Psychology and Neuroscience Faculty Database
Psychology and Neuroscience
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > pn > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Elizabeth J. Marsh    :chronological  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Journal Articles   
@article{fds371649,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Huang, S and Marsh, EJ and Kay, AC},
   Title = {The Role of Structure-Seeking in Moral Punishment},
   Journal = {Social Justice Research},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {410-431},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11211-023-00416-8},
   Abstract = {Four studies (total N = 1586) test the notion that people
             are motivated to punish moral rule violators because
             punishment offers a way to obtain structure and order in the
             world. First, in a correlational study, increased need for
             structure was associated with the stronger endorsement
             punishment for moral rule violators. This relationship
             between need for structure and punishment was not driven by
             political conservatism. Three experimental studies then
             tested, and corroborated, our main causal hypotheses: that
             threats to structure increase punitive judgments for moral
             rule violators (i.e., a compensatory mechanism; Study 2) and
             that a lack of punishment for wrongdoing (relative to
             punishment for wrongdoing) makes the world seem less
             structured in the moment (Studies 3 and 4). We compare and
             contrast our structure-based account of moral punishment to
             other theories and findings across the punishment
             literature.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11211-023-00416-8},
   Key = {fds371649}
}

@article{fds371744,
   Author = {Taylor, MK and Marsh, EJ and Samanez-Larkin, GR},
   Title = {Heuristic decision-making across adulthood.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {508-518},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000726},
   Abstract = {In general, research on aging and decision-making has grown
             in recent years. Yet, little work has investigated how
             reliance on classic heuristics may differ across adulthood.
             For example, younger adults rely on the availability of
             information from memory when judging the relative frequency
             of plane crashes versus car accidents, but it is unclear if
             older adults are similarly reliant on this heuristic. In the
             present study, participants aged 20-90 years old made
             judgments that could be answered by relying on five
             different heuristics: anchoring, availability, recognition,
             representativeness, and sunk-cost bias. We found no evidence
             of age-related differences in the use of the classic
             heuristics-younger and older adults employed anchoring,
             availability, recognition, and representativeness to equal
             degrees in order to make decisions. However, replicating
             past work, we found age-related differences in the sunk-cost
             bias-older adults were more likely to avoid this fallacy
             compared to younger adults. We explain these different
             patterns by drawing on the distinctive roles that stored
             knowledge and personal experience likely play across
             heuristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000726},
   Key = {fds371744}
}

@article{fds370236,
   Author = {Eliseev, ED and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Understanding why searching the internet inflates confidence
             in explanatory ability},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {711-720},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.4058},
   Abstract = {People rely on the internet for easy access to information,
             setting up potential confusion about the boundaries between
             an individual's knowledge and the information they find
             online. Across four experiments, we replicated and extended
             past work showing that online searching inflates people's
             confidence in their knowledge. Participants who searched the
             internet for explanations rated their explanatory ability
             higher than participants who read but did not search for the
             same explanations. Two experiments showed that extraneous
             web page content (pictures) does not drive this effect. The
             last experiment modeled how search engines yield results;
             participants saw (but did not search for) a list of hits,
             which included “snippets” that previewed web page
             content, before reading the explanations. Participants in
             this condition were as confident as participants who
             searched online. Previewing hits primes to-be-read content,
             in a modern-day equivalent of Titchener's famous example of
             a brief glance eliciting false feelings of
             familiarity.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.4058},
   Key = {fds370236}
}

@article{fds371568,
   Author = {Stone, AR and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Belief in COVID-19 misinformation: Hopeful claims are rated
             as truer},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {37},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {399-408},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.4042},
   Abstract = {Misinformation surrounding COVID-19 spread rapidly and
             widely, posing a significant threat to public health. Here,
             we examined whether some types of misinformation are more
             believable than others, to the extent that they offer people
             hope in uncertain times. An initial group of subjects rated
             a series of COVID-19 misinformation statements for whether
             each made them feel more or less hopeful (if true). Based on
             these ratings, we selected two sets of misinformation that
             differed in their average rated hopefulness; the two sets
             did not differ in word length or reading ease. In two
             studies, people rated their belief in each statement.
             Results from both studies revealed that people rated the
             more hopeful misinformation (e.g., COVID cures and
             prevention methods) as truer than less hopeful
             misinformation (e.g., transmission vectors). These findings
             are consistent with a motivated reasoning account of
             misinformation acceptance.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.4042},
   Key = {fds371568}
}

@article{fds363189,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Whitehead, PS and Marsh, EJ and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Prior exposure increases judged truth even during periods of
             mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1997-2007},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02101-4},
   Abstract = {Much of our day is spent mind-wandering-periods of
             inattention characterized by a lack of awareness of external
             stimuli and information. Whether we are paying attention or
             not, information surrounds us constantly-some true and some
             false. The proliferation of false information in news and
             social media highlights the critical need to understand the
             psychological mechanisms underlying our beliefs about what
             is true. People often rely on heuristics to judge the truth
             of information. For example, repeated information is more
             likely to be judged as true than new information (i.e., the
             illusory truth effect). However, despite the prevalence of
             mind wandering in our daily lives, current research on the
             contributing factors to the illusory truth effect have
             largely ignored periods of inattention as experimentally
             informative. Here, we aim to address this gap in our
             knowledge, investigating whether mind wandering during
             initial exposure to information has an effect on later
             belief in the truth of that information. That is, does the
             illusory truth effect occur even when people report not
             paying attention to the information at hand. Across three
             studies we demonstrate that even during periods of mind
             wandering, the repetition of information increases truth
             judgments. Further, our results suggest that the severity of
             mind wandering moderated truth ratings, such that greater
             levels of mind wandering decreased truth judgements for
             previously presented information.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-022-02101-4},
   Key = {fds363189}
}

@article{fds361194,
   Author = {Taylor, MK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Predicting others' knowledge in younger and older
             adulthood.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {943-953},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-02036-2},
   Abstract = {Our beliefs about aging affect how we interact with others.
             For example, people know that episodic memory declines with
             age, and as a result, older adults' memories are less likely
             to be trusted. However, not all aspects of remembering
             decline with age; semantic memory (knowledge) increases
             across adulthood and is relatively unaffected in healthy
             aging. In the current work, we examined people's awareness
             of this pattern. Participants estimated the knowledge of
             hypothetical younger and older adults; in some studies, they
             also predicted and demonstrated their own knowledge on the
             same measures. Across studies, both younger and older adults
             estimated that older adults would perform better on a
             knowledge test, demonstrating awareness that knowledge is
             not impaired with aging. Furthermore, people's beliefs about
             their own knowledge influenced the predictions they made
             about others' knowledge. We discuss how this work informs
             theories of metacognition and contributes to positive
             self-perceptions in older adulthood.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-021-02036-2},
   Key = {fds361194}
}

@article{fds362549,
   Author = {Whitehead, PS and Zamary, A and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Transfer of category learning to impoverished
             contexts.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {1035-1044},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-02031-7},
   Abstract = {Learning often happens in ideal conditions, but then must be
             applied in less-than-ideal conditions - such as when a
             learner studies clearly illustrated examples of rocks in a
             book but then must identify them in a muddy field. Here we
             examine whether the benefits of interleaving (vs. blocking)
             study schedules, as well as the use of feature descriptions,
             supports the transfer of category learning in new,
             impoverished contexts. Specifically, keeping the study
             conditions constant, we evaluated learners' ability to
             classify new exemplars in the same neutral context versus in
             impoverished contexts in which certain stimulus features are
             occluded. Over two experiments, we demonstrate that
             performance in new, impoverished contexts during test is
             greater for participants who received an interleaved (vs.
             blocked) study schedule, both for novel and for studied
             exemplars. Additionally, we show that this benefit extends
             to both a short (3-min) or long (48-h) test delay. The
             presence of feature descriptions during learning had no
             impact on transfer. Together, these results extend the
             growing literature investigating how changes in context
             during category learning or test impacts performance and
             provide support for the use of interleaving to promote the
             far transfer of category knowledge to impoverished
             contexts.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-021-02031-7},
   Key = {fds362549}
}

@article{fds360002,
   Author = {Yang, BW and Deffler, SA and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {A comparison of memories of fiction and autobiographical
             memories.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. General},
   Volume = {151},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1089-1106},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001125},
   Abstract = {People consume, remember, and discuss not only memories of
             lived experiences, but also events from works of fiction,
             such as books, movies, and TV shows. We argue that these
             <i>memories of fiction</i> represent an important category
             of event memory, best understood within an autobiographical
             memory framework. How do fictional events yield
             psychological realities even when they are known to be
             invented? We explored this question in three studies by
             comparing the memory content, phenomenological qualities,
             and functional roles of naturally occurring personal
             memories to memories of fiction. In Studies 1 and 2, we
             characterized the subjective experience of memories of
             fiction by adapting established measures of autobiographical
             remembering, such as the Autobiographical Memory
             Questionnaire (Rubin et al., 2003), Centrality of Event
             Scale (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006), and items from the Thinking
             About Life Experiences Scale (Bluck et al., 2005; Pillemer
             et al., 2015). In Study 3, we investigated similarities and
             differences in personal memories and memories of fiction for
             events from childhood or the recent past. In doing so, we
             observed the impact of a unique property of memories of
             fiction: their ability to be repeatedly reexperienced in
             their original form. Taken together, we argue that memories
             of fiction can be considered similar to other forms of
             autobiographical remembering and describe a theoretical
             framework for understanding memories of fiction in the
             context of other event memories. (PsycInfo Database Record
             (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xge0001125},
   Key = {fds360002}
}

@article{fds362981,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Whitehead, PS and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {The cognitive processes underlying false
             beliefs},
   Journal = {Journal of Consumer Psychology},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {359-369},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1289},
   Abstract = {Why do consumers sometimes fall for spurious claims—for
             example, brain training games that prevent cognitive
             decline, toning sneakers that sculpt one's body, flower
             essence that cures depression—and how can consumers
             protect themselves in the modern world where information is
             shared quickly and easily? As cognitive scientists, we view
             this problem through the lens of what we know, more
             generally, about how people evaluate information for its
             veracity, and how people update their beliefs. That is, the
             same processes that support true belief can also encourage
             people to sometimes believe misleading or false information.
             Anchoring on the large literature on truth and belief
             updating allows predictions about consumer behavior; it also
             highlights possible solutions while casting doubt on other
             possible responses to misleading communications.},
   Doi = {10.1002/jcpy.1289},
   Key = {fds362981}
}

@article{fds359784,
   Author = {McDaniel, MA and Marsh, EJ and Gouravajhala, R},
   Title = {Individual Differences in Structure Building: Impacts on
             Comprehension and Learning, Theoretical Underpinnings, and
             Support for Less Able Structure Builders.},
   Journal = {Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the
             Association for Psychological Science},
   Volume = {17},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {385-406},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/17456916211000716},
   Abstract = {In this article, we highlight an underappreciated individual
             difference: structure building. Structure building is
             integral to many everyday activities and involves creating
             coherent mental representations of conversations, texts,
             pictorial stories, and other events. People vary in this
             ability in a way not generally captured by other better
             known concepts and individual difference measures.
             Individuals with lower structure-building ability
             consistently perform worse on a range of comprehension and
             learning measures than do individuals with higher
             structure-building ability, both in the laboratory and in
             the classroom. Problems include a range of comprehension
             processes, including encoding factual content, inhibiting
             irrelevant information, and constructing a cohesive
             situation model of a text or conversation. Despite these
             problems, recent research is encouraging in that techniques
             to improve the learning outcomes for low-ability structure
             builders have been identified. We argue that the accumulated
             research warrants the recognition of structure building as
             an important individual difference in cognitive functioning
             and that additional theoretical work is needed to understand
             the underpinnings of structure-building deficits.},
   Doi = {10.1177/17456916211000716},
   Key = {fds359784}
}

@article{fds366196,
   Author = {Yang, BW and Stone, AR and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Asymmetry in belief revision},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1072-1082},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3991},
   Abstract = {Information can change: science advances, newspapers retract
             claims, and reccomendations shift. Successfully navigating
             the world requires updating and changing beliefs, a process
             that is sensitive to a person's motivation to change their
             beliefs as well as the credibility of the source providing
             the new information. Here, we report three studies that
             consistently identify an additional factor influencing
             belief revision. Specifically, we document an asymmetry in
             belief revision: people are better able to believe in a
             claim once thought to be false, as opposed to unbelieving
             something once believed to be true. We discuss how this
             finding integrates and extends prior research on social and
             cognitive contributions to belief revisions. This work has
             implications for understanding the widespread prevalence and
             persistence of false beliefs in contemporary
             societies.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.3991},
   Key = {fds366196}
}

@article{fds369704,
   Author = {Whitehead, PS and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Reforming the Seven Sins of Memory to Emphasize Interactions
             and Adaptiveness},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {11},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {482-484},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/mac0000093},
   Abstract = {Memory errors can take many forms: forgetting an ice cream
             container in the back of a hot car, recalling an accident in
             a way that absolves one of culpability, or believing that
             election misinformation is true, among many others. Much
             research seeks to understand such errors. They provide the
             basic scientist with windows into understanding how memory
             works and have implications in a myriad of real-world
             domains including but not limited to eyewitness testimony,
             advertising, education, and the proliferation of political
             misinformation (Schacter, 2022b; see also Baddeley et al.,
             2002; Dunlosky et al., 2013; Loftus, 1979). In an effort to
             gain traction on such errors, the review by Schacter (2022a)
             builds on prior work (Schacter, 1999, 2022a, 2022b) that
             classifies memory errors into the seven sins of memory: the
             sins of commission include misattribution (incorrectly
             remembering the source of a memory), bias (knowledge or
             beliefs shaping memory of the past), suggestibility
             (misleading suggestions leading to memory errors or false
             memories), and persistence (the retrieval of aversive
             memories), aswell as the sins of omission such as transience
             (forgetting information over time), absentmindedness (lack
             of attention leading to forgetting), and blocking (failure
             to retrieve information stored in memory). This taxonomy
             serves several important functions: it emphasizes that there
             is more than one kind of memory error and highlights
             errors’ similarities and differences; it offers a
             convenient way of talking about memory errors (for both
             scientists and the general public); it also coins catchy
             labels that attract attention to the science of memory
             errors. While taxonomies are powerful because they simplify,
             this should not be at the cost of understanding the complex
             cognitive processes that underlie these memory sins.
             Therefore, 2 decades after the original publication of the
             “Seven Sins of Memory” (Schacter, 1999), we believe this
             taxonomy should more explicitly reflect two things, neither
             of which we think is particularly controversial: first, that
             many memory errors reflect a confluence of sins, and second,
             that it is time to more enthusiastically embrace a “cup
             half-full” approach, emphasizing the adaptive nature of
             memory},
   Doi = {10.1037/mac0000093},
   Key = {fds369704}
}

@article{fds359232,
   Author = {Eliseev, ED and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Externalizing autobiographical memories in the digital
             age.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {1072-1081},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.08.005},
   Abstract = {People externalize their autobiographical memories by
             creating representations that exist outside of their minds.
             Externalizations often serve personal and social functions,
             consistent with theorized functions of autobiographical
             memory. With new digital technologies, people are
             documenting more memories than ever and are sharing them
             with larger audiences. However, these technologies do not
             change the core cognitive processes involved in
             autobiographical memory, but instead present novel
             situations that affect how these processes are deployed.
             Smartphones allow events to be recorded as they unfold, thus
             directing attention and sometimes impairing memory. Social
             media increase the frequency of reviewing and sharing
             records which reactivate memories, potentially strengthening
             or updating them. Overall, externalization in the digital
             age changes what people attend to and remember about their
             own experiences.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2021.08.005},
   Key = {fds359232}
}

@article{fds366033,
   Author = {Yang, B and Deffler, SA and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {A Comparison of Memories of Fiction and Autobiographical
             Memories},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/58kpb},
   Abstract = {<p>People consume, remember, and discuss not only memories
             of lived experiences, but also events from works of fiction,
             such as books, movies, and television shows. We argue that
             these memories of fiction represent an important category of
             event memory, best understood within an autobiographical
             memory framework. How do fictional events yield
             psychological realities even when they are known to be
             invented? We explored this question in three studies by
             comparing the memory content, phenomenological qualities,
             and functional roles of naturally occurring personal
             memories to memories of fiction. In Studies 1 and 2, we
             characterized the subjective experience of memories of
             fiction by adapting established measures of autobiographical
             remembering, such as the Autobiographical Memory
             Questionnaire (Rubin et al., 2003), Centrality of Event
             Scale (Berntsen &amp;amp; Rubin, 2006), and items from the
             Thinking About Life Experiences scale (Bluck et al., 2005;
             Pillemer et al., 2015). In Study 3, we investigated
             similarities and differences in personal memories and
             memories of fiction for events from childhood or the recent
             past. In doing so, we observed the impact of a unique
             property of memories of fiction: their ability to be
             repeatedly re-experienced in their original form. Taken
             together, we argue that memories of fiction can be
             considered similar to other forms of autobiographical
             remembering and describe a theoretical framework for
             understanding memories of fiction in the context of other
             event memories.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/58kpb},
   Key = {fds366033}
}

@article{fds358039,
   Author = {Yang, BW and Vargas Restrepo and C and Stanley, ML and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Truncating Bar Graphs Persistently Misleads
             Viewers},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {298-311},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.10.002},
   Abstract = {Data visualizations and graphs are increasingly common in
             both scientific and mass media settings. While graphs are
             useful tools for communicating patterns in data, they also
             have the potential to mislead viewers. In five studies, we
             provide empirical evidence that y-axis truncation leads
             viewers to perceive illustrated differences as larger (i.e.,
             a truncation effect). This effect persisted after viewers
             were taught about the effects of y-axis truncation and was
             robust across participants, with 83.5% of participants
             across these 5 studies showing a truncation effect. We also
             found that individual differences in graph literacy failed
             to predict the size of individuals’ truncation effects.
             PhD students in both quantitative fields and the humanities
             were susceptible to the truncation effect, but quantitative
             PhD students were slightly more resistant when no warning
             about truncated axes was provided. We discuss the
             implications of these results for the underlying mechanisms
             and make practical recommendations for training critical
             consumers and creators of graphs.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.10.002},
   Key = {fds358039}
}

@article{fds358037,
   Author = {Datta, N and Bidopia, T and Datta, S and Mittal, G and Alphin, F and Herbert, BM and Marsh, EJ and Fitzsimons, GJ and Strauman, TJ and Zucker, NL},
   Title = {Internal states and interoception along a spectrum of eating
             disorder symptomology.},
   Journal = {Physiol Behav},
   Volume = {230},
   Pages = {113307},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113307},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: Recent studies on atypical interoceptive
             capabilities have focused on clinical populations, including
             anorexia nervosa[1,2]. The present exploratory study aims to
             characterize the influence of disordered eating symptomology
             on interoceptive capabilities in college students, a
             population for which dangerous dieting behaviors may emerge.
             METHOD: Ninety-nine participants were randomized to consume
             a blinded high calorie or low calorie midday shake.
             Participants reported frequency of eating disorder
             cognitions and behaviors; indicated changes in satiety,
             happiness, and energy pre- and post-consumption; and guessed
             the calories in their shake. Outcomes (perceived satiety,
             changes in mood, and caloric guess) were regressed on eating
             disorder symptoms scores, the high/low calorie shake
             condition, and the interaction between these predictors.
             RESULTS: Those randomized to receive the high calorie shake
             reported feeling fuller, but only when endorsing lower
             levels of eating concern. Those randomized to the high
             calorie shake reported greater post-meal happiness, but only
             at greater levels of eating concerns. Lastly, those with
             lower levels of eating restraint reported an expected
             positive association between level of fullness and calorie
             guess, but those with higher levels of eating restraint did
             not exhibit any relationship between perceived fullness and
             calorie guess. DISCUSSION: Results of this exploratory
             suggest that irregular eating habits (e.g., not eating a
             sufficient amount for lunch) may have direct consequences on
             interoceptive capabilities. Further, these capacities may be
             impacted by individual differences in eating concern and
             restraint. Preliminary findings suggest that impairment in
             deciphering visceral signals may be associated with the
             degree of eating disorder symptomology; such impairment may
             occur at lower levels of symptomatology than normative data
             would indicate.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113307},
   Key = {fds358037}
}

@article{fds358038,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Taylor, MK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Cultural Identity Changes the Accessibility of
             Knowledge},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {44-54},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.07.008},
   Abstract = {Culture plays a significant role in determining what people
             believe and claim to know. Here, we argue that, in addition
             to shaping what people come to know, culture influences the
             accessibility of that knowledge. In five studies, we
             examined how activating participants’ American identities
             (a cultural identity) influenced their ability to retrieve
             well-known information: the 50 U. S. states. Activating
             participants’ American identities—relative to other
             identities—led them to retrieve more U. S. states over
             brief periods of time; the effect disappeared over longer
             periods of time. Overall, our results suggest that the
             identity activated affects the speed with which relevant
             knowledge is retrieved, but that the effect is not large in
             magnitude (perhaps contributing to why we did not find the
             effect in Study 4). This work provides the first evidence
             that cultural identity influences not only what one knows
             but also its accessibility.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.07.008},
   Key = {fds358038}
}

@article{fds354158,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Stone, AR and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Cheaters claim they knew the answers all
             along.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {341-350},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01812-w},
   Abstract = {Cheating has become commonplace in academia and beyond. Yet,
             almost everyone views themselves favorably, believing that
             they are honest, trustworthy, and of high integrity. We
             investigate one possible explanation for this apparent
             discrepancy between people's actions and their favorable
             self-concepts: People who cheat on tests believe that they
             knew the answers all along. We found consistent
             correlational evidence across three studies that, for those
             particular cases in which participants likely cheated, they
             were more likely to report that they knew the answers all
             along. Experimentally, we then found that participants were
             more likely to later claim that they knew the answers all
             along after having the opportunity to cheat to find the
             correct answers - relative to exposure to the correct
             answers without the opportunity to cheat. These findings
             provide new insights into relationships between memory,
             metacognition, and the self-concept.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-020-01812-w},
   Key = {fds354158}
}

@article{fds355823,
   Author = {Arnold, KM and Eliseev, ED and Stone, AR and McDaniel, MA and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Two routes to the same place: learning from quick
             closed-book essays versus open-book essays},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {33},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {229-246},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2021.1903011},
   Abstract = {Knowing when and how to most effectively use writing as a
             learning tool requires understanding the cognitive processes
             driving learning. Writing is a generative activity that
             often requires students to elaborate upon and organise
             information. Here we examine what happens when a standard
             short writing task is (or is not) combined with a known
             mnemonic, retrieval practice. In two studies, we compared
             learning from writing short open-book versus closed-book
             essays. Despite closed-book essays being shorter and taking
             less time, students learned just as much as from writing
             longer and more time intensive open-book essays. These
             results differ from students’ own perceptions that they
             learned more from writing open-book essays. Analyses of the
             essays themselves suggested a trade-off in cognitive
             processes; closed-book essays required the retrieval of
             information but resulted in lower quality essays as judged
             by naïve readers. Implications for educational practice and
             possible roles for individual differences are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1080/20445911.2021.1903011},
   Key = {fds355823}
}

@article{fds354151,
   Author = {Datta, N and Bidopia, T and Datta, S and Mittal, G and Alphin, F and Marsh,
             EJ and Fitzsimons, GJ and Strauman, TJ and Zucker,
             NL},
   Title = {Meal skipping and cognition along a spectrum of restrictive
             eating.},
   Journal = {Eat Behav},
   Volume = {39},
   Pages = {101431},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2020.101431},
   Abstract = {OBJECTIVE: Inadequate nutrition adversely impacts brain
             development and cognitive functioning (Pollitt et al.,
             1983). Studies examining the acute impact of eating regular
             meals on cognition have reported inconsistent findings,
             necessitating the exploration of individual differences in
             samples contributing to equivocal results. The present study
             examines the impact of skipping lunch on cognitive ability
             in college-aged students by including eating restraint as a
             moderator. METHODS: Participants were 99 college-aged
             students (M = 19.7 years, SD = 1.5) randomized to a
             blinded 'lunch' or 'lunch-omission' condition, and assessed
             on memory, attention, processing speed, set shifting, and
             eating disorder symptomology. RESULTS: Regressing long and
             short-term memory on the lunch manipulation, eating
             restraint scores, and their interaction revealed significant
             interactions: those who had lunch had superior memory
             performance, but only for those reporting lower levels of
             eating restraint. Regressing set shifting speed on the
             manipulation, those who had lunch had slower set shifting
             speed than those who skipped, but only for those reporting
             lower levels of eating restraint. CONCLUSIONS: Results
             suggest that skipping lunch may have immediate consequences
             on cognition, however, cognitive enhancing effects may be
             diminished in the presence of even low levels of eating
             restraint. Findings highlight the significance of purported
             subclinical levels of eating restraint and may inform health
             education strategies.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.eatbeh.2020.101431},
   Key = {fds354151}
}

@article{fds354257,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Marsh, EJ and Kay, AC},
   Title = {Structure-seeking as a psychological antecedent of beliefs
             about morality.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. General},
   Volume = {149},
   Number = {10},
   Pages = {1908-1918},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000752},
   Abstract = {People differ in their beliefs about the objectivity of
             moral claims. We investigated a possible psychological
             antecedent that might be associated with people's beliefs
             about the objectivity of moral claims. More specifically, we
             examined the relationship between the endorsement of moral
             objectivism and one's need to see the world as structured,
             ordered, and predictable. By believing that the world
             comprises objective facts about morality, a simple, rigid,
             and unambiguous structure is imposed on the moral landscape
             that is invariant to the whims and preferences of any
             particular person or group. Our results suggest that those
             more in need of personal structure and order in their lives
             are indeed more likely to endorse moral objectivism. We
             discuss the implications of these results for psychological
             theories of control and structure-seeking, and for
             cooperation, prosociality, social orderliness, and social
             goal pursuit. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all
             rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xge0000752},
   Key = {fds354257}
}

@article{fds349927,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Black-Maier, AC and Campbell, K and Marsh, EJ and Persky,
             AM},
   Title = {Regaining access to marginal knowledge in a classroom
             setting},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1005-1012},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3679},
   Abstract = {Students learn large amounts of information, but not all of
             it is remembered after courses end – meaning that valuable
             class time is often spent reviewing background material.
             Crucially, laboratory research suggests different strategies
             will be effective when reactivating previously learned
             information (i.e. marginal knowledge), as opposed to
             learning new information. In two experiments, we evaluated
             whether these laboratory results translated to the
             classroom. Topics from prior courses were tested to document
             which information students could no longer retrieve. Half
             were assigned to a not-tested control and half to the
             intervention; for these topics, students answered
             multiple-choice questions (without feedback) that gave them
             the chance to recognize the information they had failed to
             retrieve. Weeks later, students completed a final assessment
             on all topics. Crucially, multiple-choice testing increased
             the retrieval of previously forgotten information, providing
             the first classroom demonstration of the reactivation of
             marginal knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.3679},
   Key = {fds349927}
}

@article{fds349628,
   Author = {De Brigard and F and Gessell, B and Yang, BW and Stewart, G and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Remembering possible times: Memory for details of past,
             future, and counterfactual simulations.},
   Journal = {Psychology of Consciousness: Theory Research, and
             Practice},
   Volume = {7},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {331-339},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000220},
   Abstract = {People’s capacity to mentally simulate future events
             (episodic future thinking) as well as what could have
             occurred in the past but did not (episodic counterfactual
             thinking) critically depends on their capacity to retrieve
             episodic memories. All 3 mental simulations are likely
             adaptive in that they involve rehearsing possible scenarios
             with the goal of improving future performance. However, the
             extent to which these mental simulations are useful at a
             later time depends on how well they are later remembered.
             Unfortunately, little is known about how such simulations
             are remembered. In the current study, we explored this issue
             by asking participants to retrieve episodic memories and
             generate future and counterfactual simulations in response
             to 4 cues: particular places, people, objects, and times. A
             day later participants received 3 of the 4 cues and were
             asked to recall the remaining 1. Our results indicate that
             people and locations are equally well remembered, regardless
             of the temporal orientation of the mental simulation. In
             contrast, objects in future simulations are recalled less
             frequently than are those in memories. Time was poorly
             remembered across conditions but especially when remembering
             a future or a counterfactual simulation. In light of these
             results, we discuss how temporal information may be
             incorporated into our hypothetical episodic simulations.
             (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights
             reserved)},
   Doi = {10.1037/cns0000220},
   Key = {fds349628}
}

@article{fds345812,
   Author = {Brashier, NM and Eliseev, ED and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {An initial accuracy focus prevents illusory
             truth.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {194},
   Pages = {104054},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104054},
   Abstract = {News stories, advertising campaigns, and political
             propaganda often repeat misleading claims, increasing their
             persuasive power. Repeated statements feel easier to
             process, and thus truer, than new ones. Surprisingly, this
             illusory truth effect occurs even when claims contradict
             young adults' stored knowledge (e.g., repeating The fastest
             land animal is the leopard makes it more believable). In
             four experiments, we tackled this problem by prompting
             people to behave like "fact checkers." Focusing on accuracy
             at exposure (giving initial truth ratings) wiped out the
             illusion later, but only when participants held relevant
             knowledge. This selective benefit persisted over a delay.
             Our findings inform theories of how people evaluate truth
             and suggest practical strategies for coping in a "post-truth
             world."},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104054},
   Key = {fds345812}
}

@article{fds348415,
   Author = {Brashier, NM and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Judging Truth.},
   Journal = {Annual review of psychology},
   Volume = {71},
   Pages = {499-515},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050807},
   Abstract = {Deceptive claims surround us, embedded in fake news,
             advertisements, political propaganda, and rumors. How do
             people know what to believe? Truth judgments reflect
             inferences drawn from three types of information: base
             rates, feelings, and consistency with information retrieved
             from memory. First, people exhibit a bias to accept incoming
             information, because most claims in our environments are
             true. Second, people interpret feelings, like ease of
             processing, as evidence of truth. And third, people can (but
             do not always) consider whether assertions match facts and
             source information stored in memory. This three-part
             framework predicts specific illusions (e.g., truthiness,
             illusory truth), offers ways to correct stubborn
             misconceptions, and suggests the importance of converging
             cues in a post-truth world, where falsehoods travel further
             and faster than the truth.},
   Doi = {10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050807},
   Key = {fds348415}
}

@article{fds366034,
   Author = {Yang, B and Restrepo, CV and Stanley, M},
   Title = {Truncating Bar Graphs Persistently Misleads
             Viewers},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7aq4h},
   Abstract = {<p>In press: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.10.002
             Data visualizations and graphs are increasingly common in
             both scientific and mass media settings. While graphs are
             useful tools for communicating patterns in data, they also
             have the potential to mislead viewers. In five studies, we
             provide empirical evidence that y-axis truncation leads
             viewers to perceive illustrated differences as larger (i.e.,
             a truncation effect). This effect persisted after viewers
             were taught about the effects of y-axis truncation and was
             robust across participants, with 83.5% of participants
             across all 5 studies showing a truncation effect. We also
             found that individual differences in graph literacy failed
             to predict the size of individuals’ truncation effects.
             PhD students in both quantitative fields and the humanities
             were susceptible to the truncation effect, but quantitative
             PhD students were slightly more resistant when no warning
             about truncated axes was provided. We discuss the
             implications of these results for the underlying mechanisms
             and make practical recommendations for training critical
             consumers and creators of graphs.</p>},
   Doi = {10.31234/osf.io/7aq4h},
   Key = {fds366034}
}

@article{fds341737,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Retrieval-Based Learning in Children},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {111-116},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721418806673},
   Abstract = {Testing oneself with flash cards, using a clicker to respond
             to a teacher’s questions, and teaching another student are
             all effective ways to learn information. These learning
             strategies work, in part, because they require the retrieval
             of information from memory, a process known to enhance later
             memory. However, little research has directly examined
             retrieval-based learning in children. We review the emerging
             literature on the benefits of retrieval-based learning for
             preschool and elementary school students and draw on other
             literatures for further insights. We reveal clear evidence
             for the benefits of retrieval-based learning in children
             (starting in infancy). However, we know little about the
             developmental trajectory. Overall, the benefits are largest
             when the initial retrieval practice is effortful but
             successful.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0963721418806673},
   Key = {fds341737}
}

@article{fds341736,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Rajaram, S},
   Title = {The Digital Expansion of the Mind: Implications of Internet
             Usage for Memory and Cognition},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-14},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.11.001},
   Abstract = {The internet is rapidly changing what information is
             available as well as how we find it and share it with
             others. Here we examine how this “digital expansion of the
             mind” changes cognition. We begin by identifying ten
             properties of the internet that likely affect cognition,
             roughly organized around internet content (e.g., the sheer
             amount of information available), internet usage (e.g., the
             requirement to search for information), and the people and
             communities who create and propagate content (e.g., people
             are connected in an unprecedented fashion). We use these
             properties to explain (or ask questions about)
             internet-related phenomena, such as habitual reliance on the
             internet, the propagation of misinformation, and
             consequences for autobiographical memory, among others. Our
             goal is to consider the impact of internet usage on many
             aspects of cognition, as people increasingly rely on the
             internet to seek, post, and share information.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.11.001},
   Key = {fds341736}
}

@article{fds338051,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Yang, BW and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {When the Unlikely Becomes Likely: Qualifying Language Does
             Not Influence Later Truth Judgments},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {118-129},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.08.004},
   Abstract = {Judgments and decisions are frequently made under
             uncertainty. People often express and interpret this
             uncertainty with epistemic qualifiers (e.g., likely,
             improbable). We investigate the extent to which qualifiers
             influence truth judgments over time. In four studies,
             participants studied qualified statements, and two days
             later they rated the truth of previously qualified
             statements along with new statements. Previously qualified
             statements were rated as more likely true than new
             statements, even when the qualifiers had distinctly opposite
             meanings (i.e., certain versus impossible; Study 1) and when
             all qualifiers cast doubt on the veracity of the statements
             (e.g., improbable, impossible; Studies 2–4). Three
             additional studies suggested that this effect was not
             dependent on memory for the qualifiers. Consistent with a
             fluency interpretation, prior exposure made the statements
             easier to read, driving truth judgments, and overriding the
             influence of qualifying information. Implications for
             improving communication using qualifiers are
             discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.08.004},
   Key = {fds338051}
}

@article{fds342150,
   Author = {Rajaram, S and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Cognition in the Internet Age: What are the Important
             Questions?},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {46-49},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.01.004},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.01.004},
   Key = {fds342150}
}

@article{fds339752,
   Author = {Wang, W-C and Brashier, NM and Wing, EA and Marsh, EJ and Cabeza,
             R},
   Title = {Neural basis of goal-driven changes in knowledge
             activation.},
   Journal = {The European journal of neuroscience},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {3389-3396},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14196},
   Abstract = {Depending on a person's goals, different aspects of stored
             knowledge are accessed. Decades of behavioral work document
             the flexible use of knowledge, but little neuroimaging work
             speaks to these questions. We used representational
             similarity analysis to investigate whether the relationship
             between brain activity and semantic structure of statements
             varied in two tasks hypothesized to differ in the degree to
             which knowledge is accessed: judging truth (semantic task)
             and judging oldness (episodic task). During truth judgments,
             but not old/new recognition judgments, a left-lateralized
             network previously associated with semantic memory exhibited
             correlations with semantic structure. At a neural level,
             people activate knowledge representations in different ways
             when focused on different goals. The present results
             demonstrate the potential of multivariate approaches in
             characterizing knowledge storage and retrieval, as well as
             the ways that it shapes our understanding and long-term
             memory.},
   Doi = {10.1111/ejn.14196},
   Key = {fds339752}
}

@article{fds333724,
   Author = {Wang, W-C and Brashier, NM and Wing, EA and Marsh, EJ and Cabeza,
             R},
   Title = {Knowledge supports memory retrieval through familiarity, not
             recollection.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologia},
   Volume = {113},
   Pages = {14-21},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.019},
   Abstract = {Semantic memory, or general knowledge of the world, guides
             learning and supports the formation and retrieval of new
             episodic memories. Behavioral evidence suggests that this
             knowledge effect is supported by recollection-a more
             controlled form of memory retrieval generally accompanied by
             contextual details-to a greater degree than familiarity-a
             more automatic form of memory retrieval generally absent of
             contextual details. In the current study, we used functional
             magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the role
             that regions associated with recollection and familiarity
             play in retrieving recent instances of known (e.g., The
             Summer Olympic Games are held four years apart) and unknown
             (e.g., A flaky deposit found in port bottles is beeswing)
             statements. Our results revealed a surprising pattern:
             Episodic retrieval of known statements recruited regions
             associated with familiarity, but not recollection. Instead,
             retrieval of unknown statements recruited regions associated
             with recollection. These data, in combination with quicker
             reaction times for the retrieval of known than unknown
             statements, suggest that known statements can be
             successfully retrieved on the basis of familiarity, whereas
             unknown statements were retrieved on the basis of
             recollection. Our results provide insight into how knowledge
             influences episodic retrieval and demonstrate the role of
             neuroimaging in providing insights into cognitive processes
             in the absence of explicit behavioral responses.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.019},
   Key = {fds333724}
}

@article{fds332045,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Black-Maier, AC and Raley, ND and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Retrieving and applying knowledge to different examples
             promotes transfer of learning.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Applied},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {433-446},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xap0000142},
   Abstract = {Introducing variability during learning often facilitates
             transfer to new contexts (i.e., generalization). The goal of
             the present study was to explore the concept of variability
             in an area of research where its effects have received
             little attention: learning through retrieval practice. In
             four experiments, we investigated whether retrieval practice
             with different examples of a concept promotes greater
             transfer than repeated retrieval practice with the same
             example. Participants watched video clips from a lecture
             about geological science and answered application questions
             about concepts: either the same question three times or
             three different questions. Experiments 3 and 4 also included
             conditions that involved repeatedly studying the information
             in the application questions (either the same example or
             three different examples). Two days later, participants took
             a final test with new application questions. All four
             experiments showed that variability during retrieval
             practice produced superior transfer of knowledge to new
             examples. Experiments 3 and 4 also showed a testing effect
             and a benefit from studying different examples. Overall,
             these findings suggest that repeatedly retrieving and
             applying knowledge to different examples is a powerful
             method for acquiring knowledge that will transfer to a
             variety of new contexts. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/xap0000142},
   Key = {fds332045}
}

@article{fds331410,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Yang, BW},
   Title = {A Call to Think Broadly about Information
             Literacy},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {401-404},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.09.012},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.09.012},
   Key = {fds331410}
}

@article{fds332982,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Family Matters: Measuring Impact Through One's Academic
             Descendants.},
   Journal = {Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the
             Association for Psychological Science},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1130-1132},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691617719759},
   Abstract = {Scientific contributions take many forms, not all of which
             result in fame or are captured in traditional metrics of
             success (e.g., h factor). My focus is on one of the most
             lasting and important contributions a scientist can make:
             training scientists who go on to train scientists, who in
             turn train more scientists, etc. Academic genealogies
             provide many examples of scientists whose names might not be
             recognizable today but who trained psychologists that went
             on to publish very influential work. Of course success
             results from a combination of many factors (including but
             not limited to the student's abilities and motivation, luck,
             institutional resources, mentoring, etc.), but the field
             should find more ways to acknowledge the role that mentoring
             does play.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1745691617719759},
   Key = {fds332982}
}

@article{fds326719,
   Author = {Brashier, NM and Umanath, S and Cabeza, R and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Competing cues: Older adults rely on knowledge in the face
             of fluency.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {331-337},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000156},
   Abstract = {Consumers regularly encounter repeated false claims in
             political and marketing campaigns, but very little empirical
             work addresses their impact among older adults. Repeated
             statements feel easier to process, and thus more truthful,
             than new ones (i.e., illusory truth). When judging truth,
             older adults' accumulated general knowledge may offset this
             perception of fluency. In two experiments, participants read
             statements that contradicted information stored in memory; a
             post-experimental knowledge check confirmed what individual
             participants knew. Unlike young adults, older adults
             exhibited illusory truth only when they lacked knowledge
             about claims. This interaction between knowledge and fluency
             extends dual-process theories of aging. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000156},
   Key = {fds326719}
}

@article{fds326720,
   Author = {Arnold, KM and Umanath, S and Thio, K and Reilly, WB and McDaniel, MA and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Understanding the cognitive processes involved in writing to
             learn.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Applied},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {115-127},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xap0000119},
   Abstract = {Writing is often used as a tool for learning. However,
             empirical support for the benefits of writing-to-learn is
             mixed, likely because the literature conflates diverse
             activities (e.g., summaries, term papers) under the single
             umbrella of writing-to-learn. Following recent trends in the
             writing-to-learn literature, the authors focus on the
             underlying cognitive processes. They draw on the largely
             independent writing-to-learn and cognitive psychology
             learning literatures to identify important cognitive
             processes. The current experiment examines learning from 3
             writing tasks (and 1 nonwriting control), with an emphasis
             on whether or not the tasks engaged retrieval. Tasks that
             engaged retrieval (essay writing and free recall) led to
             better final test performance than those that did not (note
             taking and highlighting). Individual differences in
             structure building (the ability to construct mental
             representations of narratives; Gernsbacher, Varner, & Faust,
             1990) modified this effect; skilled structure builders
             benefited more from essay writing and free recall than did
             less skilled structure builders. Further, more essay-like
             responses led to better performance, implicating the
             importance of additional cognitive processes such as
             reorganization and elaboration. The results highlight how
             both task instructions and individual differences affect the
             cognitive processes involved when writing-to-learn, with
             consequences for the effectiveness of the learning strategy.
             (PsycINFO Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/xap0000119},
   Key = {fds326720}
}

@article{fds322500,
   Author = {Cantor, AD and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Expertise effects in the Moses illusion: detecting
             contradictions with stored knowledge.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {25},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {220-230},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1152377},
   Abstract = {People frequently miss contradictions with stored knowledge;
             for example, readers often fail to notice any problem with a
             reference to the Atlantic as the largest ocean. Critically,
             such effects occur even though participants later
             demonstrate knowing the Pacific is the largest ocean (the
             Moses Illusion) [Erickson, T. D., & Mattson,
             M. E. (1981). From words to meaning: A semantic illusion.
             Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 20, 540-551].
             We investigated whether such oversights disappear when
             erroneous references contradict information in one's expert
             domain, material which likely has been encountered many
             times and is particularly well-known. Biology and history
             graduate students monitored for errors while answering
             biology and history questions containing erroneous
             presuppositions ("In what US state were the forty-niners
             searching for oil?"). Expertise helped: participants were
             less susceptible to the illusion and less likely to later
             reproduce errors in their expert domain. However, expertise
             did not eliminate the illusion, even when errors were bolded
             and underlined, meaning that it was unlikely that people
             simply skipped over errors. The results support claims that
             people often use heuristics to judge truth, as opposed to
             directly retrieving information from memory, likely because
             such heuristics are adaptive and often lead to the correct
             answer. Even experts sometimes use such shortcuts,
             suggesting that overlearned and accessible knowledge does
             not guarantee retrieval of that information.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2016.1152377},
   Key = {fds322500}
}

@article{fds322020,
   Author = {Wang, W-C and Brashier, NM and Wing, EA and Marsh, EJ and Cabeza,
             R},
   Title = {On Known Unknowns: Fluency and the Neural Mechanisms of
             Illusory Truth.},
   Journal = {Journal of cognitive neuroscience},
   Volume = {28},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {739-746},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00923},
   Abstract = {The "illusory truth" effect refers to the phenomenon whereby
             repetition of a statement increases its likelihood of being
             judged true. This phenomenon has important implications for
             how we come to believe oft-repeated information that may be
             misleading or unknown. Behavioral evidence indicates that
             fluency, the subjective ease experienced while processing
             information, underlies this effect. This suggests that
             illusory truth should be mediated by brain regions
             previously linked to fluency, such as the perirhinal cortex
             (PRC). To investigate this possibility, we scanned
             participants with fMRI while they rated the truth of unknown
             statements, half of which were presented earlier (i.e.,
             repeated). The only brain region that showed an interaction
             between repetition and ratings of perceived truth was PRC,
             where activity increased with truth ratings for repeated,
             but not for new, statements. This finding supports the
             hypothesis that illusory truth is mediated by a fluency
             mechanism and further strengthens the link between PRC and
             fluency.},
   Doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_00923},
   Key = {fds322020}
}

@article{fds326830,
   Author = {Arnold, KM and Daniel, DB and Jensen, JL and Mcdaniel, MA and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Structure Building Predicts Grades in College Psychology and
             Biology},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {30},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {454-459},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3226},
   Abstract = {Knowing what skills underlie college success can allow
             students, teachers, and universities to identify and to help
             at-risk students. One skill that may underlie success across
             a variety of subject areas is structure building, the
             ability to create mental representations of narratives
             (Gernsbacher, Varner, & Faust, 1990). We tested if
             individual differences in structure-building ability
             predicted success in two college classes: introductory to
             psychology and introductory biology. In both cases,
             structure building predicted success. This effect was
             robust, with structure building explaining variance in
             course grades even after accounting for high school GPA and
             SAT scores (in the psychology course) or a measure of domain
             knowledge (in the biology course). The results support the
             claim that structure building is an important individual
             difference, one that is associated with learning in
             different domains.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.3226},
   Key = {fds326830}
}

@article{fds322499,
   Author = {Mullet, HG and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Correcting false memories: Errors must be noticed and
             replaced.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {44},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {403-412},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0571-x},
   Abstract = {Memory can be unreliable. For example, after reading The new
             baby stayed awake all night, people often misremember that
             the new baby cried all night (Brewer, 1977); similarly,
             after hearing bed, rest, and tired, people often falsely
             remember that sleep was on the list (Roediger & McDermott,
             1995). In general, such false memories are difficult to
             correct, persisting despite warnings and additional study
             opportunities. We argue that errors must first be detected
             to be corrected; consistent with this argument, two
             experiments showed that false memories were nearly
             eliminated when conditions facilitated comparisons between
             participants' errors and corrective feedback (e.g.,
             immediate trial-by-trial feedback that allowed direct
             comparisons between their responses and the correct
             information). However, knowledge that they had made an error
             was insufficient; unless the feedback message also contained
             the correct answer, the rate of false memories remained
             relatively constant. On the one hand, there is nothing
             special about correcting false memories: simply labeling an
             error as "wrong" is also insufficient for correcting other
             memory errors, including misremembered facts or
             mistranslations. However, unlike these other types of
             errors--which often benefit from the spacing afforded by
             delayed feedback--false memories require a special
             consideration: Learners may fail to notice their errors
             unless the correction conditions specifically highlight
             them.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-015-0571-x},
   Key = {fds322499}
}

@article{fds322501,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Cantor, AD and M. Brashier and N},
   Title = {Believing that Humans Swallow Spiders in Their Sleep: False
             Beliefs as Side Effects of the Processes that Support
             Accurate Knowledge},
   Journal = {Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research
             and Theory},
   Volume = {64},
   Pages = {93-132},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2015.09.003},
   Abstract = {Humans can store, maintain, and retrieve an impressive
             amount of information—but the processes that support
             accurate knowledge can also lead to errors, such as the
             false belief that humans swallow eight spiders in their
             sleep each year. In this chapter, we review characteristics
             of the knowledge base and explore how five adaptive
             properties that support accurate knowledge can also lead to
             the learning, storage, and retrieval of falsehoods. First,
             people exhibit a bias to believe information is true since,
             most of the time, incoming information is indeed true.
             Second, we utilize a fluency-based heuristic for judging
             truth since—again, most of the time—easy processing
             reliably signals that something is true. Third, the
             knowledge base is productive: people use existing knowledge
             to make new inferences, which are typically accurate but
             occasionally are inappropriate and result in errors. Fourth,
             existing knowledge supports new learning, so our ingrained
             misconceptions can foster new errors and interfere with
             learning the truth. Fifth, because it would be too taxing to
             carefully compare all incoming information to stored
             knowledge, we do not require a perfect match and often
             accept information as “good enough.” As a result, errors
             that are similar to the truth often slip by undetected, and
             sometimes are later reproduced. Finally, we discuss methods
             for correcting errors and potential barriers to the
             correction of misconceptions. In particular, it is essential
             to refute the error as well as provide a simple alternative
             to replace it. Overall, the processes that support accurate
             knowledge and false beliefs are the same, and can lead to
             competing representations in memory.},
   Doi = {10.1016/bs.plm.2015.09.003},
   Key = {fds322501}
}

@article{fds252727,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Brashier, NM and Payne, BK and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Knowledge does not protect against illusory
             truth.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. General},
   Volume = {144},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {993-1002},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {0096-3445},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000098},
   Abstract = {In daily life, we frequently encounter false claims in the
             form of consumer advertisements, political propaganda, and
             rumors. Repetition may be one way that insidious
             misconceptions, such as the belief that vitamin C prevents
             the common cold, enter our knowledge base. Research on the
             illusory truth effect demonstrates that repeated statements
             are easier to process, and subsequently perceived to be more
             truthful, than new statements. The prevailing assumption in
             the literature has been that knowledge constrains this
             effect (i.e., repeating the statement "The Atlantic Ocean is
             the largest ocean on Earth" will not make you believe it).
             We tested this assumption using both normed estimates of
             knowledge and individuals' demonstrated knowledge on a
             postexperimental knowledge check (Experiment 1). Contrary to
             prior suppositions, illusory truth effects occurred even
             when participants knew better. Multinomial modeling
             demonstrated that participants sometimes rely on fluency
             even if knowledge is also available to them (Experiment 2).
             Thus, participants demonstrated knowledge neglect, or the
             failure to rely on stored knowledge, in the face of fluent
             processing experiences.},
   Doi = {10.1037/xge0000098},
   Key = {fds252727}
}

@article{fds252733,
   Author = {Deffler, SA and Brown, AS and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Judging the familiarity of strangers: does the context
             matter?},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1041-1047},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {August},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0769-0},
   Abstract = {Context affects face recognition, with people more likely to
             recognize an acquaintance when that person is encountered in
             an expected and familiar place. However, we demonstrate that
             a familiar context can also incorrectly lead to feeling that
             a stranger is known. More specifically, we asked whether a
             familiar place can increase the belief that a stranger has
             been encountered outside of the experimental context (e.g.,
             in the news or in real life). Novel faces were paired with
             novel places, famous places (landmarks), or neutral (solid
             color) backgrounds, and participants rated the
             pre-experimental familiarity of each novel face. Across four
             experiments, participants misinterpreted the familiarity of
             the landmark backgrounds as evidence of knowing the faces
             outside of the experimental context. This effect only
             disappeared when participants failed to integrate the face
             with the place, judging that the two did not fit together.
             Our findings suggest that familiarity is not judged in
             isolation; rather, people are unable to completely
             disentangle the familiarity of the individual components of
             a scene.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-014-0769-0},
   Key = {fds252733}
}

@article{fds252728,
   Author = {Brown, AS and Croft Caderao and K and Fields, LM and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Borrowing Personal Memories},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {471-477},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://hdl.handle.net/10161/10380 Duke open
             access},
   Abstract = {The present investigation documents memory borrowing in
             college-age students, defined as the telling of others'
             autobiographical stories as if they are one's own. In both
             pilot and online surveys, most undergraduates admit to
             borrowing personal stories from others or using details from
             others' experiences to embellish their own retellings. These
             behaviors appear primarily motivated by a desire to
             permanently incorporate others' experiences into one's own
             autobiographical record (appropriation), but other reasons
             include to temporarily create a more coherent or engaging
             conversational exchange (social connection), simplify
             conveying somebody else's interesting experience
             (convenience), or make oneself look good (status
             enhancement). A substantial percentage of respondents
             expressed uncertainty as to whether an autobiographical
             experience actually belonged to them or to someone else, and
             most respondents have confronted somebody over ownership of
             a particular story. Documenting memory borrowing is
             important as the behavior has potential consequences for the
             creation of false memories.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.3130},
   Key = {fds252728}
}

@article{fds252734,
   Author = {Cantor, AD and Eslick, AN and Marsh, EJ and Bjork, RA and Bjork,
             EL},
   Title = {Multiple-choice tests stabilize access to marginal
             knowledge.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {43},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {193-205},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0462-6},
   Abstract = {Marginal knowledge refers to knowledge that is stored in
             memory, but is not accessible at a given moment. For
             example, one might struggle to remember who wrote The Call
             of the Wild, even if that knowledge is stored in memory.
             Knowing how best to stabilize access to marginal knowledge
             is important, given that new learning often requires
             accessing and building on prior knowledge. While even a
             single opportunity to restudy marginal knowledge boosts its
             later accessibility (Berger, Hall, & Bahrick, 1999), in many
             situations explicit relearning opportunities are not
             available. Our question is whether multiple-choice tests
             (which by definition expose the learner to the correct
             answers) can also serve this function and, if so, how
             testing compares to restudying given that tests can be
             particularly powerful learning devices (Roediger & Karpicke,
             2006). In four experiments, we found that multiple-choice
             testing had the power to stabilize access to marginal
             knowledge, and to do so for at least up to a week.
             Importantly, such tests did not need to be paired with
             feedback, although testing was no more powerful than
             studying. Overall, the results support the idea that one's
             knowledge base is unstable, with individual pieces of
             information coming in and out of reach. The present findings
             have implications for a key educational challenge: ensuring
             that students have continuing access to information they
             have learned.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-014-0462-6},
   Key = {fds252734}
}

@article{fds252740,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Dolan, PO and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Learning misinformation from fictional sources:
             understanding the contributions of transportation and
             item-specific processing.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {167-177},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.877146},
   Abstract = {People often pick up incorrect information about the world
             from movies, novels and other fictional sources. The
             question asked here is whether such sources are a
             particularly potent source of misinformation. On the one
             hand, story-reading involves transportation into a fictional
             world, with a possible reduction in access to one's prior
             knowledge (likely reducing the chances that the reader will
             notice errors). On the other hand, stories encourage
             relational processing as readers create mental models,
             decreasing the likelihood that they will encode and remember
             more peripheral details like erroneous facts. To test these
             ideas, we examined suggestibility after readers were exposed
             to misleading references embedded in stories and lists that
             were matched on a number of dimensions. In two experiments,
             suggestibility was greater following exposure to
             misinformation in a list of sentences rather than a coherent
             story, even though the story was rated as more engaging than
             the list. Furthermore, processing the story with an
             item-specific processing task (inserting missing letters)
             increased later suggestibility, whereas this task had no
             impact on suggestibility when misinformation was presented
             within a list. The type of processing used when reading a
             text affects suggestibility more than engagement with the
             text.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2013.877146},
   Key = {fds252740}
}

@article{fds252735,
   Author = {Mullet, HG and Umanath, S and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Recent study, but not retrieval, of knowledge protects
             against learning errors.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {42},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1239-1249},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0437-7},
   Abstract = {Surprisingly, people incorporate errors into their knowledge
             bases even when they have the correct knowledge stored in
             memory (e.g., Fazio, Barber, Rajaram, Ornstein, & Marsh,
             2013). We examined whether heightening the accessibility of
             correct knowledge would protect people from later
             reproducing misleading information that they encountered in
             fictional stories. In Experiment 1, participants studied a
             series of target general knowledge questions and their
             correct answers either a few minutes (high accessibility of
             knowledge) or 1 week (low accessibility of knowledge) before
             exposure to misleading story references. In Experiments 2a
             and 2b, participants instead retrieved the answers to the
             target general knowledge questions either a few minutes or 1
             week before the rest of the experiment. Reading the relevant
             knowledge directly before the story-reading phase protected
             against reproduction of the misleading story answers on a
             later general knowledge test, but retrieving that same
             correct information did not. Retrieving stored knowledge
             from memory might actually enhance the encoding of relevant
             misinformation.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-014-0437-7},
   Key = {fds252735}
}

@article{fds287923,
   Author = {Mullet, HG and Butler, AC and Verdin, B and von Borries, R and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Delaying feedback promotes transfer of knowledge despite
             student preferences to receive feedback immediately},
   Journal = {Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
             Cognition},
   Volume = {3},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {222-229},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {2211-3681},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.05.001},
   Abstract = {Educators and researchers who study human learning often
             assume that feedback is most effective when given
             immediately. However, a growing body of research has
             challenged this assumption by demonstrating that delaying
             feedback can facilitate learning. Advocates for immediate
             feedback have questioned the generalizability of this
             finding, suggesting that such effects only occur in highly
             controlled laboratory settings. We report a pair of
             experiments in which the timing of feedback was manipulated
             in an upper-level college engineering course. Students
             practiced applying their knowledge of complex engineering
             concepts on weekly homework assignments, and then received
             feedback either immediately after the assignment deadline or
             1 week later. When students received delayed feedback, they
             performed betteron subsequent course exams that contained
             new problems about the same concepts. Although delayed
             feedback produced superior transfer of knowledge, students
             reported that they benefited most from immediate feedback,
             revealing a metacognitive disconnect between actual and
             perceived effectiveness.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.05.001},
   Key = {fds287923}
}

@article{fds252744,
   Author = {Umanath, S and Dolan, PO and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Ageing and the Moses illusion: older adults fall for Moses
             but if asked directly, stick with Noah.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {481-492},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23705952},
   Abstract = {Many people respond "two" to the question "How many animals
             of each kind did Moses take on the ark?", even though they
             know the reference should be to Noah. The Moses Illusion
             demonstrates a failure to apply stored knowledge (Erickson &
             Mattson, 1981). Of interest was whether older adults' robust
             knowledge bases would protect them from vulnerability to
             this illusion. Of secondary interest were any age
             differences in the memorial consequences of the illusion,
             and whether older adults' prior knowledge would protect them
             from later reproducing information from distorted questions
             (e.g., later saying that Moses took two animals of each kind
             on the ark). Surprisingly, older adults fell for the Moses
             Illusion more often than did younger adults. However,
             falling for the illusion did not affect older adults' later
             memory; they were less suggestible than young adults. Most
             importantly, older adults were more likely to recover from
             exposure to distorted questions and respond correctly.
             Explanations of these findings, drawing on theories of
             cognitive ageing, are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2013.799701},
   Key = {fds252744}
}

@article{fds252739,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Marsh, EJ and Slavinsky, JP and Baraniuk,
             RG},
   Title = {Integrating Cognitive Science and Technology Improves
             Learning in a STEM Classroom},
   Journal = {Educational Psychology Review},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {331-340},
   Publisher = {Springer Nature},
   Year = {2014},
   ISSN = {1040-726X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9256-4},
   Abstract = {The most effective educational interventions often face
             significant barriers to widespread implementation because
             they are highly specific, resource intense, and/or
             comprehensive. We argue for an alternative approach to
             improving education: leveraging technology and cognitive
             science to develop interventions that generalize, scale, and
             can be easily implemented within any curriculum. In a
             classroom experiment, we investigated whether three simple,
             but powerful principles from cognitive science could be
             combined to improve learning. Although implementation of
             these principles only required a few small changes to
             standard practice in a college engineering course, it
             significantly increased student performance on exams. Our
             findings highlight the potential for developing inexpensive,
             yet effective educational interventions that can be
             implemented worldwide. © 2014 Springer Science+Business
             Media New York.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10648-014-9256-4},
   Key = {fds252739}
}

@article{fds252741,
   Author = {Slavinsky, JP and Davenport, KJ and Butler, AC and Marsh, EJ and Baraniuk, RG},
   Title = {Open online platforms advancing DSP education},
   Journal = {ICASSP, IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech
             and Signal Processing - Proceedings},
   Pages = {8771-8775},
   Publisher = {IEEE},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   ISSN = {1520-6149},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ICASSP.2013.6639379},
   Abstract = {Two open, online educational platforms, OpenStax Exercises
             and OpenStax Tutor, are working to revolutionize the way in
             which students learn concepts in diverse subject areas. Born
             and tested in the area of signal processing education, these
             tools bring to bear cutting-edge ideas in cognitive science
             and machine learning to automatically build personalized
             learning pathways for today's students and to advance the
             field of learning science. These platforms are introduced
             and initial results discussed. © 2013 IEEE.},
   Doi = {10.1109/ICASSP.2013.6639379},
   Key = {fds252741}
}

@article{fds252743,
   Author = {Dunlosky, J and Rawson, KA and Marsh, EJ and Nathan, MJ and Willingham,
             DT},
   Title = {Improving Students' Learning With Effective Learning
             Techniques: Promising Directions From Cognitive and
             Educational Psychology.},
   Journal = {Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of
             the American Psychological Society},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {4-58},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {1529-1006},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266},
   Abstract = {Many students are being left behind by an educational system
             that some people believe is in crisis. Improving educational
             outcomes will require efforts on many fronts, but a central
             premise of this monograph is that one part of a solution
             involves helping students to better regulate their learning
             through the use of effective learning techniques.
             Fortunately, cognitive and educational psychologists have
             been developing and evaluating easy-to-use learning
             techniques that could help students achieve their learning
             goals. In this monograph, we discuss 10 learning techniques
             in detail and offer recommendations about their relative
             utility. We selected techniques that were expected to be
             relatively easy to use and hence could be adopted by many
             students. Also, some techniques (e.g., highlighting and
             rereading) were selected because students report relying
             heavily on them, which makes it especially important to
             examine how well they work. The techniques include
             elaborative interrogation, self-explanation, summarization,
             highlighting (or underlining), the keyword mnemonic, imagery
             use for text learning, rereading, practice testing,
             distributed practice, and interleaved practice. To offer
             recommendations about the relative utility of these
             techniques, we evaluated whether their benefits generalize
             across four categories of variables: learning conditions,
             student characteristics, materials, and criterion tasks.
             Learning conditions include aspects of the learning
             environment in which the technique is implemented, such as
             whether a student studies alone or with a group. Student
             characteristics include variables such as age, ability, and
             level of prior knowledge. Materials vary from simple
             concepts to mathematical problems to complicated science
             texts. Criterion tasks include different outcome measures
             that are relevant to student achievement, such as those
             tapping memory, problem solving, and comprehension. We
             attempted to provide thorough reviews for each technique, so
             this monograph is rather lengthy. However, we also wrote the
             monograph in a modular fashion, so it is easy to use. In
             particular, each review is divided into the following
             sections: General description of the technique and why it
             should work How general are the effects of this technique?
              2a. Learning conditions  2b. Student characteristics
              2c. Materials  2d. Criterion tasks Effects in
             representative educational contexts Issues for
             implementation Overall assessment The review for each
             technique can be read independently of the others, and
             particular variables of interest can be easily compared
             across techniques. To foreshadow our final recommendations,
             the techniques vary widely with respect to their
             generalizability and promise for improving student learning.
             Practice testing and distributed practice received high
             utility assessments because they benefit learners of
             different ages and abilities and have been shown to boost
             students' performance across many criterion tasks and even
             in educational contexts. Elaborative interrogation,
             self-explanation, and interleaved practice received moderate
             utility assessments. The benefits of these techniques do
             generalize across some variables, yet despite their promise,
             they fell short of a high utility assessment because the
             evidence for their efficacy is limited. For instance,
             elaborative interrogation and self-explanation have not been
             adequately evaluated in educational contexts, and the
             benefits of interleaving have just begun to be
             systematically explored, so the ultimate effectiveness of
             these techniques is currently unknown. Nevertheless, the
             techniques that received moderate-utility ratings show
             enough promise for us to recommend their use in appropriate
             situations, which we describe in detail within the review of
             each technique. Five techniques received a low utility
             assessment: summarization, highlighting, the keyword
             mnemonic, imagery use for text learning, and rereading.
             These techniques were rated as low utility for numerous
             reasons. Summarization and imagery use for text learning
             have been shown to help some students on some criterion
             tasks, yet the conditions under which these techniques
             produce benefits are limited, and much research is still
             needed to fully explore their overall effectiveness. The
             keyword mnemonic is difficult to implement in some contexts,
             and it appears to benefit students for a limited number of
             materials and for short retention intervals. Most students
             report rereading and highlighting, yet these techniques do
             not consistently boost students' performance, so other
             techniques should be used in their place (e.g., practice
             testing instead of rereading). Our hope is that this
             monograph will foster improvements in student learning, not
             only by showcasing which learning techniques are likely to
             have the most generalizable effects but also by encouraging
             researchers to continue investigating the most promising
             techniques. Accordingly, in our closing remarks, we discuss
             some issues for how these techniques could be implemented by
             teachers and students, and we highlight directions for
             future research.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1529100612453266},
   Key = {fds252743}
}

@article{fds221830,
   Author = {Fazio, L. K. and Dolan, P. O. and Marsh, E. J},
   Title = {Learning misinformation from fictional sources:
             Understanding the contributions of transportation and
             item-specific processing},
   Journal = {Memory},
   Year = {2013},
   Abstract = {People often pick up incorrect information about the world
             from movies, novels and other fictional sources. The
             question asked here is whether such sources are a
             particularly potent source of misinformation. On the one
             hand, story-reading involves transportation into a fictional
             world, with a consequent reduction in access to one’s
             prior knowledge (likely reducing the chances that the reader
             will notice errors). On the other hand, stories encourage
             relational processing as readers create mental models,
             decreasing the likelihood they will encode and remember more
             peripheral details like erroneous facts. To test these
             ideas, we examined suggestibility after readers were exposed
             to misleading references embedded in stories and lists that
             were matched on a number of dimensions. In two experiments,
             suggestibility was greater following exposure to
             misinformation in a list of sentences rather than a coherent
             story, even though the story was rated as more engaging than
             the list. Furthermore, processing the story with an
             item-specific processing task (inserting missing letters)
             increased later suggestibility, whereas this task had no
             impact on suggestibility when misinformation was presented
             within a list. The type of processing used when reading a
             text affects suggestibility more than engagement with the
             text.},
   Key = {fds221830}
}

@article{fds252763,
   Author = {Umanath, S and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Aging and the memorial consequences of catching
             contradictions with prior knowledge.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1033-1038},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22308998},
   Abstract = {This experiment tested the possibility that older adults are
             less susceptible to semantic illusions because they are more
             likely to notice contradictions with stored knowledge. Older
             and young adults encoded stories containing factual
             inaccuracies; critically, half the participants were
             instructed to mark any errors they noticed. Older adults
             reproduced fewer story-errors on a later general knowledge
             test, but there were no age differences in marking errors
             during encoding. However, older adults were better able to
             recover and answer correctly after failing to notice errors
             during story-reading. Implications for false memories and
             semantic illusions are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0027242},
   Key = {fds252763}
}

@article{fds252760,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Butler, AC and Umanath, S},
   Title = {Using Fictional Sources in the Classroom: Applications from
             Cognitive Psychology},
   Journal = {Educational Psychology Review},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {449-469},
   Publisher = {Springer Nature},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {1040-726X},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9204-0},
   Abstract = {Fictional materials are commonly used in the classroom to
             teach course content. Both laboratory experiments and
             classroom demonstrations illustrate the benefits of using
             fiction to help students learn accurate information about
             the world. However, fictional sources often contain
             factually inaccurate content, making them a potent vehicle
             for learning misinformation about the world. We briefly
             review theoretical issues relevant to whether learners
             process fictional sources differently before exploring how
             individual differences, learning activities, and assessment
             characteristics may affect learning from fiction. This
             review focuses on our own experimental approaches for
             studying learning from fiction, including learning from
             short stories and from films, while connecting to a broader
             educational literature on learning from fictional sources.
             Throughout the review, implications for educational use and
             future directions for experimental research are noted. ©
             2012 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s10648-012-9204-0},
   Key = {fds252760}
}

@article{fds252759,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Dennis, NA and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Inferring facts from fiction: reading correct and incorrect
             information affects memory for related information.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {487-498},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22640369},
   Abstract = {People can acquire both true and false knowledge about the
             world from fictional stories. The present study explored
             whether the benefits and costs of learning about the world
             from fictional stories extend beyond memory for directly
             stated pieces of information. Of interest was whether
             readers would use correct and incorrect story references to
             make deductive inferences about related information in the
             story, and then integrate those inferences into their
             knowledge bases. Participants read stories containing
             correct, neutral, and misleading references to facts about
             the world; each reference could be combined with another
             reference that occurred in a later sentence to make a
             deductive inference. Later they answered general knowledge
             questions that tested for these deductive inferences. The
             results showed that participants generated and retained the
             deductive inferences regardless of whether the inferences
             were consistent or inconsistent with world knowledge, and
             irrespective of whether the references were placed
             consecutively in the text or separated by many sentences.
             Readers learn more than what is directly stated in stories;
             they use references to the real world to make both correct
             and incorrect inferences that are integrated into their
             knowledge bases.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2012.682067},
   Key = {fds252759}
}

@article{fds304691,
   Author = {Umanath, S and Butler, AC and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Positive and Negative Effects of Monitoring Popular Films
             for Historical Inaccuracies},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {556-567},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.2827},
   Abstract = {Summary: History educators often use popular films in the
             classroom to teach critical thinking through an exercise
             that involves identifying historical inaccuracies in the
             films. We investigated how this exercise affects the
             acquisition of true and false historical knowledge. In two
             experiments, subjects studied texts about historical topics
             and watched clips from corresponding films. Each film
             contained one piece of information that contradicted the
             text (i.e. misinformation). Some subjects received
             instructions to monitor for inaccuracies in the films. After
             a delay, they were tested on the texts. Monitoring
             instructions did not reduce subjects' acquisition of
             misinformation, and even when subjects successfully detected
             the inaccuracies, they sometimes still reproduced the
             misinformation. However, when they received feedback about
             the inaccuracies, the production of misinformation was
             substantially reduced. Overall, these findings indicate that
             educators should provide feedback when using popular films
             for this critical thinking exercise so that students do not
             acquire false knowledge. © 2012 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.2827},
   Key = {fds304691}
}

@article{fds252757,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Lozito, JP and Umanath, S and Bjork, EL and Bjork,
             RA},
   Title = {Using verification feedback to correct errors made on a
             multiple-choice test.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {645-653},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22640417},
   Abstract = {A key educational challenge is how to correct students'
             errors and misconceptions so that they do not persist.
             Simply labelling an answer as correct or incorrect on a
             short-answer test (verification feedback) does not improve
             performance on later tests; error correction requires
             receiving answer feedback. We explored the generality of
             this conclusion and whether the effectiveness of
             verification feedback depends on the type of test with which
             it is paired. We argue that, unlike for short-answer tests,
             learning whether one's multiple-choice selection is correct
             or incorrect should help participants narrow down the
             possible answers and identify specific lures as false. To
             test this proposition we asked participants to answer a
             series of general knowledge multiple-choice questions. They
             received no feedback, answer feedback, or verification
             feedback, and then took a short-answer test immediately and
             two days later. Verification feedback was just as effective
             as answer feedback for maintaining correct answers.
             Importantly, verification feedback allowed learners to
             correct more of their errors than did no feedback, although
             it was not as effective as answer feedback. Overall,
             verification feedback conveyed information to the learner,
             which has both practical and theoretical
             implications.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2012.684882},
   Key = {fds252757}
}

@article{fds252758,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Fazio, LK and Goswick, AE},
   Title = {Memorial consequences of testing school-aged
             children.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {899-906},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22891857},
   Abstract = {A large literature shows that retrieval practice is a
             powerful tool for enhancing learning and memory in
             undergraduates (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a). Much less work
             has examined the memorial consequences of testing
             school-aged children. Our focus is on multiple-choice tests,
             which are potentially problematic since they minimise
             retrieval practice and also expose students to errors (the
             multiple-choice lures). To examine this issue, second
             graders took a multiple-choice general knowledge test (e.g.,
             What country did the Pilgrims come from: England, Germany,
             Ireland, or Spain?) and later answered a series of short
             answer questions, some of which corresponded to questions on
             the earlier multiple-choice test. Without feedback, the
             benefits of prior testing outweighed the costs for easy
             questions. However, for hard questions, the large increase
             in multiple-choice lure answers on the final test meant that
             the cost of prior testing outweighed the benefits when no
             feedback was provided. This negative testing effect was
             eliminated when children received immediate feedback
             (consisting of the correct answer) after each
             multiple-choice selection. Implications for educational
             practice are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2012.708757},
   Key = {fds252758}
}

@article{fds252752,
   Author = {Goswick, AE and Mullet, HG and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Suggestibility from stories: Can production difficulties and
             source monitoring explain a developmental
             reversal?},
   Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Development},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {607-616},
   Publisher = {Informa UK Limited},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.710864},
   Abstract = {Children’s memories improve throughout childhood, and this
             improvement is often accompanied by a reduction in
             suggestibility. In this context, it is surprising that older
             children learn and reproduce more factual errors from
             stories than do younger children (Fazio & Marsh, 2008). The
             present study examined whether this developmental reversal
             is limited to production tests, or whether younger children
             are still less suggestible when choosing between the correct
             answer and story error on a multiple-choice test. A second
             goal was to further explore the role of source monitoring in
             children’s suggestibility, by examining children’s
             awareness of learning within versus before the experiment.
             Five-year-olds and 7-year-olds listened to stories
             containing correct, neutral and misleading factual
             references, and later took either a multiple-choice or
             short-answer general knowledge test. In addition, they
             judged whether each answer had occurred in the stories and
             whether they had known it before the experiment. Critically,
             a developmental reversal in suggestibility was observed on
             both tests; younger children were less suggestible even when
             faced with the story errors at test. Although older children
             showed superior source discriminability for whether their
             answers had appeared in the stories, they showed an illusion
             of prior knowledge, believing they had known their
             misinformation answers all along. To this effect, older
             children’s increased suggestibility may be due not only to
             their superior memory capacity for specific story errors,
             but also to their ability and tendency to integrate story
             information into their knowledge base.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15248372.2012.710864},
   Key = {fds252752}
}

@article{fds252753,
   Author = {Wing, EA and Marsh, EJ and Cabeza, R},
   Title = {Neural correlates of retrieval-based memory enhancement: An
             fMRI study of the testing effect},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologica},
   Volume = {51},
   Number = {12},
   Pages = {2360-2370},
   Year = {2012},
   ISSN = {0028-3932},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.004},
   Abstract = {Restudying material is a common method for learning new
             information, but not necessarily an effective one. Research
             on the testing effect shows that practice involving
             retrieval from memory can facilitate later memory in
             contrast to passive restudy. Despite extensive behavioral
             work, the brain processes that make retrieval an effective
             learning strategy remain unclear. In the present experiment,
             we explored how initially retrieving items affected memory a
             day later as compared to a condition involving traditional
             restudy. In contrast to restudy, initial testing that
             contributed to future memory success was associated with
             engagement of several regions including the anterior
             hippocampus, lateral temporal cortices, and medial
             prefrontal cortex (PFC). Additionally, testing enhanced
             hippocampal connectivity with ventrolateral PFC and midline
             regions. These findings indicate that the testing effect may
             be contingent on processes that are typically thought to
             support memory success at encoding (e.g. relational binding,
             selection and elaboration of semantically-related
             information) in addition to those more often associated with
             retrieval (e.g. memory search).},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.004},
   Key = {fds252753}
}

@article{fds252754,
   Author = {Umanath, S and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Understanding how prior knowledge influences memory in older
             adults},
   Journal = {Perspectives on Psychological Science},
   Volume = {9},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {408-426},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691614535933},
   Abstract = {In aging, episodic memory function shows serious declines,
             whereas ability to use one’s general knowledge either
             improves or remains stable over the lifespan. Our focus is
             on the often overlooked but critical role of intact prior
             knowledge as a factor that contributes to older adults’
             episodic memory performance. Here, we describe the negative
             and the less often discussed positive influences of prior
             knowledge on older adults’ memories. We address when prior
             knowledge supports versus leads remembering astray in aging,
             considering the roles of episodic memory failures, the
             content of to-be-remembered information, and explicit
             instructions to apply prior knowledge. Overall, we argue
             that prior knowledge is a key factor in understanding older
             adults’ memory performance, influencing memory through
             proactive interference, and has the potential to serve as a
             compensatory mechanism.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1745691614535933},
   Key = {fds252754}
}

@article{fds252755,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Godbole, N and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Explanation feedback is better than correct answer feedback
             for promoting transfer of learning},
   Journal = {Journal of Educational Psychology},
   Volume = {105},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {290-298},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031026},
   Abstract = {Among the many factors that influence the efficacy of
             feedback on learning, the information contained in the
             feedback message is arguably the most important.
             Surprisingly, prior research has produced little evidence to
             suggest that there is a benefit to increasing the complexity
             of the feedback message beyond providing the correct answer.
             However, the final test in most of these studies consisted
             of a repetition of the same questions from the initial test.
             The present research investigated whether feedback that
             provides an explanation of the correct answer promotes
             superior transfer of learning to new questions. In two
             experiments, participants studied prose passages and then
             took an initial short answer test on concepts from the text.
             After each question, they received correct answer feedback,
             explanation feedback, or no feedback. Two days later,
             participants returned for a final test that consisted of
             both repeated questions and new transfer questions. The
             results showed that correct answer feedback and explanation
             feedback led to equivalent performance on the repeated
             questions, but explanation feedback produced superior
             performance on the new transfer questions.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0031026},
   Key = {fds252755}
}

@article{fds252756,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Barber, SJ and Rajaram, S and Ornstein, PA and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Creating illusions of knowledge: Learning errors that
             contradict prior knowledge},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: General},
   Volume = {142},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-5},
   Year = {2012},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028649},
   Abstract = {Most people know that the Pacific is the largest ocean on
             Earth and that Edison invented the light bulb. Our question
             is whether this knowledge is stable, or if people will
             incorporate errors into their knowledge bases, even if they
             have the correct knowledge stored in memory. Subjects
             answered general knowledge questions two weeks before
             reading stories that contained errors (e.g., “Franklin
             invented the light bulb”). On a later general knowledge
             test, subjects reproduced story errors despite previously
             answering the questions correctly. This misinformation
             effect was found even for questions that were answered
             correctly on the initial test with the highest level of
             confidence. Illusions of knowledge do not depend on ignorant
             learners; errors can enter the knowledge base even when
             learners have the knowledge necessary to catch the
             errors.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0028649},
   Key = {fds252756}
}

@article{fds252762,
   Author = {Umanath, S and Butler, AC and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Using popular films to enhance classroom learning: Mnemonic
             effects of monitoring misinformation},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {26},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {556-567},
   Year = {2012},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.2827},
   Abstract = {History educators often use popular films in the classroom
             to teach critical thinking through an exercise that involves
             identifying historical inaccuracies in the films. We
             investigated how this exercise affects the acquisition of
             true and false historical knowledge. In two experiments,
             subjects studied texts about historical topics and watched
             clips from corresponding films. Each film contained one
             piece of information that contradicted the text (i.e.,
             misinformation). Some subjects received instructions to
             monitor for inaccuracies in the films. After a delay, they
             were tested on the texts. Monitoring instructions did not
             reduce subjects’ acquisition of misinformation, and even
             when subjects successfully detected the inaccuracies, they
             sometimes still reproduced the misinformation. However, when
             they received feedback about the inaccuracies, the
             production of misinformation was substantially reduced.
             Overall, these findings indicate that educators should
             provide feedback when using popular films for this critical
             thinking exercise so that students do not acquire false
             knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.2827},
   Key = {fds252762}
}

@article{fds358867,
   Author = {Dunlosky, J and Rawson, KA and Marsh, EJ and Nathan, MJ and Willingham,
             DT},
   Title = {Improving students' learning and comprehension: Promising
             directions from cognitive and educational
             psychology},
   Journal = {Psychological Science in the Public Interest},
   Year = {2012},
   Key = {fds358867}
}

@article{fds252765,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {The hypercorrection effect persists over a week, but
             high-confidence errors return.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1238-1244},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21989771},
   Abstract = {People's knowledge about the world often contains
             misconceptions that are well-learned and firmly believed.
             Although such misconceptions seem hard to correct, recent
             research has demonstrated that errors made with higher
             confidence are more likely to be corrected with feedback, a
             finding called the hypercorrection effect. We investigated
             whether this effect persists over a 1-week delay. Subjects
             answered general-knowledge questions about science, rated
             their confidence in each response, and received correct
             answer feedback. Half of the subjects reanswered the same
             questions immediately, while the other half reanswered them
             after a 1-week delay. The hypercorrection effect occurred on
             both the immediate and delayed final tests, but error
             correction decreased on the delayed test. When subjects
             failed to correct an error on the delayed test, they
             sometimes reproduced the same error from the initial test.
             Interestingly, high-confidence errors were more likely than
             low-confidence errors to be reproduced on the delayed test.
             These findings help to contextualize the hypercorrection
             effect within the broader memory literature by showing that
             high-confidence errors are more likely to be corrected, but
             they are also more likely to be reproduced if the correct
             answer is forgotten.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-011-0173-y},
   Key = {fds252765}
}

@article{fds252764,
   Author = {Eslick, AN and Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Ironic effects of drawing attention to story
             errors.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {184-191},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21294039},
   Abstract = {Readers learn errors embedded in fictional stories and use
             them to answer later general knowledge questions (Marsh,
             Meade, & Roediger, 2003). Suggestibility is robust and
             occurs even when story errors contradict well-known facts.
             The current study evaluated whether suggestibility is linked
             to participants' inability to judge story content as correct
             versus incorrect. Specifically, participants read stories
             containing correct and misleading information about the
             world; some information was familiar (making error discovery
             possible), while some was more obscure. To improve
             participants' monitoring ability, we highlighted (in red
             font) a subset of story phrases requiring evaluation;
             readers no longer needed to find factual information.
             Rather, they simply needed to evaluate its correctness.
             Readers were more likely to answer questions with story
             errors if they were highlighted in red font, even if they
             contradicted well-known facts. Although highlighting
             to-be-evaluated information freed cognitive resources for
             monitoring, an ironic effect occurred: Drawing attention to
             specific errors increased rather than decreased later
             suggestibility. Failure to monitor for errors, not failure
             to identify the information requiring evaluation, leads to
             suggestibility.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2010.543908},
   Key = {fds252764}
}

@article{fds252766,
   Author = {Bottoms, HC and Eslick, AN and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Memory and the Moses illusion: failures to detect
             contradictions with stored knowledge yield negative memorial
             consequences.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {670-678},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20706955},
   Abstract = {Although contradictions with stored knowledge are common in
             daily life, people often fail to notice them. For example,
             in the Moses illusion, participants fail to notice errors in
             questions such as "How many animals of each kind did Moses
             take on the Ark?" despite later showing knowledge that the
             Biblical reference is to Noah, not Moses. We examined
             whether error prevalence affected participants' ability to
             detect distortions in questions, and whether this in turn
             had memorial consequences. Many of the errors were
             overlooked, but participants were better able to catch them
             when they were more common. More generally, the failure to
             detect errors had negative memorial consequences, increasing
             the likelihood that the errors were used to answer later
             general knowledge questions. Methodological implications of
             this finding are discussed, as it suggests that typical
             analyses likely underestimate the size of the Moses
             illusion. Overall, answering distorted questions can yield
             errors in the knowledge base; most importantly, prior
             knowledge does not protect against these negative memorial
             consequences.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211.2010.501558},
   Key = {fds252766}
}

@article{fds252769,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Sink, HE},
   Title = {Access to handouts of presentation slides during lecture:
             Consequences for learning},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {691-706},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1579},
   Abstract = {Teachers often lecture with presentation software such as
             Microsoft PowerPoint; however, little research has examined
             the effects of this new technology on learning. One issue
             that arises is whether or not to give students copies of the
             lecture slides, and if so when. A survey documented that
             students prefer to receive lecture slides before class,
             whereas instructors were less pronounced in their
             preferences. Two experiments examined whether having
             handouts of the slides facilitated encoding of science
             lectures. Having access to handouts of the slides during
             lecture was associated with a number of benefits: less
             note-taking (studies 1 and 2), less time needed to prepare
             for a final test (study 1), and better performance on the
             final test (study 2). Overall, receiving handouts before
             lecture helped efficient encoding of the lecture. © 2009
             John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1579},
   Key = {fds252769}
}

@article{fds252767,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Correcting false memories.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {801-803},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20483825},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797610371341},
   Key = {fds252767}
}

@article{fds252770,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Agarwal, PK and Marsh, EJ and Roediger,
             HL},
   Title = {Memorial consequences of multiple-choice testing on
             immediate and delayed tests.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {38},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {407-418},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20516221},
   Abstract = {Multiple-choice testing has both positive and negative
             consequences for performance on later tests. Prior testing
             increases the number of questions answered correctly on a
             later test but also increases the likelihood that questions
             will be answered with lures from the previous
             multiple-choice test (Roediger & Marsh, 2005). Prior
             research has shown that the positive effects of testing
             persist over a delay, but no one has examined the durability
             of the negative effects of testing. To address this,
             subjects took multiple-choice and cued recall tests (on
             subsets of questions) both immediately and a week after
             studying. Although delay reduced both the positive and
             negative testing effects, both still occurred after 1 week,
             especially if the multiple-choice test had also been
             delayed. These results are consistent with the argument that
             recollection underlies both the positive and negative
             testing effects.},
   Doi = {10.3758/mc.38.4.407},
   Key = {fds252770}
}

@article{fds252768,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Huelser, BJ and Johnson, A and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Receiving right/wrong feedback: consequences for
             learning.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {335-350},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20408043},
   Abstract = {Prior work suggests that receiving feedback that one's
             response was correct or incorrect (right/wrong feedback)
             does not help learners, as compared to not receiving any
             feedback at all (Pashler, Cepeda, Wixted, & Rohrer, 2005).
             In three experiments we examined the generality of this
             conclusion. Right/wrong feedback did not aid error
             correction, regardless of whether participants learned facts
             embedded in prose (Experiment 1) or translations of foreign
             vocabulary (Experiment 2). While right/wrong feedback did
             not improve the overall retention of correct answers
             (Experiments 1 and 2), it facilitated retention of
             low-confidence correct answers (Experiment 3). Reviewing the
             original materials was very useful to learners, but this
             benefit was similar after receiving either right/wrong
             feedback or no feedback (Experiments 1 and 2). Overall,
             right/wrong feedback conveys some information to the
             learner, but is not nearly as useful as being told the
             correct answer or having the chance to review the
             to-be-learned materials.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658211003652491},
   Key = {fds252768}
}

@article{fds252745,
   Author = {Brown, AS and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Digging into Déjà Vu: Recent Research on Possible
             Mechanisms},
   Volume = {53},
   Number = {C},
   Pages = {33-62},
   Booktitle = {The Psychology of Learning and Motivation},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Editor = {Brian Ross},
   Year = {2010},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0079-7421},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000278834500002&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {The déjà vu experience has piqued the interest of
             philosophers and physicians for over 150 years, and has
             recently begun to connect to research on fundamental
             cognitive mechanisms. Following a brief description of the
             nature of this recognition anomaly, this chapter summarizes
             findings from several laboratories that are related to this
             memory phenomenon. In our labs, we have found support for
             three possible mechanisms that could trigger déjà vu. The
             first is split perception, which posits that a déjà vu is
             caused by a brief glance at an object or scene just prior to
             a fully aware look. Thus, the perception is split into two
             parts and appears to be eerily duplicated. A second
             mechanism is implicit memory, whereby a prior setting
             actually has been experienced before by the person but
             stored in such an indistinct manner that only the sense of
             familiarity is resurrected. Another example of an implicit
             memory effect involves a single part of a larger scene that
             is familiar but not identified as such, with the result that
             the strong sense of familiarity associated with this portion
             inappropriately bleeds over onto the entire scene. Others
             have found support for gestalt familiarity, that the
             framework of the present setting closely resembles something
             experienced before in outline but not in specifics. We also
             present physiological evidence from brain and cognitive
             dysfunctions that relate to our understanding of déjà vu.
             Finally, some important but unresolved issues in déjà vu
             research are noted, ones that should guide future research
             on the topic. © 2010, Elsevier Inc. All rights
             reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0079-7421(10)53002-0},
   Key = {fds252745}
}

@article{fds252761,
   Author = {Brown, AS and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Creating illusions of past encounter through brief
             exposure.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {534-538},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {May},
   ISSN = {0956-7976},
   url = {http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000265774700002&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=47d3190e77e5a3a53558812f597b0b92},
   Abstract = {Titchener (1928) suggested that briefly glancing at a scene
             could make it appear strangely familiar when it was fully
             processed moments later. The closest laboratory
             demonstration used words as stimuli, and showed that briefly
             glancing at a to-be-judged word increased the subject's
             belief that it had been presented in an earlier study list
             (Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989). We evaluated whether a hasty
             glance could elicit a false belief in a prior encounter,
             from a time and place outside of the experiment. This goal
             precluded using word stimuli, so we had subjects evaluate
             unfamiliar symbols. Each symbol was preceded by a brief
             exposure to an identical symbol, a different symbol, or no
             symbol. A brief glance at an identical symbol increased
             attributions to preexperimental experience, relative to a
             glance at a different symbol or no symbol, providing a
             possible mechanism for common illusions of false
             recognition.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02337.x},
   Key = {fds252761}
}

@article{fds252771,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Agarwal, PK and Roediger, HL},
   Title = {Memorial consequences of answering SAT II
             questions.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Applied},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-11},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {1076-898X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19309212},
   Abstract = {Many thousands of students take standardized tests every
             year. In the current research, we asked whether answering
             standardized test questions affects students' later test
             performance. Prior research has shown both positive and
             negative effects of multiple-choice testing on later tests,
             with negative effects arising from students selecting
             incorrect alternatives on multiple-choice tests and then
             believing they were correct (Roediger & Marsh, 2005). In the
             current experiments, undergraduates and high school students
             answered multiple-choice questions retired from SAT II tests
             (that are no longer in the testing pool) on biology,
             chemistry, U.S. history, and world history, and later
             answered cued-recall questions about these subjects. In 3
             experiments, we observed positive testing effects: More
             final cued-recall questions were answered correctly if the
             items had appeared on the initial multiple-choice test. We
             also sometimes observed negative testing effects: intrusions
             of multiple-choice distractors as answers on the final
             cued-recall test. Students who scored well on the initial
             test benefited from taking the test, but lower achieving
             students showed either less benefit (undergraduates) or
             costs from the testing (high school students).},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0014721},
   Key = {fds252771}
}

@article{fds252772,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Surprising feedback improves later memory.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {88-92},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19145015},
   Abstract = {The hypercorrection effect is the finding that
             high-confidence errors are more likely to be corrected after
             feedback than are low-confidence errors (Butterfield &
             Metcalfe, 2001). In two experiments, we explored the idea
             that the hypercorrection effect results from increased
             attention to surprising feedback. In Experiment 1,
             participants were more likely to remember the appearance of
             the presented feedback when the feedback did not match
             expectations. In Experiment 2, we replicated this effect
             using more distinctive sources and also demonstrated the
             hypercorrection effect in this modified paradigm. Overall,
             participants better remembered both the surface features and
             the content of surprising feedback.},
   Doi = {10.3758/pbr.16.1.88},
   Key = {fds252772}
}

@article{fds252773,
   Author = {Barber, SJ and Rajaram, S and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Fact learning: how information accuracy, delay, and repeated
             testing change retention and retrieval experience.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {934-946},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210802360603},
   Abstract = {Previous classroom studies have shown that the phenomenology
             of studied facts changes over time. However, pedagogical
             needs preclude both the study of errors and the separation
             of the effects that delay and repeated testing have on
             retention and retrieval experience. We addressed these
             issues together in an experiment where participants read
             stories containing correct and misleading information and
             provided Remember, Just Know, and Familiar judgements on
             immediate and delayed general knowledge tests. After 2 days,
             information learned from the stories shifted from Remembered
             to Just Known, but repeated testing attenuated this shift.
             Interestingly, similar patterns of retrieval and
             phenomenology were observed for correct and misleading
             information with one important difference--the shift over
             time to Just Knowing was significantly greater for correct
             than for misleading information. Together, these findings
             show the roles of information accuracy, delay, and testing
             in determining both retention and the subjective experience
             of retrieval.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210802360603},
   Key = {fds252773}
}

@article{fds304690,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Slowing presentation speed increases illusions of
             knowledge.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {180-185},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18605500},
   Abstract = {Prior research on false memories has shown that
             suggestibility is often reduced when the presentation rate
             is slowed enough to allow monitoring. We examined whether
             slowing presentation speed would reduce factual errors
             learned from fictional stories. Would subjects use the extra
             time to detect the errors in the stories, reducing
             reproduction of these errors on a later test? Surprisingly,
             slowing presentation speed increased the production of story
             errors on a later general knowledge test. Instructing the
             reader to mark whether each sentence contained an error,
             however, did decrease suggestibility. Readers appear to
             passively accept information presented in stories and need a
             constant reminder to monitor for errors. These results
             highlight differences between typical episodic false
             memories and illusions of knowledge (such as learning from
             fiction). Manipulations that reduce suggestibility for
             episodic false memories do not always reduce suggestibility
             for illusions of knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.3758/pbr.15.1.180},
   Key = {fds304690}
}

@article{fds252774,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Older, not younger, children learn more false facts from
             stories.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {106},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {1081-1089},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0010-0277},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17540354},
   Abstract = {Early school-aged children listened to stories that
             contained correct and incorrect facts. All ages answered
             more questions correctly after having heard the correct fact
             in the story. Only the older children, however, produced
             story errors on a later general knowledge test. Source
             errors did not drive the increased suggestibility in older
             children, as they were better at remembering source than
             were the younger children. Instead, different processes are
             involved in learning correct and incorrect facts from
             fictional sources. All ages benefited from hearing correct
             answers because they activated a pre-existing semantic
             network. Older children, however, were better able to form
             memories of the misinformation and thus showed greater
             suggestibility on the general knowledge test.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.012},
   Key = {fds252774}
}

@article{fds252775,
   Author = {Brown, AS and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Evoking false beliefs about autobiographical
             experience.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {186-190},
   Year = {2008},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.1.186},
   Abstract = {In two experiments, we demonstrate that laboratory
             procedures can evoke false beliefs about autobiographical
             experience. After shallowly processing photographs
             ofreal-world locations, participants returned 1 week
             (Experiments 1 and 2) or 3 weeks (Experiment 2) later to
             evaluate whether they had actually visited each of a series
             of new and old pictured locations. Mundane and unique scenes
             from an unfamiliar college campus (Duke or SMU) were shown
             zero, one, or two times in the first session. Prior exposure
             increased participants' beliefs that they had visited
             locations that they had never actually visited. Furthermore,
             participants gave higher visit ratings to mundane than to
             unique scenes, and this did not vary with exposure frequency
             or delay. This laboratory procedure for inducing
             autobiographical false beliefs may have implications for
             better understanding various illusions of
             recognition.},
   Doi = {10.3758/pbr.15.1.186},
   Key = {fds252775}
}

@article{fds252776,
   Author = {Fazio, LK and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Slowing presentation speed increases illusions of
             knowledge},
   Journal = {Psychonomic Bulletin & Review},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {181-185},
   Year = {2008},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18605500},
   Abstract = {Prior research on false memories shows that suggestibility
             is often reduced when the presentation rate is slowed enough
             to allow monitoring. We examined whether slowing
             presentation speed would reduce factual errors learned from
             fictional stories. Would subjects use the extra time to
             detect the errors in the stories, reducing their
             reproduction on a later test? Surprisingly, slowing
             presentation speed increased the production of story errors
             on a later general knowledge test. Instructing the reader to
             mark whether each sentence contained an error, however, did
             decrease suggestibility. Readers appear to passively accept
             information presented in stories, and need a constant
             reminder to monitor for errors. These results highlight
             differences between typical episodic false memories and
             illusions of knowledge (such as learning from fiction).
             Manipulations that reduce suggestibility for episodic false
             memories do not always reduce suggestibility for illusions
             of knowledge.},
   Key = {fds252776}
}

@article{fds252778,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Dolan, PO},
   Title = {Test-induced priming of false memories.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {479-483},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {June},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17874592},
   Abstract = {Of interest was whether prior testing of related words
             primes false memories in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM)
             paradigm. After studying lists of related words, subjects
             made old-new judgments about zero, three, or six related
             items before being tested on critical nonpresented lures.
             When the recognition test was self-paced, prior testing of
             list items led to faster false recognition judgments, but
             did not increase the rate of false alarms to lures from
             studied lists. Critically, this pattern changed when
             decision making at test was speeded. When forced to respond
             quickly--presumably precluding the use of monitoring
             processes--clear test-induced priming effects were observed
             in the rate of false memories. The results are consistent
             with an activation-monitoring explanation of false memories
             and support that retrieving veridical memories can be a
             source of memory error.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03194093},
   Key = {fds252778}
}

@article{fds252777,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Roediger, HL and Bjork, RA and Bjork,
             EL},
   Title = {The memorial consequences of multiple-choice
             testing.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {194-199},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {April},
   ISSN = {1069-9384},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17694900},
   Abstract = {The present article addresses whether multiple-choice tests
             may change knowledge even as they attempt to measure it.
             Overall, taking a multiple-choice test boosts performance on
             later tests, as compared with non-tested control conditions.
             This benefit is not limited to simple definitional
             questions, but holds true for SAT II questions and for items
             designed to tap concepts at a higher level in Bloom's (1956)
             taxonomy of educational objectives. Students, however, can
             also learn false facts from multiple-choice tests; testing
             leads to persistence of some multiple-choice lures on later
             general knowledge tests. Such persistence appears due to
             faulty reasoning rather than to an increase in the
             familiarity of lures. Even though students may learn false
             facts from multiple-choice tests, the positive effects of
             testing outweigh this cost.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03194051},
   Key = {fds252777}
}

@article{fds252779,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Retelling is not the same as recalling: Implications for
             memory},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {16},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {16-20},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {February},
   ISSN = {0963-7214},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00467.x},
   Abstract = {In contrast to laboratory free recall (which emphasizes
             detailed and accurate remembering), conversational
             retellings depend upon the speaker's goals, the audience,
             and the social context more generally. Because memories are
             frequently retrieved in social contexts, retellings of
             events are often incomplete or distorted, with consequences
             for later memory. Selective rehearsal contributes to the
             memory effects, as does the schema activated during
             retelling. Retellings can be linked to memory errors
             observed in domains such as eyewitness testimony and
             flashbulb memories; in all of these situations, people
             retell events rather than engage in verbatim remembering.
             Copyright © 2007 Association for Psychological
             Science.},
   Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00467.x},
   Key = {fds252779}
}

@article{fds252780,
   Author = {Butler, AC and Marsh, EJ and Goode, MK and Roediger,
             HL},
   Title = {When additional multiple-choice lures aid versus hinder
             later memory},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {941-956},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0888-4080},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1239},
   Abstract = {Three experiments were conducted to investigate whether
             increasing the number of lures on a multiple-choice test
             helps, hinders or has no effect on later memory. All three
             patterns have been reported in the literature. In Experiment
             1, the stimuli were unrelated word lists, and increasing the
             number of lures on an initial multiple-choice test led to
             better performance on later free recall and cued recall
             tasks. In contrast, in Experiments 2 and 3, stimuli were
             facts from prose materials, and increasing the number of
             multiple-choice lures led to robust costs in cued recall and
             smaller costs in free recall. The results indicate that
             performance on the initial multiple-choice test is a
             critical factor. When initial multiple-choice performance
             was near ceiling, testing with additional lures led to
             superior performance on subsequent tests. However, at lower
             levels of multiple-choice performance, testing with
             additional lures produced costs on later test. Copyright ©
             2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1239},
   Key = {fds252780}
}

@article{fds252782,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {When does generation enhance memory for location?},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and
             cognition},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1216-1220},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0278-7393},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16938059},
   Abstract = {Generation is thought to enhance both item-specific and
             relational processing of generated targets as compared with
             read words (M. A. McDaniel & P. J. Waddill, 1990).
             Generation facilitates encoding of the cue-target relation
             and sometimes boosts encoding of relations across list
             items. Of interest is whether generation can also increase
             the encoding of target-location associations. Because the
             literature on this point is mixed, 3 procedural differences
             between 2 studies (E. J. Marsh, G. Edelman, & G. H. Bower,
             2001; N. W. Mulligan, 2004) were identified and manipulated.
             A positive generation effect was found for location memory,
             but this effect was reduced when subjects wrote down the
             study words and when the filler task involved generation.
             Generation can enhance location memory in addition to item
             memory but only if the experimental parameters do not
             interfere with the processing benefits of
             generation.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1216},
   Key = {fds252782}
}

@article{fds252781,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Fazio, LK},
   Title = {Learning errors from fiction: difficulties in reducing
             reliance on fictional stories.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1140-1149},
   Year = {2006},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0090-502X},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17128612},
   Abstract = {Readers rely on fiction as a source of information, even
             when fiction contradicts relatively well-known facts about
             the world (Marsh, Meade, and Roediger, 2003). Of interest
             was whether readers could monitor fiction for errors, in
             order to reduce suggestibility. In Experiment 1, warnings
             about errors in fiction did not reduce students' reliance on
             stories. In Experiment 2, all subjects were warned before
             reading stories written at 6th- or 12th-grade reading
             levels. Even though 6th-grade stories freed resources for
             monitoring, suggestibility was not reduced. In Experiment 3,
             suggestibility was reduced but not eliminated when subjects
             pressed a key each time they detected an error during story
             reading. Readers do not appear to spontaneously monitor
             fiction for its veracity, but can do so if reminded on a
             trial-by-trial basis.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03193260},
   Key = {fds252781}
}

@article{fds252783,
   Author = {Roediger, HL and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {The positive and negative consequences of multiple-choice
             testing.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and
             cognition},
   Volume = {31},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1155-1159},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {September},
   ISSN = {0278-7393},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.1155},
   Abstract = {Multiple-choice tests are commonly used in educational
             settings but with unknown effects on students' knowledge.
             The authors examined the consequences of taking a
             multiple-choice test on a later general knowledge test in
             which students were warned not to guess. A large positive
             testing effect was obtained: Prior testing of facts aided
             final cued-recall performance. However, prior testing also
             had negative consequences. Prior reading of a greater number
             of multiple-choice lures decreased the positive testing
             effect and increased production of multiple-choice lures as
             incorrect answers on the final test. Multiple-choice testing
             may inadvertently lead to the creation of false
             knowledge.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.1155},
   Key = {fds252783}
}

@article{fds252784,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Tversky, B and Hutson, M},
   Title = {How eyewitnesses talk about events: Implications for
             memory},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {531-544},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1095},
   Abstract = {Eyewitnesses to traumatic events typically talk about them,
             and they may do so for different reasons. Of interest was
             whether qualitatively different retellings would lead to
             differences in later memory. All participants watched a
             violent film scene; one third talked about their emotional
             reactions to the film (as one might do when talking to a
             friend), one third described the events of the film (as the
             police might request), and one third did unrelated tasks.
             Following a delay, all participants were tested on their
             memories for the clip. Talking about emotions led to better
             memory for one's emotions, but also led to subjectivity and
             a greater proportion of major errors in free recall.
             Differences were minimized on tests providing more retrieval
             cues, suggesting that retellings' consequences for memory
             are greater when retellers have to generate their own
             retrieval structures. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1095},
   Key = {fds252784}
}

@article{fds252748,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Balota, DA and Roediger, HL},
   Title = {Learning facts from fiction: effects of healthy aging and
             early-stage dementia of the Alzheimer type.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychology},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {115-129},
   Year = {2005},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0894-4105},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15656769},
   Abstract = {Healthy younger and older adults and individuals with very
             mild or mild dementia of the Alzheimer type (DAT) listened
             to and read fictional stories containing correct and
             incorrect facts about the world. Of interest was their use
             of this story information to answer questions on a later
             test of general world knowledge. Prior exposure to
             relatively well-known facts boosted all subjects' ability to
             correctly answer general knowledge questions. Reading
             incorrect facts in the stories led to misinformation effects
             in healthy older adults (although these effects were smaller
             than those observed in younger adults). DAT individuals
             showed reduced effects of story exposure; effects were
             greatest in a situation that reminded DAT individuals that
             the stories might provide the answers to the questions.
             Benefits of story reading depended on activation of the
             semantic network, whereas costs of story reading were more
             dependent on episodic memory processes.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0894-4105.19.1.115},
   Key = {fds252748}
}

@article{fds252746,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Bower, GH},
   Title = {The role of rehearsal and generation in false memory
             creation.},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {748-761},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000170},
   Abstract = {The current research investigated one possible mechanism
             underlying false memories in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott
             (DRM) paradigm. In the DRM paradigm, participants who study
             lists of related words (e.g., "table, sitting, bench ...")
             frequently report detailed memories for the centrally
             related but non-presented critical lure (e.g., "chair"). One
             possibility is that participants covertly call to mind the
             critical non-presented lure during the study phase, and
             later misattribute memory for this internally generated
             event to its external presentation. To investigate this, the
             DRM paradigm was modified to allow collection of on-line
             thoughts during the study phase. False recognition increased
             following generation during study. False recognition also
             increased following study of longer lists; this effect was
             partially explained by the fact that longer lists were more
             likely to elicit generations of the critical lure during
             study. Generation of the lure during study contributes to
             later false recognition, although it does not explain the
             entire effect.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210344000170},
   Key = {fds252746}
}

@article{fds252747,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Story stimuli for creating false beliefs about the
             world.},
   Journal = {Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a
             journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc},
   Volume = {36},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {650-655},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {November},
   ISSN = {0743-3808},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15641411},
   Abstract = {Fiction is not always accurate, and this has consequences
             for readers. In laboratory studies, the reading of short
             stories led participants to produce story errors as facts on
             a later test of general knowledge (Marsh, Meade, & Roediger,
             2003). The present article describes these story stimuli in
             detail, so that interested researchers will be able to use
             the stimuli and change them as needed for particular
             research projects. This article provides instructions for
             using the stories and suggestions for modifying them; it is
             a manual for one way of creating suggestibility. The full
             set of stories and reading comprehension questions may be
             downloaded from www.psychonomic.org/archive/.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03206546},
   Key = {fds252747}
}

@article{fds252785,
   Author = {Luminet, O and Curci, A and Marsh, EJ and Wessel, I and Constantin, T and Gencoz, F and Yogo, M},
   Title = {The cognitive, emotional, and social impacts of the
             September 11 attacks: group differences in memory for the
             reception context and the determinants of flashbulb
             memory.},
   Journal = {The Journal of general psychology},
   Volume = {131},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {197-224},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {July},
   ISSN = {0022-1309},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/genp.131.3.197-224},
   Abstract = {The authors examined group differences in memories for
             hearing the news of and reactions to the September 11
             attacks in 2001. They measured memory for reception context
             (immediate memory for the circumstances in which people
             first heard the news) and 11 predictors of the consistency
             of memory for reception context over time (flashbulb
             memory). Shortly after 9/11, a questionnaire was distributed
             to 3,665 participants in 9 countries. U.S. vs. non-U.S.
             respondents showed large differences in self-rated
             importance of the news and in memory for event-related
             facts. The groups showed moderate differences in background
             knowledge and emotional-feeling states. Within non-U.S.
             groups, there were large differences for emotional-feeling
             states and moderate differences for personal rehearsal,
             background knowledge, and attitudes toward the United
             States. The authors discuss the implications of those
             findings for the study of group differences in memory and
             for the formation of flashbulb memories.},
   Doi = {10.3200/genp.131.3.197-224},
   Key = {fds252785}
}

@article{fds252787,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Tversky, B},
   Title = {Spinning the stories of our lives},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {491-503},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1001},
   Abstract = {The way people talk about past events can affect the way
             they remember them (Tversky & Marsh, 2000). The current
             research explores how people naturally talk about events
             from their own lives. Participants recorded what, when, and
             how they told others about events from their lives. In
             general, participants talked about recent emotional events,
             and told them primarily to peers in order to convey facts
             and/or to entertain. Not all distorted retellings were
             regarded as 'inaccurate.' Participants labeled 61% of their
             retellings as distorted (containing exaggerations,
             omissions, minimizations, or additions) but only 42% of
             their retellings as inaccurate. Social context shaped the
             stories people told: they changed stories for different
             audiences; they exaggerated to entertain and simplified to
             inform. People construct stories as they retrieve and use
             memories in a social context. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley &
             Sons, Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.1001},
   Key = {fds252787}
}

@article{fds252786,
   Author = {Dudukovic, NM and Marsh, EJ and Tversky, B},
   Title = {Telling a story or telling it straight: The effects of
             entertaining versus accurate retellings on
             memory},
   Journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
   Volume = {18},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {125-143},
   Publisher = {WILEY},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.953},
   Abstract = {People retell events for different reasons. Sometimes they
             try to be accurate, other times entertaining. What
             characterizes retellings from different perspectives? How
             does retelling perspective affect later recall of events? In
             the current research, participants retold a story either
             three times or not at all. By instruction, retellings were
             either entertaining or accurate. Compared to accurate
             retellings, entertaining retellings contained more affect,
             but fewer sensory references. On a subsequent memory test,
             participants who retold with an accuracy goal recalled the
             greatest number of story events, and their recall protocols
             were the most accurate and detailed, and least exaggerated.
             However, recognition memory did not differ across groups,
             suggesting that differences in retrieval structures
             (necessary for recall but not recognition) were key to
             understanding later differences in memory. Compared to
             telling it straight, the creative process of telling a story
             leads to qualitative and quantitative changes in later
             recall. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley & Sons,
             Ltd.},
   Doi = {10.1002/acp.953},
   Key = {fds252786}
}

@article{fds252788,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Dolan, PO and Balota, DA and Roediger,
             HL},
   Title = {Part-set cuing effects in younger and older
             adults.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {19},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {134-144},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {March},
   ISSN = {0882-7974},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15065937},
   Abstract = {In 3 experiments, the authors examined part-set cuing
             effects in younger and older adults. Participants heard
             lists of category exemplars and later recalled them. Recall
             was uncued or cued with a subset of studied items. In
             Experiment 1, participants were cued with some of the
             category names, and they remembered fewer never-cued
             categories than a free-recall condition. In Experiment 2, a
             similar effect was observed for category exemplar cues.
             There was also an age difference: By some measures, a small
             number of cues impaired older adults more than younger.
             Experiment 3 replicated this result and found that older
             adults were disproportionately slow in the presence of cues.
             Across experiments, older adults showed robust part-set
             cuing effects, and sometimes, they were disproportionately
             impaired by cues.},
   Doi = {10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.134},
   Key = {fds252788}
}

@article{fds252789,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and McDermott, KB and Roediger, HL},
   Title = {Does test-induced priming play a role in the creation of
             false memories?},
   Journal = {Memory (Hove, England)},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {44-55},
   Year = {2004},
   Month = {January},
   ISSN = {0965-8211},
   url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15098620},
   Abstract = {We investigated the role of test-induced priming in creating
             false memories in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM)
             paradigm, in which subjects study lists of related words
             (bed, rest, awake) and then falsely recall or recognise a
             related word (sleep) on a later test. However, in
             experiments using three different procedures, we found that
             the number of related words tested prior to the critical
             word had surprisingly little impact on false recall and
             recognition. We manipulated the location of the critical
             item in tests of yes/no recognition, word-stem cued recall,
             and part-set cued recall. We consistently obtained high
             probabilities of false recall and recognition, but the
             probability was unaffected by the number of related items
             presented prior to the test of the critical item.
             Surprisingly, test-induced priming of the critical item does
             not seem to play a large role in this memory
             illusion.},
   Doi = {10.1080/09658210244000405},
   Key = {fds252789}
}

@article{fds252793,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Meade, ML and Roediger, HL},
   Title = {Learning facts from fiction},
   Journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
   Volume = {49},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {519-536},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2003},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00092-5},
   Abstract = {People's knowledge about the world comes from many sources,
             including fictional ones such as movies and novels. In three
             experiments, we investigated how people learn and integrate
             information from fictional sources with their general world
             knowledge. Subjects read a series of short stories that
             contained information about the real world. After a short
             delay, all participants took a general knowledge test.
             Subjects did indeed use information from the stories to
             answer general knowledge questions. Prior reading of facts
             boosted participants' abilities to produce both obscure and
             better-known facts, and the effect held for both correct and
             incorrect facts (misinformation). Repeated reading of the
             stories increased the effect. After a delay of one week,
             effects of story exposure were strongest for items that also
             had been tested in the first session. Subjects were aware of
             using story information, but interestingly, story exposure
             also increased belief that the facts had been known prior to
             the experiment, even for misinformation answers that were
             rarely produced without story reading. © 2003 Elsevier Inc.
             All rights reserved.},
   Doi = {10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00092-5},
   Key = {fds252793}
}

@article{fds252792,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Edelman, G and Bower, GH},
   Title = {Demonstrations of a generation effect in context
             memory.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {798-805},
   Year = {2001},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03196409},
   Abstract = {Generation often leads to increased memorability within a
             laboratory context (see, e.g., Slamecka & Graf, 1978). Of
             interest in the present study is whether the benefits of
             generation extend beyond item memory to context memory. To
             investigate this question, in three experiments, we asked
             subjects to remember in which of two contexts they had read
             or generated words. In Experiment 1, the contexts were two
             different rooms; in Experiment 2A, the contexts were two
             different computer screens; in Experiment 2B, the contexts
             were different perceptual characteristics of the
             to-be-remembered words. In all experiments, subjects were
             better at remembering the context of generated words than of
             read words.},
   Doi = {10.3758/bf03196409},
   Key = {fds252792}
}

@article{fds252791,
   Author = {Tversky, B and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Biased retellings of events yield biased
             memories.},
   Journal = {Cognitive psychology},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-38},
   Year = {2000},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0720},
   Abstract = {When people retell events, they take different perspectives
             for different audiences and purposes. In four experiments,
             we examined the effects of this postevent reorganization of
             events on memory for the original events. In each
             experiment, participants read a story, wrote a biased letter
             about one of the story characters, and later remembered the
             original story. Participants' letters contained more story
             details and more elaborations relevant to the purpose of
             their retellings. More importantly, the letter perspective
             affected the amount of information recalled (Experiments 1,
             3, and 4) and the direction of the errors in recall
             (Experiments 1 and 3) and recognition (Experiment 2).
             Selective rehearsal plays an important role in these bias
             effects: retelling involves selectively retrieving and using
             story information, with consequent differences in memory.
             However, biased memory occurred even when the biased letters
             contained little, if any, specific information (Experiment
             4) or contained the same amount and kinds of story
             information as a neutral control condition (Experiment 3).
             Biased memory is a consequence of the reorganizing schema
             guiding the retelling perspective, in addition to the
             effects of rehearsing specific information in
             retelling.},
   Doi = {10.1006/cogp.1999.0720},
   Key = {fds252791}
}

@article{fds252790,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Bower, GH},
   Title = {Applied Aspects of Source Monitoring},
   Journal = {Cognitive Technology},
   Volume = {4},
   Pages = {4-17},
   Year = {1999},
   Key = {fds252790}
}


%% Books   
@book{fds11113,
   Author = {Balota, D.A. and Marsh, E.J},
   Title = {Cognitive Psychology: Essential Readings},
   Booktitle = {Key Readings in Cognition},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press},
   Year = {2004},
   Abstract = {Cognitive psychology is an enormous field with a rich
             history. One problem confronting instructors in cognitive
             psychology courses is covering such diverse topics as
             pattern recognition, attention, memory, language,decision-making,
             and problem solving. It is virtually impossible to both
             cover these topics and also provide details regarding the
             beauty of the experimental studies that have tackled
             important topics in a single textbook or in a semester of
             lectures. The goal of this book is to help fill this void
             and provide students with the opportunity to learn about the
             details of the actual articles and chapters that have had
             major influences in the development of this
             discipline.},
   Key = {fds11113}
}

@book{fds21788,
   Author = {Roediger, H.L., III and McDermott, K.B. and Marsh,
             E.J},
   Title = {Human memory: Essential Readings},
   Series = {Key Readings in Cognition},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press},
   Year = {2004},
   Key = {fds21788}
}


%% Chapters in Books   
@misc{fds357994,
   Author = {Roediger, HL and Agarwal, PK and Kang, SHK and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Benefits of testing memory: Best practices and boundary
             conditions},
   Pages = {360-395},
   Booktitle = {Current Issues in Memory: Memory Research in the Public
             Interest},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780367618254},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003106715-22},
   Abstract = {The idea of a memory test or of a test of academic
             achievement is often circumscribed. Tests within the
             classroom are recognized as important for the assignment of
             grades, and tests given for academic assessment or
             achievement have increasingly come to determine the course
             of children’s lives: score well on such tests and people
             advance, are placed in more challenging classes, and attend
             better schools. Psychologists have studied the effects of
             testing on later memory, of and on, for 100 years. The power
             of testing to increase learning and retention has been
             demonstrated in numerous studies using a diverse range of
             materials; but both study and test materials come in a
             multitude of formats. A recent study delved more deeply into
             the issue of whether the kind of test influences the testing
             effect. The great bulk of the literature on testing effects
             shows the benefit of a single initial test relative to
             either no test or to reading control condition.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781003106715-22},
   Key = {fds357994}
}

@misc{fds356457,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Yang, BW},
   Title = {Broadening the autobiographical record to include memories
             of fiction},
   Pages = {32-46},
   Booktitle = {Memory Quirks: The Study of Odd Phenomena in
             Memory},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9780367209650},
   Key = {fds356457}
}

@misc{fds356458,
   Author = {Brown, AS and Fields, LM and Cadero, KC and Chmielewski, M and Denman,
             D and Marsh, EJ},
   Title = {Autobiographical editing: Revising our personal
             past},
   Pages = {3-19},
   Booktitle = {Memory Quirks: The Study of Odd Phenomena in
             Memory},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9780367209650},
   Key = {fds356458}
}

@misc{fds360003,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Stanley, ML},
   Title = {False beliefs: Byproducts of an adaptive knowledge
             base?},
   Pages = {131-146},
   Booktitle = {The Psychology of Fake News: Accepting, Sharing, and
             Correcting Misinformation},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781000179033},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780429295379-10},
   Abstract = {Pizzagate. The Bowling Green Massacre. Pope Francis
             endorsing Donald Trump for president. How should we combat
             such “fake news” stories, which often go viral? As
             cognitive scientists, we view this problem through the lens
             of what we know about the construction, representation, and
             updating of belief and knowledge more generally. That is, we
             argue that the same efficient processes that support the
             learning of veridical information also support the
             processing of fake news and sometimes produce false beliefs.
             Anchoring on the larger psychological literature on belief
             and knowledge highlights possible solutions while casting
             doubt on other possible interventions. For example, we are
             not optimistic about interventions that focus learners on
             assessing the credibility of information sources, given that
             such information is likely forgotten over time. Overall, we
             argue that the basic science of belief and knowledge
             provides a useful foundation for thinking about reactions to
             and consequences of fake news.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780429295379-10},
   Key = {fds360003}
}

@misc{fds354277,
   Author = {Marsh, E and Drew, E},
   Title = {Correcting Student Errors and Misconceptions},
   Pages = {437-459},
   Booktitle = {The Cambridge Handbook of Cognition and Education},
   Publisher = {Cambridge University Press},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {February},
   ISBN = {9781108245104},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/9781108235631.018},
   Abstract = {This Handbook reviews a wealth of research in cognitive and
             educational psychology that investigates how to enhance
             learning and instruction to aid students struggling to learn
             and to advise teachers on how best to support student
             learning.},
   Doi = {10.1017/9781108235631.018},
   Key = {fds354277}
}

@misc{fds333503,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Arnold, KM},
   Title = {Retelling experiences and writing essays how: Storytelling
             reflects and changes memory},
   Pages = {137-155},
   Booktitle = {Representations in Mind and World: Essays Inspired by
             Barbara Tversky},
   Publisher = {Routledge},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {July},
   ISBN = {9781138829695},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315169781},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315169781},
   Key = {fds333503}
}

@misc{fds328071,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Mullet, HG},
   Title = {Stories and movies can mislead},
   Pages = {87-101},
   Booktitle = {False and Distorted Memories},
   Publisher = {PSYCHOLOGY PRESS},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {October},
   ISBN = {9781138832015},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315736242},
   Doi = {10.4324/9781315736242},
   Key = {fds328071}
}

@misc{fds221848,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J and . and Umanath, S},
   Title = {Learning from fictional sources: An instance of knowledge
             neglect},
   Booktitle = {Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied
             Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational
             Sciences},
   Publisher = {MIT Press},
   Editor = {D. N. Rapp and J. Braasch},
   Year = {2013},
   Key = {fds221848}
}

@misc{fds221851,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Cantor, A. D},
   Title = {Learning from multiple-choice tests},
   Booktitle = {Proceedings from the Center for Integrative Research on
             Cognition, Learning, and Education STEM conference},
   Editor = {M. McDaniel and G. Frey},
   Year = {2013},
   Key = {fds221851}
}

@misc{fds200891,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Roediger, H. L., III},
   Title = {Episodic and autobiographical memory},
   Pages = {472-494},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of Psychology: Volume 4, 2nd edition},
   Publisher = {John Wiley & Sons},
   Editor = {A. F. Healy and R. W. Proctor.},
   Year = {2013},
   Key = {fds200891}
}

@misc{fds185585,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Butler, A. C},
   Title = {Memory in educational settings},
   Pages = {299-317},
   Booktitle = {Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
   Editor = {D. Resiberg},
   Year = {2013},
   Key = {fds185585}
}

@misc{fds252730,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Fazio, LK},
   Title = {Learning from fictional sources},
   Pages = {395-412},
   Booktitle = {The Foundations of Remembering: Essays in Honor Of Henry L.
             Roediger, III},
   Publisher = {PSYCHOLOGY PRESS},
   Year = {2011},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780203837672},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203837672},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203837672},
   Key = {fds252730}
}

@misc{fds252731,
   Author = {Roediger, HL and Agarwal, PK and Kang, SHK and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Benefits of testing memory: Best practices and boundary
             conditions},
   Pages = {13-49},
   Booktitle = {Current Issues in Applied Memory Research},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press},
   Editor = {G. M. Davies and D.B. Wright},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {November},
   ISBN = {9780203869611},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203869611},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203869611},
   Key = {fds252731}
}

@misc{fds361308,
   Author = {Roediger, HL and Agarwal, PK and Kang, SHK and Marsh,
             EJ},
   Title = {Benefits of testing memory: Best practices and boundary
             conditions},
   Pages = {13-49},
   Booktitle = {Current Issues in Applied Memory Research},
   Year = {2009},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9781841697277},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203869611-10},
   Abstract = {The idea of a memory test or of a test of academic
             achievement is often circumscribed. Tests within the
             classroom are recognized as important for the assignment of
             grades, and tests given for academic assessment or
             achievement have increasingly come to determine the course
             of children’s lives: score well on such tests and you
             advance, are placed in more challenging classes, and attend
             better schools. Against this widely acknowledged backdrop of
             the importance of testing in educational life (not just in
             the US, but all over the world), it would be di?cult to
             justify the claim that testing is not used enough in
             educational practice. In fact, such a claim may seem to be
             ludicrous on the face of it. However, this is just the claim
             we will make in this chapter: Education in schools would
             greatly bene?t from additional testing, and the need for
             increased testing probably increases with advancement in the
             educational system. In addition, students should use
             self-testing as a study strategy in preparing for their
             classes.},
   Doi = {10.4324/9780203869611-10},
   Key = {fds361308}
}

@misc{fds366405,
   Author = {Marsh, EJ and Eslick, AN and Fazio, LK},
   Title = {False memories},
   Volume = {2},
   Series = {Volume 2 of Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive
             Reference.},
   Pages = {221-238},
   Booktitle = {Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference},
   Publisher = {Elsevier},
   Editor = {H. L. Roediger, III},
   Year = {2007},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780123705099},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012370509-9.00144-3},
   Abstract = {A complete understanding of human memory requires us to
             comprehend memory’s failures as well as its successes. In
             addition to being inherently interesting, memory errors
             provide insight into how memory functions for successful,
             accurate retrieval. This chapter outlines several areas in
             which human memory is less than perfect, including false
             memory for words, eyewitness suggestibility, verbal
             overshadowing, false fame, imagination inflation, and
             implanted memories for entire events. Not surprisingly,
             given the complexity of memory, there are many different
             ways that error can enter the system, from encoding to
             retrieval. Together, the data highlight the constructive
             nature of memory, as proposed by Bartlett
             (1932).},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-012370509-9.00144-3},
   Key = {fds366405}
}

@misc{fds52416,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Fazio, L. K},
   Title = {Learning facts from fiction},
   Pages = {395-411},
   Booktitle = {The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L.
             Roediger},
   Publisher = {Psychology Press},
   Editor = {James Nairne},
   Year = {2007},
   Key = {fds52416}
}

@misc{fds21783,
   Author = {Luminet, O. and Curci, A. and Marsh, E. J. and Wessel, I. and Constantin, T. and Gencoz, F. and Yogo, M},
   Title = {The cognitive, emotional, and social impact of the September
             11th Attacks: Group differences in memory for the reception
             context and its determinants},
   Series = {pp. 210-223},
   Booktitle = {Constructive Memory},
   Publisher = {Sofia, Bulgaria: New Bulgarian University},
   Editor = {B. Kokinov and W. Hirst},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds21783}
}

@misc{fds21784,
   Author = {Roediger, H.L., III and Marsh, E.J},
   Title = {Episodic and Autobiographical Memory},
   Pages = {475-497},
   Booktitle = {Handbook of Psychology: volume 4, Experimental
             Psychology},
   Publisher = {NY: John Wiley & Sons},
   Editor = {A.F. Healy and R.W. Proctor},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds21784}
}

@misc{fds21785,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J},
   Title = {Memory: Myths, Mysteries, and Realities},
   Series = {3e},
   Pages = {1605-1609},
   Booktitle = {Encyclopedia of Education},
   Publisher = {New York: Macmillan},
   Editor = {J. Guthrie},
   Year = {2003},
   Key = {fds21785}
}

@misc{fds21786,
   Author = {Jacoby, L.L. and Marsh, E.J. and & Dolan, P.O},
   Title = {Forms of Bias: Age-Related Differences in
             Memory},
   Pages = {240-252},
   Booktitle = {Perspectives on Human Memory and Cognitive Aging: Essays in
             Honoiur of Fergus Craik},
   Publisher = {Philadelphia: Psychology Press},
   Editor = {M. Naveh-Benjamin and M.Moscovitch and H.L. Roediger,
             III},
   Year = {2002},
   Key = {fds21786}
}

@misc{fds21787,
   Author = {Roediger, H.L., III and Marsh, E.J and Lee, S.C},
   Title = {Varieties of Memory},
   Series = {3e},
   Booktitle = {Memory and Cognitive Processes, volume 2 of H. Pashler(ed)
             Steven's Handbook, of Experimental Psychology},
   Publisher = {New York: John Wiley & Sons},
   Editor = {D. Medin},
   Year = {2002},
   Key = {fds21787}
}


%% Commentaries/Book Reviews   
@article{fds70556,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Multhaup, K.},
   Title = {Dual coding theory: It's not just for cognitive
             psychologists anymore. A review of Mind and Its
             Evolution},
   Journal = {PsycCritiques},
   Year = {2007},
   Key = {fds70556}
}

@article{fds52415,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Fazio, L. K},
   Title = {Finding memory in hard-to-reach places. A Review of Why life
             speeds up as you get older: How memory shapes our past (ISBN
             0-532-83424-4) by Douwe Draaisma},
   Journal = {PsyCRITIQUES},
   Year = {2006},
   Key = {fds52415}
}


%% Book Reviews   
@article{fds169205,
   Author = {Marsh, E. J. and Eslick, A. N},
   Title = {Review of Why students don’t like school by D.
             Willingham},
   Journal = {Educational Horizons},
   Volume = {87},
   Pages = {206-210},
   Year = {2009},
   Key = {fds169205}
}


%% Other   
@misc{fds169197,
   Author = {Roediger, H. L., III and Marsh, E. J},
   Title = {False memories},
   Journal = {Scholarpedia},
   Volume = {4},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {3858},
   Year = {2009},
   Key = {fds169197}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Postdocs * Reload * Login