to the kinds of goals
             children are pursuing in specific social situations. In this
             article, the authors describe several theoretical models of
             social information processing, each of which includes goals
             as a crucial component in motivating children's behavior.
             The authors also review evidence from studies of children
             who are aggressive, withdrawn/submissive, or prosocial that
             points to an association between children's goals, their
             strategies for coping with problematic social tasks, and
             their ultimate success or failure in social relationships.
             Attention is also given to various social-cognitive
             processes that may be linked to children's tendencies to
             formulate adaptive versus maladaptive goals. Finally, the
             implications of a social goals perspective for interventions
             with children with emotional and behavioral problems is
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1177/106342669900700304},
   Key = {fds250606}
}

@article{fds250607,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Gazelle, H},
   Title = {Loneliness, peer relations, and language disorder in
             childhood},
   Journal = {Topics in Language Disorders},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {16-33},
   Publisher = {Ovid Technologies (Wolters Kluwer Health)},
   Year = {1999},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00011363-199902000-00004},
   Abstract = {Children with language disorders have higher than average
             rates of peer relationship problems, suggesting that they
             are also at risk for loneliness. A review of research on
             loneliness as an emotional consequence of peer relationship
             difficulties in childhood is preceded by a discussion of the
             particular relevance of this literature for children with
             language difficulties. Evidence from research on loneliness
             indicates that peer acceptance, participation in friendship,
             friendship quality, and victimization by peers each
             contribute to children's feelings of loneliness at school.
             Suggestions are made concerning intervention efforts to
             reduce loneliness for children with language
             problems.},
   Doi = {10.1097/00011363-199902000-00004},
   Key = {fds250607}
}

@article{fds250646,
   Author = {Rose, AJ and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's goals and strategies in response to conflicts
             within a friendship},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {69-79},
   Year = {1999},
   ISSN = {0012-1649},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.69},
   Abstract = {Little is known about the skills required for friendship, as
             distinct from those required for peer acceptance. The
             present study examined whether children's goals and
             strategies in friendship conflict situations are predictive
             of their friendship adjustment, after accounting for level
             of peer acceptance. Fourth- and 5th-grade children (N = 696)
             responded to 30 hypothetical situations in which they were
             having a conflict with a friend. Results indicated that
             children's goals were highly related to their strategies and
             that children's goals and strategies were predictive of
             their real-life friendship adjustment. Pursuing the goal of
             revenge toward a friend was the goal or strategy most
             strongly associated with lacking friends and having
             poor-quality friendships. Gender differences were also found
             for each goal and strategy, with girls displaying a more
             prosocial goal and strategy orientation than
             boys.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.69},
   Key = {fds250646}
}

@article{fds304643,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Gazelle, H},
   Title = {Loneliness, Peer Relations, and Language Disorder in
             Childhood},
   Journal = {Topics in Language Disorders},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {16-33},
   Year = {1999},
   Abstract = {Children with language disorders have higher than average
             rates of peer relationship problems, suggesting that they
             are also at risk for loneliness. A review of research on
             loneliness as an emotional consequence of peer relationship
             difficulties in childhood is preceded by a discussion of the
             particular relevance of this literature for children with
             language difficulties. Evidence from research on loneliness
             indicates that peer acceptance, participation in friendship,
             friendship quality, and victimization by peers each
             contribute to children's feelings of loneliness at school.
             Suggestions are made concerning intervention efforts to
             reduce loneliness for children with language
             problems.},
   Key = {fds304643}
}

@article{fds304642,
   Author = {Erdley, CA and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Linkages between children's beliefs about the legitimacy of
             aggression and their behavior},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {321-339},
   Year = {1998},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00070},
   Abstract = {The purposes of this study were to learn whether children's
             beliefs about the legitimacy of aggression can be reliably
             assessed and whether these beliefs relate to children's
             everyday social behavior with peers, as well as their
             responses to hypothetical ambiguous provocation situations.
             Fourth- and fifth-grade students (n = 781) responded to a
             16-item questionnaire designed to measure children's beliefs
             about the legitimacy of aggression. Children's behavioral
             orientation was assessed using two methods: (1) children's
             responses to ten hypothetical situations involving ambiguous
             provocation, and (2) peer evaluations of children's
             aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behavior. Results
             indicated that children's beliefs about the legitimacy of
             aggression were reliably measured. Furthermore, results from
             both measures of behavioral style showed that children who
             believed strongly in the legitimacy of aggression were more
             aggressive, less withdrawn, and less prosocial. The findings
             suggest that one focus of efforts to decrease children's
             aggression should be the modification of their beliefs about
             the legitimacy of aggressive actions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/1467-9507.00070},
   Key = {fds304642}
}

@article{fds250608,
   Author = {Erdley, CA and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Linkages between children's beliefs about the legitimacy of
             aggression and their behavior},
   Journal = {Social Development},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {321-329},
   Year = {1998},
   Abstract = {The purposes of this study were to learn whether children's
             beliefs about the legitimacy of aggression can be reliably
             assessed and whether these beliefs relate to children's
             everyday social behavior with peers, as well as their
             responses to hypothetical ambiguous provocation situations.
             Fourth- and fifth-grade students (n = 781) responded to a
             16-item questionnaire designed to measure children's beliefs
             about the legitimacy of aggression. Children's behavioral
             orientation was assessed using two methods: (1) children's
             responses to ten hypothetical situations involving ambiguous
             provocation, and (2) peer evaluations of children's
             aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behavior. Results
             indicated that children's beliefs about the legitimacy of
             aggression were reliably measured. Furthermore, results from
             both measures of behavioral style showed that children who
             believed strongly in the legitimacy of aggression were more
             aggressive, less withdrawn, and less prosocial. The findings
             suggest that one focus of efforts to decrease children's
             aggression should be the modification of their beliefs about
             the legitimacy of aggressive actions.},
   Key = {fds250608}
}

@article{fds250589,
   Author = {Hopmeyer, A and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's responses to peer conflicts involving a rights
             infraction},
   Journal = {Merrill-Palmer Quarterly},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {235-254},
   Year = {1997},
   Abstract = {Examined here is whether the generalization about the
             prosocial style of well-accepted children applies to peer
             conflicts involving a rights infraction. Fourth- and
             fifth-grade children (N = 140) were individually interviewed
             about their strategies for handling hypothetical situations
             in which a peer infringes upon their rights. Results
             revealed that well-accepted children were neither aggressive
             nor particularly prosocial in conflict situations. Instead,
             they responded with a variety of verbally assertive
             strategies. Low-accepted children, by contrast, relied more
             heavily on the assistance of adults. Supplementary data
             collection with 215 fourth- and fifth-grade children
             suggested the psychological distinctiveness of "rights
             infraction" types of conflict. Overall, the results qualify
             earlier generalizations about the behavioral basis of peer
             acceptance and suggest the need for situation-specific
             assessments of children's social competence.},
   Key = {fds250589}
}

@article{fds250609,
   Author = {Chung, T and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's goals and strategies in peer conflict
             situations},
   Journal = {Merrill-Palmer Quarterly},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {125-147},
   Year = {1996},
   Abstract = {This study was designed to investigate whether children's
             goals in peer conflict situations are related to the
             strategies they propose for resolving conflicts and to learn
             how children's strategies relate to their acceptance by the
             peer group. Fourth-through sixth-grade children (80 boys, 62
             girls) were asked about their goals and strategies in
             response to 12 hypothetical peer conflict situations.
             Results indicated that children's strategies for responding
             to peer conflicts differed according to their goal
             orientation. Furthermore, children's selection of strategies
             correlated with peer acceptance, although the linkages were
             different for boys and girls. Peer acceptance was negatively
             related to hostile/coercive strategies for girls, and
             negatively to adult-seeking strategies for boys.
             Implications of the present findings for social skills
             intervention are discussed.},
   Key = {fds250609}
}

@article{fds250647,
   Author = {Erdley, CA and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's social goals and self-efficacy perceptions as
             influences on their responses to ambiguous
             provocation},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {67},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1329-1344},
   Year = {1996},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01799.x},
   Abstract = {This study examined whether children who vary in their
             behavioral responses (aggression vs. withdrawal vs. problem
             solving) to ambiguous provocation but who are similar in
             their attributional processes differ in their social goals
             and self-efficacy perceptions. In response to 10
             hypothetical situations involving ambiguous provocation,
             fourth- and fifth-grade students (n = 781) indicated whether
             or not the protagonist intended to cause the harm and
             reported how they would respond to the protagonist's action.
             Newly developed measures assessed children's situated social
             goals and self-efficacy perceptions. Results indicated that
             the aggressive, withdrawn, and problem-solving responders
             differed in their social goals and self-efficacy
             perceptions. The strength of the findings, compared to
             earlier work on children's goals and self-efficacy
             perceptions, suggests the importance of a situated
             social-cognitive assessment in which children's thoughts are
             measured in a specific kind of social situation and are
             related to their reported behavior in the same type of
             situation.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01799.x},
   Key = {fds250647}
}

@article{fds250610,
   Author = {Wentzel, K and Asher, SR},
   Title = {The academic lives of neglected, rejected, popular, and
             controversial children},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {66},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {754-763},
   Year = {1995},
   ISSN = {0009-3920},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00903.x},
   Abstract = {The purposes of this study were to examine academically
             relevant characteristics of different sociometric status
             groups and to learn about the academic orientations of
             behavioral subgroups of rejected children. Results from a
             sample of 423 sixth and seventh graders (ages 11-13)
             suggested that sociometrically neglected children have quite
             positive academic profiles. When compared with average
             status children, these students reported higher levels of
             motivation, were described by teachers as more
             self-regulated learners, as more prosocial and compliant,
             and as being better liked by teachers. Analyses of two
             behavioral subgroups of rejected children indicated that
             aggressive-rejected but not submissive-rejected children
             have problematic academic profiles. Relations of neglected
             and aggressive-rejected status to academic adjustment in
             young adolescents' lives is discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00903.x},
   Key = {fds250610}
}

@article{fds250611,
   Author = {Parker, JG and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links
             with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and
             social dissatisfaction},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {611-621},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1993},
   ISSN = {0012-1649},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.611},
   Abstract = {The distinction between friendship adjustment and acceptance
             by the peer group was examined. Third- through 5th-grade
             children (N = 881) completed sociometric measures of
             acceptance and friendship, a measure of loneliness, a
             questionnaire on the features of their very best
             friendships, and a measure of their friendship satisfaction.
             Results indicated that many low-accepted children had best
             friends and were satisfied with these friendships. However,
             these children's friendships were lower than those of other
             children on most dimensions of quality. Having a friend,
             friendship quality, and group acceptance made separate
             contributions to the prediction of loneliness. Results
             indicate the utility of the new friendship quality measure
             and the value of distinguishing children's friendship
             adjustment from their general peer acceptance.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.611},
   Key = {fds250611}
}

@article{fds250612,
   Author = {Cassidy, J and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Loneliness and peer relations in young children},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {63},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {350-365},
   Year = {1992},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01632.x},
   Abstract = {Recent studies indicate that feelings of loneliness and
             social dissatisfaction can be reliably assessed with third-
             through sixth-grade children, and that children who are
             sociometrically rejected by their peers are significantly
             more lonely than other children. The present research was
             designed (a) to examine whether loneliness could be reliably
             assessed in a population younger than previously studied,
             (b) to learn whether young children who are poorly accepted
             by peers report elevated levels of loneliness and social
             dissatisfaction, (c) to assess whether young children
             understand the concept of loneliness, and (d) to examine the
             behavioral characteristics of lonely young children.
             Kindergarten and first-grade children (N = 440) responded to
             a questionnaire about feelings of loneliness and social
             dissatisfaction in school. A subset of children (N = 46)
             were individually interviewed to assess their understanding
             of loneliness. To assess sociometric status and behavior,
             peers were asked to respond to various sociometric measures
             and behavioral assessment items. Teachers also provided
             behavioral information about children using a newly
             developed instrument. Results indicated that nearly all
             children understood loneliness, that loneliness was reliably
             assessed in young children, and that poorly accepted
             children were more lonely than other children. In addition,
             children who reported the most loneliness were found to
             differ from others on several behavioral
             dimensions.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01632.x},
   Key = {fds250612}
}

@article{fds250613,
   Author = {Williams, GA and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Assessment of loneliness at school among children with mild
             mental retardation},
   Journal = {American Journal of Mental Retardation},
   Volume = {96},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {373-385},
   Year = {1992},
   Abstract = {This study was designed to determine whether children with
             mild mental retardation understand the concept of
             loneliness, whether their feelings of loneliness at school
             can be reliably assessed, and whether there are differences
             in loneliness between children with and without mental
             retardation. Results from a sample of 62 students with mild
             mental retardation and 62 students without retardation, ages
             8 through 13, indicated that (a) high percentages of both
             groups understood what loneliness means, (b) a loneliness
             questionnaire yielded satisfactory internal reliability with
             both groups, and (c) boys but not girls with mental
             retardation reported significantly more loneliness than did
             children without mental retardation. Suggestions were made
             for future research on loneliness in school
             settings.},
   Key = {fds250613}
}

@article{fds250614,
   Author = {Parkhurst, JT and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Peer rejection in middle school: Subgroup differences in
             behavior, loneliness, and interpersonal concerns},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {231-241},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1992},
   ISSN = {0012-1649},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.231},
   Abstract = {This research was designed to identify patterns of behavior
             and emotional response associated with peer rejection in
             early adolescence. Seventh- and eighth-grade middle-school
             students (N = 450) were administered positive and negative
             sociometric nominations, peer behavioral assessment items, a
             loneliness and social dissatisfaction questionnaire, and a
             newly developed interpersonal concerns questionnaire.
             Results indicated that most rejected students were
             aggressive or submissive, but it was the combination of
             aggressiveness or submissiveness with low levels of
             prosocial behavior that was associated with peer rejection.
             With regard to students' affective experiences,
             submissive-rejected students, when compared with
             average-status students, were found to report higher levels
             of loneliness and worry about their relations with others.
             Aggressive-rejected students did not differ on these
             dimensions from average-status students.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.231},
   Key = {fds250614}
}

@article{fds38357,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Williams, G.A.},
   Title = {Helping children without friends in home and school
             contexts},
   Booktitle = {Children's social development: Information for teachers and
             parents},
   Publisher = {Urbana, IL: University of Illinois},
   Year = {1987},
   Key = {fds38357}
}

@article{fds250615,
   Author = {Taylor, AR and Asher, SR and Williams, GA},
   Title = {The social adaptation of mainstreamed mildly retarded
             children},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {58},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1321-1334},
   Year = {1987},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130624},
   Abstract = {The purpose of the present study was to assess the social
             adaptation of mainstreamed mildly retarded children,
             comparing them to a matched sample of regular education
             students. Results indicated that mildly retarded children
             were quite rejected by their peers, and, compared to
             nonretarded classmates, they reported significantly more
             dissatisfaction and anxiety about their peer relations. With
             respect to behavioral style, there were no group differences
             in peer- or teacher-reported aggressiveness or
             disruptiveness. However, retarded children were perceived as
             more shy and avoidant, as less cooperative, and as less
             likely to exhibit leadership skills. Further analysis of the
             behavioral assessment data, via cluster analysis, revealed 2
             subtypes of rejected retarded children: an internalizing
             group perceived as displaying high levels of shy/avoidant
             behavior and an externalizing group perceived as high in
             aggressive and/or disruptive behavior. In addition, the
             internalizing group reported higher social anxiety than did
             the externalizing group. Implications for school-based
             intervention research with mildly retarded children are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.2307/1130624},
   Key = {fds250615}
}

@article{fds250616,
   Author = {Parker, JG and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are
             low-accepted children at risk?},
   Journal = {Psychological Bulletin},
   Volume = {102},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {357-389},
   Year = {1987},
   ISSN = {0033-2909},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.102.3.357},
   Abstract = {In this review, we examine the oft-made claim that
             peer-relationship difficulties in childhood predict serious
             adjustment problems in later life. The article begins with a
             framework for conceptualizing and assessing children's peer
             difficulties and with a discussion of conceptual and
             methodological issues in longitudinal risk research.
             Following this, three indexes of problematic peer
             relationships (acceptance, aggressiveness, and
             shyness/withdrawal) are evaluated as predictors of three
             later outcomes (dropping out of school, criminality, and
             psychopathology). The relation between peer difficulties and
             later maladjustment is examined in terms of both the
             consistency and strength of prediction. A review and
             analysis of the literature indicates general support for the
             hypothesis that children with poor peer adjustment are at
             risk for later life difficulties. Support is clearest for
             the outcomes of dropping out and criminality. It is also
             clearest for low acceptance and aggressiveness as
             predictors, whereas a link between shyness/withdrawal and
             later maladjustment has not yet been adequately tested. The
             article concludes with a critical discussion of the implicit
             models that have guided past research in this area and a set
             of recommendations for the next generation of research on
             the risk hypothesis. © 1987 American Psychological
             Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0033-2909.102.3.357},
   Key = {fds250616}
}

@article{fds250617,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Dodge, KA},
   Title = {Identifying children who are rejected by their
             peers},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {444-449},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1986},
   ISSN = {0012-1649},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.444},
   Abstract = {Recent research indicates the importance of distinguishing
             between sociometrically neglected children and
             sociometrically rejected children. Overall, rejected
             children exhibit more serious adjustment problems in
             childhood and in later life. However, making the distinction
             between neglected status and rejected status traditionally
             has required administering a negative-nomination sociometric
             measure, a measure viewed by some researchers and school
             personnel as having potentially harmful effects. In this
             article, we propose and evaluate an alternative method of
             identifying rejected children, which involves the joint use
             of positive-nomination and rating-scale measures. The
             results indicate that the alternative method accurately
             identifies a high percentage of rejected children (91.2%)
             and that the stability of rejected status, identified using
             the new method, is similar to that obtained in previous
             research. The method proposed here should make it possible
             to identify rejected children when circumstances do not
             allow for the administration of a negative-nomination
             measure. © 1986 American Psychological Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.444},
   Key = {fds250617}
}

@article{fds250618,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Hymel, S},
   Title = {Coaching in social skills for children who lack friends in
             school},
   Journal = {Social Work in Education},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {205-218},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press (OUP)},
   Year = {1986},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cs/8.4.205},
   Abstract = {Successful peer relationships are critical to children's
             social development, even more so in view of the increase of
             single-parent families and children's early enrollment in
             activities such as day care. This article discusses the
             literature, describes sociometric procedures for identifying
             children without friends, and provides two examples of
             social skills training. © 1986 by the National Association
             of Social Workers, Inc.},
   Doi = {10.1093/cs/8.4.205},
   Key = {fds250618}
}

@article{fds250619,
   Author = {Bursuck, WD and Asher, SR},
   Title = {The relationship between social competence and achievement
             in elementary school children},
   Journal = {Journal of Clinical Child Psychology},
   Volume = {15},
   Pages = {41-49},
   Year = {1986},
   Key = {fds250619}
}

@article{fds250620,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Wheeler, VA},
   Title = {Children's loneliness: a comparison of rejected and
             neglected peer status.},
   Journal = {Journal of consulting and clinical psychology},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {500-505},
   Year = {1985},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0022-006X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.53.4.500},
   Abstract = {Recent research indicates that a considerable number of
             children report extreme feelings of loneliness and that
             unpopular children are more lonely than popular children. In
             the present study, we assessed feelings of loneliness of two
             subgroups of unpopular children, those who were
             sociometrically rejected versus those who were
             sociometrically neglected. Data on popular, average, and
             controversial children were also collected. Results from 200
             third- through sixth-grade children indicated that rejected
             children were the most lonely group and that this group
             differed significantly from other status groups. Neglected
             children did not differ from higher status peers. Overall,
             the results provide added evidence of the utility of the
             distinction between neglected versus rejected status and
             provide support for earlier conclusions that rejected
             children are more at risk than are other status groups. ©
             1985 American Psychological Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037//0022-006x.53.4.500},
   Key = {fds250620}
}

@article{fds250621,
   Author = {Renshaw, PD and Asher, SR},
   Title = {The study of children's goals: A reply to Gresham's
             commentary},
   Journal = {Merrill-Palmer Quarterly},
   Volume = {31},
   Pages = {105-109},
   Year = {1985},
   Key = {fds250621}
}

@article{fds250622,
   Author = {Markell, RA and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's interactions in dyads: Interpersonal influence
             and sociometric status},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {55},
   Pages = {1412-1424},
   Year = {1984},
   Key = {fds250622}
}

@article{fds250623,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Hymel, S and Renshaw, PD},
   Title = {Loneliness in children},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {55},
   Pages = {1456-1464},
   Year = {1984},
   Key = {fds250623}
}

@article{fds250624,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {Social competence and peer status: Recent advances and
             future directions},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {54},
   Pages = {1427-1434},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds250624}
}

@article{fds250625,
   Author = {Renshaw, PD and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's goals and strategies for social
             interaction},
   Journal = {Merrill-Palmer Quarterly},
   Volume = {29},
   Pages = {353-374},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds250625}
}

@article{fds250626,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Taylor, AR},
   Title = {Social skill training with children: Evaluating processes
             and outcomes},
   Journal = {Studies in Educational Evaluation},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {237-245},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {1982},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0191-491X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-491X(82)90027-X},
   Doi = {10.1016/0191-491X(82)90027-X},
   Key = {fds250626}
}

@article{fds250627,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Markell, RA and Hymel, S},
   Title = {Identifying children at risk in peer relations: A critique
             of the rate-of-interaction approach to assessment},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {52},
   Pages = {1239-1245},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds250627}
}

@article{fds250628,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Wigfield, A},
   Title = {Influence of comparison training on children's referential
             communication},
   Journal = {Journal of Educational Psychology},
   Volume = {73},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {232-241},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1981},
   ISSN = {0022-0663},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.73.2.232},
   Abstract = {Assessed whether teaching children to engage in comparison
             activity improves their referential communication
             performance. In contrast to previous communication training
             studies, the training focused on a specifiable unitary
             skill, employed a teaching procedure with known
             effectiveness, and controlled for practice and other
             familiarity effects. Two training experiments with 3rd- and
             4th-grade children were conducted. Results from Exp I
             indicate that 33 trained Ss improved more than 32 control Ss
             in communication accuracy. Furthermore, gains were
             maintained at the 1-mo follow-up. Exp II (20 undergraduates;
             43 children) examined training effects on message appraisal
             as well as message production. Results indicate significant
             training effects on both tasks. Results demonstrate that
             inadequate comparison processing contributes to children's
             poor communication performance. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database
             Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved). © 1981 American
             Psychological Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0022-0663.73.2.232},
   Key = {fds250628}
}

@article{fds250629,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Taylor, AR},
   Title = {Social outcomes of mainstreaming: Sociometric assessment and
             beyond},
   Journal = {Exceptional Education Quarterly},
   Volume = {1},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {13-30},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds250629}
}

@article{fds250630,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Renshaw, PD and Geraci, RL},
   Title = {Children's friendships and social competence},
   Journal = {International Journal of Psycholinguistics},
   Volume = {7},
   Pages = {27-39},
   Year = {1980},
   Key = {fds250630}
}

@article{fds250631,
   Author = {Singleton, LC and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Racial integration and children's peer preferences: An
             investigation of developmental and cohort
             differences},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {50},
   Pages = {936-941},
   Year = {1979},
   Key = {fds250631}
}

@article{fds250632,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {Influence of topic interest on black children's and white
             children's reading comprehension},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {50},
   Pages = {686-690},
   Year = {1979},
   Key = {fds250632}
}

@article{fds250633,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Singleton, LC and Tinsley, BR and Hymel,
             S},
   Title = {A reliable sociometric measure for preschool
             children},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {15},
   Pages = {443-444},
   Year = {1979},
   Key = {fds250633}
}

@article{fds376457,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Singleton, LC},
   Title = {Cross-race acceptance in integrated schools},
   Journal = {Equity and Excellence in Education},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {17-20},
   Year = {1978},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0020486780160503},
   Doi = {10.1080/0020486780160503},
   Key = {fds376457}
}

@article{fds250634,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Hymel, S and Wigfield, A},
   Title = {Influence of topic interest on children's reading
             comprehension},
   Journal = {Journal of Reading Behavior},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {35-47},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {1978},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862967809547253},
   Abstract = {Previous research indicates that children comprehend more of
             high-than low-interest material when each child is given a
             mixture of both types of material. This effect could be due
             to a contrast effect whereby children selectively respond to
             the more appealing topics in their set of passages. In the
             present study fifth grade children each received either all
             high-interest cloze passages or all low-interest cloze
             passages but not both. Children's cloze responses were
             scored by the exact replacement method and by a method which
             included synonyms as correct. Results were that children
             comprehended more of high- than low-interest material,
             indicating that the interest effect is not dependent on a
             contrast phenomenon. Synonym production data indicated that
             high-achieving children generated more synonyms than
             low-achieving children and that boys produced more synonyms
             than girls. Thus, scoring synonyms as correct appears to
             have informational value. © 1978, SAGE Publications. All
             rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1080/10862967809547253},
   Key = {fds250634}
}

@article{fds250635,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Singleton, LC},
   Title = {Cross-race acceptance in integrated schools},
   Journal = {Integrateducation},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {17-20},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {1978},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0020486780160503},
   Doi = {10.1080/0020486780160503},
   Key = {fds250635}
}

@article{fds250636,
   Author = {Zahavi, S and Asher, SR},
   Title = {The effect of verbal instructions on preschool children's
             aggressive behavior},
   Journal = {Journal of School Psychology},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {146-153},
   Year = {1978},
   ISSN = {0022-4405},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-4405(78)90053-5},
   Abstract = {Aggressive preschool children were instructed by their
             teacher on the harm that results from aggression, its lack
             of effects as an interpersonal strategy, and the benefits
             that result from prosocial alternatives. Results from a
             time-lagged design indicated that aggressive behavior
             decreased and positive behavior increased as a result of
             these instructions. Data on maintenance of change suggest
             the possibility that the effects may be lasting. Overall,
             findings indicate that instructing children individually can
             be an effective strategy for modifying social interaction.
             © 1978.},
   Doi = {10.1016/0022-4405(78)90053-5},
   Key = {fds250636}
}

@article{fds250637,
   Author = {Singleton, LC and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Peer preferences and social interaction among third-grade
             children in an integrated school district},
   Journal = {Journal of Educational Psychology},
   Volume = {69},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {330-336},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1977},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {0022-0663},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.69.4.330},
   Abstract = {Assessed social interaction among Black and White 3rd-grade
             children in 9 classrooms. The children were in integrated
             schools since kindergarten. Two methods were employed: a
             roster-and-rating sociometric technique and a classroom
             behavioral observation procedure using sequential time
             sampling. Sociometric data were obtained for 179 White and
             48 Black children. Observational data were obtained for 39
             White and 39 Black children. Both race and sex were found to
             be significant determinants of sociometric ratings for play
             and work. Omega-squared analyses, however, indicated that
             sex accounted for 43.2% of the variance on play and 35.2% on
             work, while race accounted for only about 1% of the variance
             on each measure. The observational data indicate a positive
             pattern of classroom interaction. (PsycINFO Database Record
             (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved). © 1977 American
             Psychological Association.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0022-0663.69.4.330},
   Key = {fds250637}
}

@article{fds250638,
   Author = {Oden, S and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Coaching children in social skills for friendship
             making},
   Journal = {Child Development},
   Volume = {48},
   Pages = {495-506},
   Year = {1977},
   Key = {fds250638}
}

@article{fds250639,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Oden, SL},
   Title = {Children's failure to communicate: An assessment of
             comparison and egocentrism explanations},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {132-139},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1976},
   ISSN = {0012-1649},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.12.2.132},
   Abstract = {Studied 2 possible explanations of why many young children
             communicate poorly on tasks that require them to
             discriminate referents from nonreferents: (a) poor
             communicators do not compare the associative strength of
             potential messages to the referent and nonreferent or (b)
             poor communicators engage in an egocentric form of
             comparison activity such that their messages have private
             but not public meaning. The latter implies that children who
             communicate poorly to another person could make effective
             use of their own messages. To examine these explanations, 64
             good and poor communicators from grades 3 and 5 were asked
             to identify referents from their own messages. Ss who
             communicated poorly were also less effective than good
             communicators on the self-communication task. Data suggest
             that poor communicators do not engage in comparison activity
             even for their own private understanding.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.12.2.132},
   Key = {fds250639}
}

@article{fds250640,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's ability to appraise their own versus another
             person's communication performance},
   Journal = {Developmental Psychology},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {24-32},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1976},
   ISSN = {0012-1649},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.12.1.24},
   Abstract = {Previous research has found that younger children are less
             accurate communicators to a listener. In the present
             research, children's ability to appraise or evaluate the
             quality of communication performance was investigated. In
             Exp I, 90 2nd-, 4th-, and 6th-grade children communicated
             messages for 15 referents. Half of the Ss then appraised
             their own messages, and half appraised the messages of a
             yoked age-mate. In the self-appraisal condition, Ss were
             asked to indicate whether or not each of their messages was
             effective. In the other-appraisal condition, Ss indicated
             whether or not each of a peer's messages was effective.
             Results indicate that younger Ss were less accurate
             appraisers of performance as well as less accurate
             communicators. Within a particular grade level, Ss were
             similar in their self-appraisal and other-appraisal
             accuracy. Exp II controlled the quality of messages that Ss
             judged across grade level. 15 Ss appraised a standard set of
             messages half of which were effective and half of which were
             ineffective. Younger Ss were less accurate evaluators of
             good and poor messages. Results from both studies are
             considered in light of evidence that younger children do not
             compare the association of messages to referents and
             nonreferents. Failure to engage in comparison activity would
             result in poor appraisal accuracy as well as poor
             communication accuracy.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0012-1649.12.1.24},
   Key = {fds250640}
}

@article{fds250641,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Parke, RD},
   Title = {The influence of sampling and comparison processes on the
             development of communication effectiveness},
   Journal = {Journal of Educational Psychology},
   Volume = {67},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {64-75},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1975},
   ISSN = {0022-0663},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0078672},
   Abstract = {Children's communication performance improves with age,
             which could result from increases in children's
             vocabularies, changes in their strategies of selecting
             messages from their vocabularies, or both. Three experiments
             were conducted to evaluate these explanations. Two types of
             communication tasks were employed. One task required that
             speakers have adequate vocabulary, while the other task had
             the additional requirement that speakers select their
             messages based on comparison activity. Results indicated
             that second-, fourth-, and sixth-grade children were equally
             effective communicators on the former task and that younger
             children were poorer communicators on the latter task. These
             data suggest that younger children have inadequate
             comparison strategies.},
   Doi = {10.1037/h0078672},
   Key = {fds250641}
}

@article{fds250642,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Markell, RA},
   Title = {Sex differences in comprehension of high- and low-interest
             reading material},
   Journal = {Journal of Educational Psychology},
   Volume = {66},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {680-687},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1974},
   ISSN = {0022-0663},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0037483},
   Abstract = {Investigated whether sex differences in reading
             comprehension are affected by variations in the interest
             level of the material. 49 male and 38 female 5th graders'
             interests were assessed using a picture-rating technique.
             Each child then read material that corresponded to his or
             her high- and low-interest areas. The cloze procedure was
             used to measure comprehension. Results indicate that boys
             read as well as girls on high-interest material but that
             they were significantly poorer readers of low-interest
             material.},
   Doi = {10.1037/h0037483},
   Key = {fds250642}
}

@article{fds250643,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Gottman, JM},
   Title = {Sex of teacher and student reading achievement},
   Journal = {Journal of Educational Psychology},
   Volume = {65},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {168-171},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {1973},
   ISSN = {0022-0663},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034986},
   Abstract = {Conducted 2 experiments to assess the effects of 5th-grade
             teacher sex on male and female student reading performance.
             Exp. I served as a pretest measure; the Iowa Test of Basic
             Skills Reading Comprehension subtest was administered to 534
             5th graders having 10 male and 10 female teachers. Exp. II
             served as a posttest measure by analyzing achievement data
             from the same school system 1 yr later. (Ss were 712 6th
             graders due to an increase in the number of male teachers to
             13.) Results show superior reading performance by girls.
             There was no significant effect of sex of teacher on male or
             female reading performance. It is suggested that further
             studies experimentally vary elements of the classroom
             environment relevant to sex-role standards rather than
             assume that these elements are correlated with sex of the
             teacher.},
   Doi = {10.1037/h0034986},
   Key = {fds250643}
}

@article{fds250603,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Allen, VL},
   Title = {Racial preference and social comparison processes},
   Journal = {Journal of Social Issues},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {157-166},
   Year = {1969},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1969.tb02584.x},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1540-4560.1969.tb02584.x},
   Key = {fds250603}
}


%% Books   
@book{fds310598,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Coie, J. D.},
   Title = {Peer rejection in childhood},
   Pages = {417 pages},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Asher, SR and Coie, JD},
   Year = {1990},
   ISBN = {9780521398367},
   Abstract = {This important collection brings together current research
             on children who encounter difficulty in gaining acceptance
             and having friendships among their peers. Translated into
             Japanese, 1996.},
   Key = {fds310598}
}

@book{fds310599,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Gottman, J.M.},
   Title = {The development of children's friendships},
   Pages = {347 pages},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Asher, SR and Gottman, JM},
   Year = {1981},
   ISBN = {9780521298063},
   Abstract = {From the perspectives of a wide range of disciplines, this
             volume studies a variety of cognitive and affective
             processes in the context of children's friendships.},
   Key = {fds310599}
}


%% Chapters in Books   
@misc{fds367651,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Weeks, MS},
   Title = {Friendships in Childhood},
   Pages = {119-134},
   Booktitle = {The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships, Second
             Edition},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781107130265},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/9781316417867.011},
   Doi = {10.1017/9781316417867.011},
   Key = {fds367651}
}

@misc{fds345885,
   Author = {Wigfield, A and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Social and motivational influences on reading},
   Pages = {423-452},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of Reading Research},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {November},
   ISBN = {9781138145269},
   Key = {fds345885}
}

@misc{fds310913,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Weeks, MS},
   Title = {Loneliness and belongingness in the college
             years},
   Pages = {283-301},
   Booktitle = {Wiley-Blackwell handbook of solitude: Psychological
             perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and
             being alone},
   Publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
   Editor = {Coplan, RJ and Bowker, JC},
   Year = {2014},
   Key = {fds310913}
}

@misc{fds310914,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Guerry, W and McDonald, KL},
   Title = {Children as friends},
   Pages = {169-194},
   Booktitle = {The SAGE handbook of child research},
   Publisher = {Sage},
   Editor = {Melton, GB and Ben-Arieh, A and Cashmore, J and Goodman, GS and Worley,
             NK},
   Year = {2014},
   Key = {fds310914}
}

@misc{fds340336,
   Author = {Weeks, MS and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Loneliness in childhood: Toward the next generation of
             assessment and research},
   Volume = {42},
   Pages = {1-39},
   Booktitle = {Advances in Child Development and Behavior},
   Publisher = {Academic Press},
   Editor = {Benson, JB},
   Year = {2012},
   Key = {fds340336}
}

@misc{fds341283,
   Author = {MacEvoy, JP and Weeks, MS and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Loneliness},
   Volume = {2},
   Pages = {178-187},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of adolescence,},
   Publisher = {Academic Press},
   Year = {2011},
   Key = {fds341283}
}

@misc{fds310915,
   Author = {Asher, SR and McDonald, KL},
   Title = {Children’s friendships},
   Series = {3rd Edition},
   Pages = {564-569},
   Booktitle = {International encyclopedia of education},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Editor = {McGraw, B and Peterson, PL and Baker, E},
   Year = {2010},
   Key = {fds310915}
}

@misc{fds310916,
   Author = {MacEvoy, JP and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Loneliness, children},
   Pages = {990-993},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of human relationships},
   Publisher = {Sage},
   Editor = {Reis, HT and Sprecher, S},
   Year = {2009},
   Key = {fds310916}
}

@misc{fds310917,
   Author = {Asher, SR and McDonald, KL},
   Title = {The behavioral basis of acceptance, rejection, and perceived
             popularity},
   Pages = {232-248},
   Booktitle = {The handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and
             groups},
   Publisher = {Gilford},
   Editor = {Rubin, KH and Bukowski, W and Laursen, B},
   Year = {2009},
   Key = {fds310917}
}

@misc{fds176145,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and MacEvoy, J.P. and McDonald, K.L.},
   Title = {Children's peer relations, social competence, and school
             adjustment: A social tasks and social goals
             perspectives.},
   Pages = {357-390},
   Booktitle = {Advances in Achievement and Motivation, Vol. 15: Social
             psychological perspectives},
   Publisher = {Bingley, UK: Emerald},
   Editor = {M.L. Maehr and S. Karabenick and T. Urdan},
   Year = {2008},
   Key = {fds176145}
}

@misc{fds310918,
   Author = {Asher, SR and MacEvoy, JP and McDonald, KL},
   Title = {Children’s peer relations, social competence, and school
             adjustment: A social tasks and social goals
             perspective},
   Volume = {15},
   Pages = {357-390},
   Booktitle = {Advances in achievement and motivation},
   Publisher = {Emerald},
   Editor = {Maehr, ML and Karabenick, S and Urdan, T},
   Year = {2008},
   Key = {fds310918}
}

@misc{fds39683,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Rose, A.J. and Gabriel, S.W.},
   Title = {Peer rejection in everyday life},
   Pages = {105-142},
   Booktitle = {Interpersonal rejection},
   Publisher = {New York: Oxford University Press},
   Editor = {M. Leary},
   Year = {2001},
   Key = {fds39683}
}

@misc{fds250583,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Rose, AJ and Gabriel, SW},
   Title = {Peer rejection in everyday life},
   Pages = {105-142},
   Booktitle = {Interpersonal Rejection},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
   Editor = {Leary, MR},
   Year = {2001},
   ISBN = {9780195130157},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130157.003.0005},
   Abstract = {© Oxford University Press, 2013. Rejection among children
             is the central topic in this part. Elementary-school
             students are the subjects of the comprehensive research,
             which discusses an extensive list of rejection behaviors
             that will unfold and explain aspects of early childhood
             development. Analysis was derived from the examination of
             ways (through the cited dialogues between students) in which
             to express non-affirmation. The study employed rating scales
             that measured the likelihood of a child to initiate a game
             with or engage in the same activity as another child.
             Results show that while there are children who are popularly
             admired in the campus, there are those who are generally
             hated by their peers. The second group of kids is termed as
             the sociometrically "rejected." Apparent reasons of dislike
             consist of social involvement, level of aggression,
             submissiveness tendencies, withdrawal instances, sense of
             humor, academic excellence, and athletic
             competitiveness.},
   Doi = {10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130157.003.0005},
   Key = {fds250583}
}

@misc{fds310919,
   Author = {Rudolph, KD and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Adaptation and maladaptation in the peer
             system},
   Series = {2nd Ed.},
   Pages = {157-175},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of developmental psychopathology},
   Publisher = {Plenum Press},
   Editor = {Sameroff, AJ and Lewis, M and Miller, SM},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds310919}
}

@misc{fds310920,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Rose, AJ},
   Title = {Children's friendships},
   Pages = {47-57},
   Booktitle = {Close relationships: A sourcebook},
   Publisher = {Sage},
   Editor = {Hendrick, C and Hendrick, SS},
   Year = {2000},
   Key = {fds310920}
}

@misc{fds310921,
   Author = {Parker, JG and Saxon, JL and Asher, SR and Kovacs,
             DM},
   Title = {Dimensions of children's friendship adjustment: Implications
             for understanding loneliness},
   Pages = {201-221},
   Booktitle = {Loneliness in childhood and adolescence},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Rotenberg, KJ and Hymel, S},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds310921}
}

@misc{fds310922,
   Author = {Troop, WP and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Teaching peer relationship competence in
             schools},
   Pages = {141-171},
   Booktitle = {Teaching in American schools},
   Publisher = {Merrill},
   Editor = {Stevens, RJ},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds310922}
}

@misc{fds310923,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Hopmeyer, A},
   Title = {Loneliness in childhood},
   Pages = {279-292},
   Booktitle = {Children’s needs II: Development, problems and
             alternatives},
   Publisher = {National Association of School Psychologists},
   Editor = {Bear, GG and Minke, KM and Thomas, A},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds310923}
}

@misc{fds310924,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Rose, AJ},
   Title = {Promoting children’s social-emotional development with
             peers},
   Pages = {196-224},
   Booktitle = {Emotional development and emotional literacy},
   Publisher = {Basic Books},
   Editor = {Salovry, P and Sluyter, D},
   Year = {1997},
   Key = {fds310924}
}

@misc{fds39619,
   Author = {Asher, S. R. and Parker, J. G. and Walker, D.
             L.},
   Title = {Distinguishing friendship from acceptance: Implications for
             intervention and assessment},
   Pages = {366-405},
   Booktitle = {The company they keep: Friendship during childhood and
             adolescence},
   Publisher = {New York: Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {W. M. Bukowski and A. F. Newcomb and W. W. Hartup},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds39619}
}

@misc{fds310925,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Parker, JG and Walker, DL},
   Title = {Distinguishing friendship from acceptance: Implications for
             intervention and assessment},
   Pages = {366-405},
   Booktitle = {The company they keep: Friendship during childhood and
             adolescence},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Bukowski, WM and Newcomb, AF and Hartup, WW},
   Year = {1996},
   Key = {fds310925}
}

@misc{fds310926,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Erdley, CA and Gabriel, S},
   Title = {Peer relations},
   Pages = {456-487},
   Booktitle = {Development through life: A handbook for
             clinicians},
   Publisher = {Blackwell},
   Editor = {Rutter, ML and Hay, DF},
   Year = {1994},
   Key = {fds310926}
}

@misc{fds38388,
   Author = {Williams, G.A. and Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Children without friends, Part 1: Their problems},
   Journal = {School-Age Connections},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {6(7)},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, College of
             Agriculture, Cooperative Extension Service},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds38388}
}

@misc{fds38389,
   Author = {Williams, G.A. and Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Children without friends, Part 2: The reasons for peer
             rejection},
   Journal = {School-Age Connections},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {5-6},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, College of
             Agriculture, Cooperative Extension Service},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds38389}
}

@misc{fds38391,
   Author = {Williams, G.A. and Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Children without friends, Part 4: Improving social
             skills},
   Journal = {School-Age Connections},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {3-4},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds38391}
}

@misc{fds38392,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Hopmeyer, A.},
   Title = {Peer relationship problems in childhood},
   Journal = {Communique},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {10},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds38392}
}

@misc{fds310927,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Gabriel, SW},
   Title = {Using a wireless transmission system to observe conversation
             and social interaction on the playground},
   Pages = {184-209},
   Booktitle = {Children on playgrounds},
   Publisher = {State University of New York Press},
   Editor = {Hart, CH},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds310927}
}

@misc{fds310928,
   Author = {Parker, JG and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Beyond group acceptance: Friendship and friendship quality
             as distinct dimensions of peer adjustment},
   Volume = {4},
   Pages = {261-294},
   Booktitle = {Advances in personal relationships},
   Publisher = {Kingsley},
   Editor = {Jones, WH and Perlman, D},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds310928}
}

@misc{fds310929,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Parkhurst, JT and Hymel, S and Williams,
             GA},
   Title = {Peer rejection and loneliness in childhood},
   Pages = {253-273},
   Booktitle = {Peer rejection in childhood},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Asher, SR and Coie, JD},
   Year = {1990},
   Key = {fds310929}
}

@misc{fds310930,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {Recent advances in the study of peer rejection},
   Pages = {3-14},
   Booktitle = {Peer rejection in childhood},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Asher, SR and Coie, JD},
   Year = {1990},
   Key = {fds310930}
}

@misc{fds310931,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Parker, JG},
   Title = {The significance of peer relationship problems in
             childhood},
   Pages = {5-23},
   Booktitle = {Social competence in developmental perspective},
   Publisher = {Academic Publishing},
   Editor = {Schneider, BH and Attili, G and Nadel, J and Weissberg,
             RP},
   Year = {1989},
   Key = {fds310931}
}

@misc{fds310932,
   Author = {Dodge, KA and Asher, SR and Parkhurst, JT},
   Title = {Social life as a goal coordination task},
   Volume = {3},
   Pages = {107-135},
   Booktitle = {Research on motivation in education},
   Publisher = {Academic Press},
   Editor = {Ames, C and Ames, R},
   Year = {1989},
   Key = {fds310932}
}

@misc{fds38387,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Parker, J.G.},
   Title = {Peer rejection: An indicator of risk},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {3, 8},
   Booktitle = {The Early Childhood Update},
   Year = {1988},
   Key = {fds38387}
}

@misc{fds310933,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {An evolving paradigm in social skill training research with
             children},
   Pages = {157-171},
   Booktitle = {Children's peer relations: Issues in assessment and
             intervention},
   Publisher = {Springer-Verlag},
   Editor = {Schneider, BH and Rubin, KH and Ledingham, JE},
   Year = {1985},
   Key = {fds310933}
}

@misc{fds310934,
   Author = {Parkhurst, JT and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Goals and concerns: Implications for the study of children's
             social competence},
   Volume = {8},
   Pages = {199-228},
   Booktitle = {Advances in clinical child psychology},
   Publisher = {Plenum},
   Editor = {Lahey, BB and Kazdin, AE},
   Year = {1985},
   Key = {fds310934}
}

@misc{fds310935,
   Author = {Ladd, GW and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Social skill training and children's peer
             relations},
   Pages = {219-244},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of social skills training and research},
   Publisher = {Wiley},
   Editor = {L'Abate, L and Milan, MA},
   Year = {1985},
   Key = {fds310935}
}

@misc{fds310936,
   Author = {Taylor, AR and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's goals and social competence: Individual
             differences in a game-playing context},
   Pages = {53-78},
   Booktitle = {Friendship in normal and handicapped children},
   Publisher = {Ablex},
   Editor = {Field, T and Roopnarine, JL and Segal, M},
   Year = {1984},
   Key = {fds310936}
}

@misc{fds310937,
   Author = {Wigfield, A and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Social and motivational influences on reading
             achievement},
   Pages = {423-452},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of reading research},
   Publisher = {Longman},
   Editor = {Pearson, PD and Barr, R and Kamil, M and Mosenthal,
             P},
   Year = {1984},
   Key = {fds310937}
}

@misc{fds38411,
   Author = {Wigfield, A. and Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Social and motivational influences on reading},
   Pages = {423-452},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of reading research},
   Publisher = {Longman},
   Address = {New York},
   Editor = {P. D. Pearson and R. Barr and M. Kamil and P. Mosenthal},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds38411}
}

@misc{fds205640,
   Author = {Wigfield, A. and Asher, S. R.},
   Title = {Social and motivational influences on reading
             achievement.},
   Pages = {423-452},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of reading research},
   Publisher = {Longman},
   Address = {New York},
   Editor = {P. D. Pearson and R. Barr and M. Kamil and P. Mosenthal},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds205640}
}

@misc{fds205641,
   Author = {Wigfield, A. and Asher, S. R.},
   Title = {Social and motivational influences on reading
             achievement.},
   Pages = {423-452},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of reading research},
   Publisher = {Longman},
   Address = {New York},
   Editor = {P. D. Pearson and R. Barr and M. Kamil and P. Mosenthal},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds205641}
}

@misc{fds310938,
   Author = {Parke, RD and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Social and personality development},
   Volume = {34},
   Pages = {465-509},
   Booktitle = {Annual review of psychology},
   Publisher = {Annual Reviews, Inc.},
   Editor = {Porter, L and Rosenweig, M},
   Year = {1983},
   Key = {fds310938}
}

@misc{fds310939,
   Author = {Renshaw, PD and Asher, SR},
   Title = {Social competence and peer status: The distinction between
             goals and strategies},
   Pages = {375-395},
   Booktitle = {Peer relationships and social skills in childhood},
   Publisher = {Springer-Verlag},
   Editor = {Rubin, KH and Ross, HS},
   Year = {1982},
   Key = {fds310939}
}

@misc{fds310940,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Renshaw, PD and Hymel, S},
   Title = {Peer relations and the development of social
             skills},
   Volume = {3},
   Pages = {137-158},
   Booktitle = {The young child: Reviews of research},
   Publisher = {National Association for the Education of Young
             Children},
   Editor = {Moore, SG and Cooper, CR},
   Year = {1982},
   Key = {fds310940}
}

@misc{fds176101,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Wigfield, A.},
   Title = {Training referential communication skills},
   Pages = {105-126},
   Booktitle = {Children's oral communication skills},
   Publisher = {New York: Academic Press},
   Editor = {W. P. Dickson},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds176101}
}

@misc{fds310942,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Hymel, S},
   Title = {Children's social competence and peer relations: Sociometric
             and behavioral assessment},
   Pages = {125-157},
   Booktitle = {Social competence},
   Publisher = {Guilford Press},
   Editor = {Wine, JD and Smye, MD},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds310942}
}

@misc{fds310943,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Renshaw, PD},
   Title = {Children without friends: Social knowledge and social skill
             training},
   Pages = {273-296},
   Booktitle = {The development of children's friendships},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Editor = {Asher, SR and Gottman, JM},
   Year = {1981},
   Key = {fds310943}
}

@misc{fds38410,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Wigfield, A.},
   Title = {Training referential communication skills},
   Series = {Technical Report No. 175},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1980},
   Key = {fds38410}
}

@misc{fds310944,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Wigfield, A},
   Title = {Topic interest and children's reading comprehension},
   Pages = {525-534},
   Booktitle = {Theoretical issues in reading comprehension},
   Publisher = {Erlbaum},
   Editor = {Spiro, RJ and Bruce, BC and Brewer, WF},
   Year = {1980},
   ISBN = {0898590361},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315107493-28},
   Abstract = {It is commonly believed that children’s willingness to
             read and their comprehension of what they read is affected
             by the interest level of the material they are given (e.g.,
             Estes and Vaughan, 1973; Fader and McNeil, 1968).
             Accordingly, considerable research has been devoted to the
             assessment of children’s interests and to comparisons of
             children’s interests with the content of children’s
             texts. What is surprising is that these lines of inquiry
             have led to little research on whether children’s interest
             in material does, in fact, influence their comprehension.
             Blom, Waite, and Zimet (1970), who have done the most
             extensive content analysis of children’s reading primers,
             have commented that what is needed is research into how
             content actually affects children’s attitudes and reading
             performance.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315107493-28},
   Key = {fds310944}
}

@misc{fds38409,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Wigfield, A.},
   Title = {Influence of comparison training on children's referential
             communication},
   Series = {Technical Report No. 139},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1979},
   Key = {fds38409}
}

@misc{fds310945,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {Referential communication},
   Pages = {175-197},
   Booktitle = {The functions of language and cognition},
   Publisher = {Academic Press},
   Editor = {Whitehurst, GJ and Zimmerman, BJ},
   Year = {1979},
   Key = {fds310945}
}

@misc{fds38396,
   Author = {Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Referential communication},
   Series = {Technical Report No. 90},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1978},
   Key = {fds38396}
}

@misc{fds38407,
   Author = {Wigfield, A. and Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Age differences in children's communication accuracy: The
             effects of task familiarity},
   Series = {Technical Report No. 96},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1978},
   Key = {fds38407}
}

@misc{fds38408,
   Author = {Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Influence of topic interest on black children's and white
             children's reading comprehension},
   Series = {Technical Report No. 99},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1978},
   Key = {fds38408}
}

@misc{fds310596,
   Author = {Asher, SR},
   Title = {Children's peer relations},
   Pages = {91-113},
   Booktitle = {Social and personality development},
   Publisher = {Holt, Rinehart & Winston},
   Editor = {Lamb, ME},
   Year = {1978},
   Key = {fds310596}
}

@misc{fds38395,
   Author = {Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Sex differences in reading achievement},
   Journal = {Reading Education Report No. 2},
   Series = {Reading Education Report No. 2},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1977},
   Key = {fds38395}
}

@misc{fds310597,
   Author = {Asher, SR and Oden, SL and Gottman, JM},
   Title = {Children's friendships in school settings},
   Volume = {1},
   Pages = {33-61},
   Booktitle = {Current topics in early childhood education},
   Publisher = {Ablex},
   Editor = {Katz, LG},
   Year = {1977},
   Key = {fds310597}
}

@misc{fds38394,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and Hymel, S. and Wigfield, A.},
   Title = {Children's comprehension of high- and low-interest material
             and a comparison of two cloze scoring methods},
   Journal = {Technical Report No. 17},
   Series = {Technical Report No. 17},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois, Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of
             Reading},
   Year = {1976},
   Key = {fds38394}
}


%% Contributions to Professional Newsletters   
@misc{fds53115,
   Author = {Asher, S.R. and McDonald, K.L.},
   Title = {Intervening to promote friendship: Experimental tests of
             hypotheses about fundamental skills and processes. A
             commentary on “Research on Friendship among Children and
             Adolescents: Findings, Problems, and Future Directions”
             (Joan G. Miller and Xinyin Chen, Eds.)},
   Journal = {Special Section of the International Society for the Study
             of Behavioral Development Newletter},
   Series = {Number 2, Serial No. 46},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds53115}
}

@misc{fds175791,
   Author = {Willilams, G.A. and Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Children without friends, Part 3: Learning about a child's
             strengths and weaknesses},
   Journal = {School-Age Connections},
   Volume = {5},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {4-5},
   Publisher = {University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, College of
             Agriculture, Cooperative Extension Service},
   Year = {1993},
   Key = {fds175791}
}


%% Book Reviews   
@article{fds38325,
   Author = {Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Review of Social competence. symptoms. and under-achievement
             in childhood: A longitudinal perspective, by Martin
             Kohn},
   Journal = {Child Development Abstracts and Bibliography},
   Volume = {53},
   Pages = {72-74},
   Year = {1979},
   Key = {fds38325}
}

@article{fds38311,
   Author = {Asher, S.R.},
   Title = {Review of "Reach, Touch, and Teach: Student Concerns and
             Process Education," by Terry Borton},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Education},
   Volume = {39},
   Pages = {21},
   Year = {1971},
   Key = {fds38311}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Postdocs * Reload * Login