Psychology and Neuroscience Faculty Database
Psychology and Neuroscience
Arts & Sciences
Duke University

 HOME > Arts & Sciences > pn > Faculty    Search Help Login pdf version printable version 

Publications of Paul Seli    :recent first  alphabetical  combined listing:

%% Journal Articles   
@article{fds335751,
   Author = {Seli, P and Cheyne, JA and Barton, KR and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Consistency of sustained attention across modalities:
             comparing visual and auditory versions of the
             SART.},
   Journal = {Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue
             canadienne de psychologie experimentale},
   Volume = {66},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {44-50},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025111},
   Abstract = {We develop and assess an auditory version of an increasingly
             widely used measure of sustained attention, the Sustained
             Attention to Response Task (SART). In two separate studies,
             the auditory SART generated slower response times and fewer
             errors than the visual SART. Proportion of errors, response
             times, and response time variability were, however,
             significantly and strongly correlated across the two
             modalities. The cross-modality correlations were generally
             equivalent to split-half correlations within modalities,
             indicating a strong agreement of the assessment of
             individual differences in sustained attention in the visual
             and auditory modalities. The foregoing results plus the
             finding that errors on the auditory SART were reduced
             suggests that the auditory SART may be a preferred
             alternative for use with populations with deficits in
             sustained attention.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0025111},
   Key = {fds335751}
}

@article{fds335752,
   Author = {Seli, P and Cheyne, JA and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Attention failures versus misplaced diligence: separating
             attention lapses from speed-accuracy trade-offs.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {21},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {277-291},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.017},
   Abstract = {In two studies of a GO-NOGO task assessing sustained
             attention, we examined the effects of (1) altering
             speed-accuracy trade-offs through instructions (emphasizing
             both speed and accuracy or accuracy only) and (2) auditory
             alerts distributed throughout the task. Instructions
             emphasizing accuracy reduced errors and changed the
             distribution of GO trial RTs. Additionally, correlations
             between errors and increasing RTs produced a U-function;
             excessively fast and slow RTs accounted for much of the
             variance of errors. Contrary to previous reports, alerts
             increased errors and RT variability. The results suggest
             that (1) standard instructions for sustained attention
             tasks, emphasizing speed and accuracy equally, produce
             errors arising from attempts to conform to the misleading
             requirement for speed, which become conflated with
             attention-lapse produced errors and (2) auditory alerts have
             complex, and sometimes deleterious, effects on attention. We
             argue that instructions emphasizing accuracy provide a more
             precise assessment of attention lapses in sustained
             attention tasks.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.017},
   Key = {fds335752}
}

@article{fds335750,
   Author = {Pennycook, G and Cheyne, JA and Seli, P and Koehler, DJ and Fugelsang,
             JA},
   Title = {Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal
             belief.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {123},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {335-346},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003},
   Abstract = {An analytic cognitive style denotes a propensity to set
             aside highly salient intuitions when engaging in problem
             solving. We assess the hypothesis that an analytic cognitive
             style is associated with a history of questioning, altering,
             and rejecting (i.e., unbelieving) supernatural claims, both
             religious and paranormal. In two studies, we examined
             associations of God beliefs, religious engagement
             (attendance at religious services, praying, etc.),
             conventional religious beliefs (heaven, miracles, etc.) and
             paranormal beliefs (extrasensory perception, levitation,
             etc.) with performance measures of cognitive ability and
             analytic cognitive style. An analytic cognitive style
             negatively predicted both religious and paranormal beliefs
             when controlling for cognitive ability as well as religious
             engagement, sex, age, political ideology, and education.
             Participants more willing to engage in analytic reasoning
             were less likely to endorse supernatural beliefs. Further,
             an association between analytic cognitive style and
             religious engagement was mediated by religious beliefs,
             suggesting that an analytic cognitive style negatively
             affects religious engagement via lower acceptance of
             conventional religious beliefs. Results for types of God
             belief indicate that the association between an analytic
             cognitive style and God beliefs is more nuanced than mere
             acceptance and rejection, but also includes adopting less
             conventional God beliefs, such as Pantheism or Deism. Our
             data are consistent with the idea that two people who share
             the same cognitive ability, education, political ideology,
             sex, age and level of religious engagement can acquire very
             different sets of beliefs about the world if they differ in
             their propensity to think analytically.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003},
   Key = {fds335750}
}

@article{fds335749,
   Author = {Jonker, TR and Seli, P and Macleod, CM},
   Title = {Less we forget: retrieval cues and release from
             retrieval-induced forgetting.},
   Journal = {Memory & cognition},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1236-1245},
   Year = {2012},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0224-2},
   Abstract = {Retrieving some items from memory can impair the subsequent
             recall of other related but not retrieved items, a
             phenomenon called retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF). The
             dominant explanation of RIF-the inhibition account-asserts
             that forgetting occurs because related items are suppressed
             during retrieval practice to reduce retrieval competition.
             This item inhibition persists, making it more difficult to
             recall the related items on a later test. In our set of
             experiments, each category was designed such that each
             exemplar belonged to one of two subcategories (e.g., each
             BIRD exemplar was either a bird of prey or a pet bird), but
             this subcategory information was not made explicit during
             study or retrieval practice. Practicing retrieval of items
             from only one subcategory led to RIF for items from the
             other subcategory when cued only with the overall category
             label (BIRD) at test. However, adapting the technique of
             Gardiner, Craik, and Birtwistle (Journal of Learning and
             Verbal Behavior 11:778-783, 1972), providing subcategory
             cues during the final test eliminated RIF. The results
             challenge the inhibition account's fundamental assumption of
             cue independence but are consistent with a cue-based
             interference account.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13421-012-0224-2},
   Key = {fds335749}
}

@article{fds335747,
   Author = {Seli, P and Carriere, JSA and Levene, M and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {How few and far between? Examining the effects of probe rate
             on self-reported mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychology},
   Volume = {4},
   Pages = {430},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00430},
   Abstract = {We examined whether the temporal rate at which thought
             probes are presented affects the likelihood that people will
             report periods of mind wandering. To evaluate this
             possibility, we had participants complete a
             sustained-attention task (the Metronome Response Task; MRT)
             during which we intermittently presented thought probes.
             Critically, we varied the average time between probes (i.e.,
             probe rate) across participants, allowing us to examine the
             relation between probe rate and mind-wandering rate. We
             observed a positive relation between these variables,
             indicating that people are more likely to report mind
             wandering as the time between probes increases. We discuss
             the methodological implications of this finding in the
             context of the mind-wandering literature, and suggest that
             researchers include a range of probe rates in future work to
             provide more insight into this methodological
             issue.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00430},
   Key = {fds335747}
}

@article{fds335748,
   Author = {Seli, P and Jonker, TR and Cheyne, JA and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Enhancing SART Validity by Statistically Controlling
             Speed-Accuracy Trade-Offs.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychology},
   Volume = {4},
   Pages = {265},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00265},
   Abstract = {Numerous studies focused on elucidating the correlates,
             causes, and consequences of inattention/attention-lapses
             employ the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART), a
             GO-NOGO task with infrequent withholds. Although the SART
             has become popular among inattention researchers, recent
             work has demonstrated its susceptibility to speed-accuracy
             trade-offs (SATOs), rendering its assessment of inattention
             problematic. Here, we propose and illustrate methods to
             statistically control for the occurrence of SATOs during
             SART performance. The statistical solutions presented here
             can be used to correct standard SART-error scores, including
             those of already-published data, thereby allowing
             researchers to re-examine existing data, and to more
             sensitively evaluate the validity of earlier
             conclusions.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00265},
   Key = {fds335748}
}

@article{fds335746,
   Author = {Seli, P and Cheyne, JA and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Wandering minds and wavering rhythms: linking mind wandering
             and behavioral variability.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and
             performance},
   Volume = {39},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-5},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030954},
   Abstract = {Mind wandering is a pervasive feature of human cognition
             often associated with the withdrawal of task-related
             executive control processes. Here, we explore the
             possibility that, in tasks requiring executive control to
             sustain consistent responding, moments of mind wandering
             could be associated with moments of increased behavioral
             variability. To test this possibility, we developed and
             administered a novel task (the metronome response task) in
             which participants were instructed to respond synchronously
             (via button presses) with the continuous rhythmic
             presentation of tones. We provide evidence (replicated
             across 2 independent samples) that response variability
             during the 5 trials preceding probe-caught reports of mind
             wandering (tuned-out and zoned-out mind wandering) is
             significantly greater than during the 5 trials preceding
             reports of on-task performance. These results suggest that,
             at least in some tasks, behavioral variability is an online
             marker of mind wandering.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0030954},
   Key = {fds335746}
}

@article{fds335745,
   Author = {Carriere, JSA and Seli, P and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in
             mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting.},
   Journal = {Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue
             canadienne de psychologie experimentale},
   Volume = {67},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {19-31},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031438},
   Abstract = {Anecdotal reports suggest that during periods of inattention
             or mind wandering, people tend to experience increased
             fidgeting. In four studies, we examined whether individual
             differences in the tendency to be inattentive and to mind
             wander in everyday life are related to the tendency to make
             spontaneous and involuntary movements (i.e., to fidget). To
             do so, we developed self-report measures of spontaneous and
             deliberate mind wandering, as well as a self-report scale to
             index fidgeting. In addition, we used several existing
             self-report measures of inattentiveness, attentional
             control, and memory failures. Across our studies, a series
             of multiple regression analyses indicated that fidgeting was
             uniquely predicted by inattentiveness and spontaneous mind
             wandering but not by other related factors, including
             deliberate mind wandering, attentional control, and memory
             failures. As a result, we suggest that only spontaneously
             wandering thoughts are related to a wandering
             body.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0031438},
   Key = {fds335745}
}

@article{fds335744,
   Author = {Seli, P and Jonker, TR and Solman, GJF and Cheyne, JA and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {A methodological note on evaluating performance in a
             sustained-attention-to-response task.},
   Journal = {Behavior research methods},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {355-363},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0266-1},
   Abstract = {We evaluated the influence of speed-accuracy trade-offs on
             performance in the sustained attention to response task
             (SART), a task often used to evaluate the effectiveness of
             techniques designed to improve sustained attention. In the
             present study, we experimentally manipulated response delay
             in a variation of the SART and found that commission errors,
             which are commonly used as an index of lapses in sustained
             attention, were a systematic function of manipulated
             differences in response delay. Delaying responses to roughly
             800 ms after stimulus onset reduced commission errors
             substantially. We suggest the possibility that any technique
             that affects response speed will indirectly alter error
             rates independently of improvements in sustained attention.
             Investigators therefore need to carefully explore, report,
             and correct for changes in response speed that accompany
             improvements in performance or, alternatively, to employ
             tasks that control for response speed.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13428-012-0266-1},
   Key = {fds335744}
}

@article{fds335743,
   Author = {Jonker, TR and Seli, P and MacLeod, CM},
   Title = {Putting retrieval-induced forgetting in context: an
             inhibition-free, context-based account.},
   Journal = {Psychological review},
   Volume = {120},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {852-872},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034246},
   Abstract = {We present a new theoretical account of retrieval-induced
             forgetting (RIF) together with new experimental evidence
             that fits this account and challenges the dominant
             inhibition account. RIF occurs when the retrieval of some
             material from memory produces later forgetting of related
             material. The inhibition account asserts that RIF is the
             result of an inhibition mechanism that acts during retrieval
             to suppress the representations of interfering competitors.
             This inhibition is enduring, such that the suppressed
             material is difficult to access on a later test and is,
             therefore, recalled more poorly than baseline material.
             Although the inhibition account is widely accepted, a
             growing body of research challenges its fundamental
             assumptions. Our alternative account of RIF instead
             emphasizes the role of context in remembering. According to
             this context account, both of 2 tenets must be met for RIF
             to occur: (a) A context change must occur between study and
             subsequent retrieval practice, and (b) the retrieval
             practice context must be the active context during the final
             test when testing practiced categories. The results of 3
             experiments, which directly test the divergent predictions
             of the 2 accounts, support the context account but cannot be
             explained by the inhibition account. In an extensive
             discussion, we survey the literature on RIF and apply our
             context account to the key findings, demonstrating the
             explanatory power of context.},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0034246},
   Key = {fds335743}
}

@article{fds335742,
   Author = {Jonker, TR and Seli, P and Cheyne, JA and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Performance reactivity in a continuous-performance task:
             implications for understanding post-error
             behavior.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1468-1476},
   Year = {2013},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.10.005},
   Abstract = {Although there has been considerable interest in the effects
             of errors on subsequent performance, relatively few studies
             have considered the effects of non-error events that contain
             some performance-relevant information, such as correct
             performance on critical trials. In the present article, we
             propose and assess a hypothesis of performance reactivity.
             In support of this hypothesis, we provide evidence of
             performance decrements following both incorrect and correct
             responses but not following performance-irrelevant events.
             More specifically, in a continuous response task (Sustained
             Attention to Response Task), we (1) replicate previous
             findings that errors of commission on rare NOGO trials
             produce decrements in subsequent performance, and (2)
             observe that correct withholds to NOGO trials produce
             decrements in subsequent accuracy relative to
             task-irrelevant tones. These results corroborate a
             hypothesis that some error-related effects on subsequent
             performance are not unique, but are instead a particularly
             salient version of a more general performance-reactivity
             effect.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2013.10.005},
   Key = {fds335742}
}

@article{fds335741,
   Author = {Ralph, BCW and Seli, P and Cheng, VOY and Solman, GJF and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Running the figure to the ground: figure-ground segmentation
             during visual search.},
   Journal = {Vision research},
   Volume = {97},
   Pages = {65-73},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2014.02.005},
   Abstract = {We examined how figure-ground segmentation occurs across
             multiple regions of a visual array during a visual search
             task. Stimuli consisted of arrays of black-and-white
             figure-ground images in which roughly half of each image
             depicted a meaningful object, whereas the other half
             constituted a less meaningful shape. The colours of the
             meaningful regions of the targets and distractors were
             either the same (congruent) or different (incongruent). We
             found that incongruent targets took longer to locate than
             congruent targets (Experiments 1, 2, and 3) and that this
             segmentation-congruency effect decreased when the number of
             search items was reduced (Experiment 2). Furthermore, an
             analysis of eye movements revealed that participants spent
             more time scrutinising the target before confirming its
             identity on incongruent trials than on congruent trials
             (Experiment 3). These findings suggest that the distractor
             context influences target segmentation and detection during
             visual search.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.visres.2014.02.005},
   Key = {fds335741}
}

@article{fds335740,
   Author = {Seli, P and Carriere, JSA and Thomson, DR and Cheyne, JA and Martens,
             KAE and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Restless mind, restless body.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and
             cognition},
   Volume = {40},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {660-668},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035260},
   Abstract = {In the present work, we investigate the hypothesis that
             failures of task-related executive control that occur during
             episodes of mind wandering are associated with an increase
             in extraneous movements (fidgeting). In 2 studies, we
             assessed mind wandering using thought probes while
             participants performed the metronome response task (MRT),
             which required them to synchronize button presses with
             tones. Participants performed this task while sitting on a
             Wii Balance Board providing us with an index of fidgeting.
             Results of Study 1 demonstrate that relative to on-task
             periods, mind wandering is indeed accompanied by increases
             in fidgeting, as well as increased response variability in
             the MRT. In Study 2, we observed that only deep mind
             wandering was associated with increases in fidgeting,
             whereas task-related response variability increased even
             during mild mind wandering. We interpret these findings in
             the context of current theories of mind wandering and
             suggest that (a) mind wandering is associated with costs not
             only to primary-task performance but also to secondary-task
             goals (e.g., controlling extraneous movements) and (b) these
             costs may depend on the degree to which task-related
             executive control processes are disengaged during mind
             wandering (i.e., depth of mind wandering).},
   Doi = {10.1037/a0035260},
   Key = {fds335740}
}

@article{fds335739,
   Author = {Thomson, DR and Seli, P and Besner, D and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {On the link between mind wandering and task performance over
             time.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {27},
   Pages = {14-26},
   Year = {2014},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.001},
   Abstract = {Here we test the hypothesis that fluctuations in subjective
             reports of mind wandering over time-on-task are associated
             with fluctuations in performance over time-on-task. In Study
             1, we employed a singleton search task and found that
             performance did not differ prior to on- and off-task
             reports, nor did individual differences in mind wandering
             predict differences in performance (so-called standard
             analytic methods). Importantly however, we find that
             fluctuations in mind wandering over time are strongly
             associated with fluctuations in behavior. In Study 2, we
             provide a replication of the relation between mind wandering
             and performance over time found in Study 1, using a Flanker
             interference task. These data indicate (1) a tight coupling
             between mind wandering and performance over time and (2)
             that a temporal-analytic approach can reveal effects of mind
             wandering on performance in tasks where standard analyses
             fail to do so. The theoretical and methodological
             implications of these findings are discussed.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.001},
   Key = {fds335739}
}

@article{fds335738,
   Author = {Ralph, BCW and Thomson, DR and Seli, P and Carriere, JSA and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Media multitasking and behavioral measures of sustained
             attention.},
   Journal = {Attention, perception & psychophysics},
   Volume = {77},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {390-401},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0771-7},
   Abstract = {In a series of four studies, self-reported media
             multitasking (using the media multitasking index; MMI) and
             general sustained-attention ability, through performance on
             three sustained-attention tasks: the metronome response task
             (MRT), the sustained-attention-to-response task (SART), and
             a vigilance task (here, a modified version of the SART). In
             Study 1, we found that higher reports of media multitasking
             were associated with increased response variability (i.e.,
             poor performance) on the MRT. However, in Study 2, no
             association between reported media multitasking and
             performance on the SART was observed. These findings were
             replicated in Studies 3a and 3b, in which we again assessed
             the relation between media multitasking and performance on
             both the MRT and SART in two large online samples. Finally,
             in Study 4, using a large online sample, we tested whether
             media multitasking was associated with performance on a
             vigilance task. Although standard vigilance decrements were
             observed in both sensitivity (A') and response times, media
             multitasking was not associated with the size of these
             decrements, nor was media multitasking associated with
             overall performance, in terms of either sensitivity or
             response times. Taken together, the results of the studies
             reported here failed to demonstrate a relation between
             habitual engagement in media multitasking in everyday life
             and a general deficit in sustained-attention
             processes.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13414-014-0771-7},
   Key = {fds335738}
}

@article{fds335736,
   Author = {Seli, P and Jonker, TR and Cheyne, JA and Cortes, K and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Can research participants comment authoritatively on the
             validity of their self-reports of mind wandering and task
             engagement?},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and
             performance},
   Volume = {41},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {703-709},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000029},
   Abstract = {The study of mind wandering rests upon the assumption that
             people are able to consistently and accurately introspect
             and report on these sorts of mental experiences. Although
             there is some initial evidence that people can indeed
             accurately report on the subjective experience of mind
             wandering, to date, no work has directly examined people's
             degree of confidence in their self-reports of mind wandering
             and the effects that confidence has on the accuracy of such
             reports. In the present study, participants completed a
             sustained-attention task during which they intermittently
             provided assessments of task engagement (i.e., whether they
             were focused on the task or mind wandering), as well as
             reports of confidence in the accuracy of their assessments.
             This study yielded 3 key findings: We found substantial
             between- and within-subject variability in both (a) reported
             mind wandering and (b) confidence in mind-wandering reports,
             and, most critically, (c) we found that the relation of
             reported mind wandering and task performance varied as a
             function of confidence. We discuss the implications of these
             findings in the context of the literature on mind
             wandering.},
   Doi = {10.1037/xhp0000029},
   Key = {fds335736}
}

@article{fds335737,
   Author = {Seli, P and Smallwood, J and Cheyne, JA and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {On the relation of mind wandering and ADHD
             symptomatology.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {629-636},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0793-0},
   Abstract = {Mind wandering seems to be a prototypical feature of
             attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, an
             important emerging distinction of mind-wandering types
             hinges on whether a given episode of mind wandering reflects
             a failure of executive control (spontaneous mind wandering)
             or the engagement of controlled processes for internal
             processing (deliberate mind wandering). Here we distinguish
             between spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering and test
             the hypothesis that symptoms of ADHD are associated with the
             former but not the latter. We assessed ADHD symptomatology
             and everyday levels of deliberate and spontaneous mind
             wandering in two large non-clinical samples (Ns = 1,354). In
             addition, to provide converging evidence, we examined rates
             of deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering in a clinically
             diagnosed ADHD sample. Results provide clear evidence that
             spontaneous, but not deliberate, mind wandering is a central
             feature of ADHD symptomatology at both the clinical and
             non-clinical level. We discuss the implications of these
             results for understanding both ADHD and mind
             wandering.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-014-0793-0},
   Key = {fds335737}
}

@article{fds335735,
   Author = {Jonker, TR and Seli, P and MacLeod, CM},
   Title = {Retrieval-Induced Forgetting and Context},
   Journal = {Current Directions in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {273-278},
   Publisher = {SAGE Publications},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721415573203},
   Abstract = {Retrieving information can result in the forgetting of
             related information, a phenomenon referred to as
             retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF). To date, the dominant
             explanation of RIF has been an inhibition account, which
             emphasizes long-term suppression of interfering memories. As
             one alternative, some have advocated for a strength-based
             interference account, which emphasizes the role of
             strengthening associations. More recently, we have proposed
             a context account, which emphasizes the role of context
             change and context reinstatement. In this article, we
             outline these three accounts of RIF and demonstrate that
             there is substantial evidence that uniquely supports our
             context account.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0963721415573203},
   Key = {fds335735}
}

@article{fds335733,
   Author = {Seli, P and Cheyne, JA and Xu, M and Purdon, C and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications
             for assessments of task-unrelated thought.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and
             cognition},
   Volume = {41},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1417-1425},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000116},
   Abstract = {Researchers of mind wandering frequently assume that (a)
             participants are motivated to do well on the tasks they are
             given, and (b) task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs) that occur
             during task performance reflect unintentional, unwanted
             thoughts that occur despite participants' best intentions to
             maintain task-focus. Given the relatively boring and tedious
             nature of most mind-wandering tasks, however, there is the
             possibility that some participants have little motivation to
             do well on such tasks, and that this lack of motivation
             might in turn result in increases specifically in
             intentional TUTs. In the present study, we explored these
             possibilities, finding that individuals reporting lower
             motivation to perform well on a sustained-attention task
             reported more intentional relative to unintentional TUTs
             compared with individuals reporting higher motivation.
             Interestingly, our results indicate that the extent to which
             participants engage in intentional versus unintentional TUTs
             does not differentially relate to performance: both types of
             off-task thought were found to be equally associated with
             performance decrements. Participants with low levels of
             task-motivation also engaged in more overall TUTs, however,
             and this increase in TUTs was associated with greater
             performance decrements. We discuss these findings in the
             context of the literature on mind wandering, highlighting
             the importance of assessing the intentionality of TUTs and
             motivation to perform well on tasks assessing mind
             wandering.},
   Doi = {10.1037/xlm0000116},
   Key = {fds335733}
}

@article{fds335734,
   Author = {Seli, P and Carriere, JSA and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Not all mind wandering is created equal: dissociating
             deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {79},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {750-758},
   Year = {2015},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0617-x},
   Abstract = {In two large samples we show a dissociation between
             trait-level tendencies to mind-wander spontaneously
             (unintentionally) and deliberately (intentionally).
             Participants completed online versions of the Mind Wandering
             Spontaneous (MW-S) and the Mind Wandering Deliberate (MW-D)
             self-report scales and the Five Facet Mindfulness
             Questionnaire (FFMQ). The results revealed that deliberate
             and spontaneous mind wandering were uniquely associated with
             some factors of the FFMQ. Notably, while the MW-D and the
             MW-S were positively associated with each other, the MW-D
             was uniquely positively associated with the 'Non-Reactivity
             to Inner Experience' factor of the FFMQ, whereas the MW-S
             was uniquely negatively associated with this factor. We also
             showed that conflating deliberate and spontaneous mind
             wandering can result in a misunderstanding of how mind
             wandering is related to other traits. We recommend that
             studies assessing individual differences in mind wandering
             should distinguish between deliberate and spontaneous
             subtypes of mind wandering to avoid possibly erroneous
             conclusions.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-014-0617-x},
   Key = {fds335734}
}

@article{fds335731,
   Author = {Seli, P},
   Title = {The Attention-Lapse and Motor Decoupling accounts of SART
             performance are not mutually exclusive.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {41},
   Pages = {189-198},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.017},
   Abstract = {There is an ongoing debate about the mechanisms purported to
             underlie performance in the Sustained-Attention-to-Response
             Task (SART). Whereas the Attention-Lapse account posits that
             SART errors result from attentional disengagement, the Motor
             Decoupling account proposes that SART errors result from
             failures to inhibit a fast, prepotent motor response,
             despite adequate attention to the task. That SART
             performance might be fully accounted for by motor decoupling
             is problematic for a Attention-Lapse account, and for the
             use of the SART as an index of attention lapses. To test
             whether SART performance is in fact fully accounted for by
             motor decoupling, I examined the relation between SART
             performance and attention lapses while controlling for motor
             decoupling. The results were clear: The SART was associated
             with attention lapses independently of motor decoupling.
             Thus, the present study suggests that both accounts are
             correct and that the SART is a valid measure of attention
             lapses.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.017},
   Key = {fds335731}
}

@article{fds335732,
   Author = {Seli, P and Risko, EF and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Assessing the associations among trait and state levels of
             deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {41},
   Pages = {50-56},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.002},
   Abstract = {Recent research has demonstrated that mind wandering can be
             subdivided into spontaneous and deliberate types, and this
             distinction has been found to hold at both the trait and
             state levels. However, to date, no attempts have been made
             to link trait-level spontaneous and deliberate mind
             wandering with state-level assessments of these two subtypes
             of mind wandering. Here we evaluated whether trait-level
             deliberate and spontaneous mind wandering map onto state
             levels of these subtypes of mind wandering. Results showed
             correspondence between trait-level reports of spontaneous
             and deliberate mind wandering and their state-level
             counterparts, indicating that people's reports on the
             intentionality of their mind wandering in the laboratory
             correspond to their reports of the intentionality of mind
             wandering in everyday life. Thus, the trait- and state-level
             scales of mind wandering were found to validate each other:
             Whereas the state-level measures provided some construct
             validity for the trait-level measures, the trait-level
             measures indicated that the state-level measures may be
             generalizable to everyday situations.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.002},
   Key = {fds335732}
}

@article{fds335730,
   Author = {Seli, P and Risko, EF and Smilek, D},
   Title = {On the Necessity of Distinguishing Between Unintentional and
             Intentional Mind Wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {685-691},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616634068},
   Abstract = {In recent years, there has been an enormous increase in the
             number of studies examining mind wandering. Although
             participants' reports of mind wandering are often assumed to
             largely reflect spontaneous, unintentional thoughts, many
             researchers' conceptualizations of mind wandering have left
             open the possibility that at least some of these reports
             reflect deliberate, intentional thought. Critically,
             however, in most investigations on the topic, researchers
             have not separately assessed each type of mind wandering;
             instead, they have measured mind wandering as a unitary
             construct, thereby conflating intentional and unintentional
             types. We report the first compelling evidence that an
             experimental manipulation can have qualitatively different
             effects on intentional and unintentional types of mind
             wandering. This result provides clear evidence that
             researchers interested in understanding mind wandering need
             to consider the distinction between unintentional and
             intentional occurrences of this phenomenon.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797616634068},
   Key = {fds335730}
}

@article{fds335728,
   Author = {Seli, P and Risko, EF and Smilek, D and Schacter,
             DL},
   Title = {Mind-Wandering With and Without Intention.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {20},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {605-617},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010},
   Abstract = {The past decade has seen a surge of research examining
             mind-wandering, but most of this research has not considered
             the potential importance of distinguishing between
             intentional and unintentional mind-wandering. However, a
             recent series of papers have demonstrated that
             mind-wandering reported in empirical investigations
             frequently occurs with and without intention, and, more
             crucially, that intentional and unintentional mind-wandering
             are dissociable. This emerging literature suggests that, to
             increase clarity in the literature, there is a need to
             reconsider the bulk of the mind-wandering literature with an
             eye toward deconvolving these two different cognitive
             experiences. In this review we highlight recent trends in
             investigations of the intentionality of mind-wandering, and
             we outline a novel theoretical framework regarding the
             mechanisms underlying intentional and unintentional
             mind-wandering.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010},
   Key = {fds335728}
}

@article{fds335729,
   Author = {Seli, P and Wammes, JD and Risko, EF and Smilek, D},
   Title = {On the relation between motivation and retention in
             educational contexts: The role of intentional and
             unintentional mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {23},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1280-1287},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0979-0},
   Abstract = {Highly motivated students often exhibit better academic
             performance than less motivated students. However, to date,
             the specific cognitive mechanisms through which motivation
             increases academic achievement are not well understood. Here
             we explored the possibility that mind wandering mediates the
             relation between motivation and academic performance, and
             additionally, we examined possible mediation by both
             intentional and unintentional forms of mind wandering. We
             found that participants reporting higher motivation to learn
             in a lecture-based setting tended to engage in less mind
             wandering, and that this decrease in mind wandering was in
             turn associated with greater retention of the lecture
             material. Critically, we also found that the influence of
             motivation on retention was mediated by both intentional and
             unintentional types of mind wandering. Not only do the
             present results advance our theoretical understanding of the
             mechanisms underlying the relation between motivation and
             academic achievement, they also provide insights into
             possible methods of intervention that may be useful in
             improving student retention in educational
             settings.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-015-0979-0},
   Key = {fds335729}
}

@article{fds335727,
   Author = {Ozubko, JD and Seli, P},
   Title = {Forget all that nonsense: The role of meaning during the
             forgetting of recollective and familiarity-based
             memories.},
   Journal = {Neuropsychologia},
   Volume = {90},
   Pages = {136-147},
   Year = {2016},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.06.026},
   Abstract = {Memory can be divided into recollection and familiarity.
             Recollection is characterized as the ability to vividly
             re-experience past events, and is believed to be supported
             by the hippocampus, whereas familiarity is defined as an
             undifferentiated feeling of knowing or acquaintance, and is
             believed to be supported by extra-hippocampal regions, such
             as the perirhinal cortex. Recent evidence suggests that the
             neural architectures of the hippocampus and neocortex lead
             information in these regions being susceptible to different
             forgetting processes. We expand on these accounts and
             propose that the neocortex may be sensitive to the semantic
             content of a trace, with more meaningful traces being more
             easily retained. The hippocampus, in contrast, is not
             hypothesized to be influenced by semantics in the same way.
             To test this new account, we use a continuous-recognition
             paradigm to examine the forgetting rates words and nonwords
             that are either recollected or familiar. We find that words
             and nonwords that are recollected are equally likely to be
             forgotten over time. However, nonwords that are familiar are
             more likely to be forgotten over time than are words that
             are familiar. Our results support recent
             neuropsychologically-based forgetting theories of
             recollection and familiarity and provide new insight into
             how and why representations are forgotten over
             time.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.06.026},
   Key = {fds335727}
}

@article{fds335726,
   Author = {Golchert, J and Smallwood, J and Jefferies, E and Seli, P and Huntenburg, JM and Liem, F and Lauckner, ME and Oligschläger, S and Bernhardt, BC and Villringer, A and Margulies,
             DS},
   Title = {Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering
             state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode,
             fronto-parietal, and limbic networks.},
   Journal = {NeuroImage},
   Volume = {146},
   Pages = {226-235},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025},
   Abstract = {Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with
             cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports
             the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a
             failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as
             well the apparently alternative view that control can
             facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content.
             We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises
             because of a failure to consider differences in the types of
             thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in
             particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using
             multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we
             examined the cortical organisation that underlies
             inter-individual differences in descriptions of the
             spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical
             thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses,
             implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions
             of the default-mode network for individuals who reported
             high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher
             reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with
             cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions
             in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control
             of cognition and attention) as well as heightened
             connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region
             that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral
             inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation
             in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus,
             with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being
             associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and
             higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in
             the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the
             intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on
             integration between the control and default-mode networks,
             with more deliberation being associated with greater
             integration between these systems. We conclude that one
             reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship
             with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts
             emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025},
   Key = {fds335726}
}

@article{fds335725,
   Author = {Seli, P and Risko, EF and Purdon, C and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Intrusive thoughts: linking spontaneous mind wandering and
             OCD symptomatology.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {81},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {392-398},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0756-3},
   Abstract = {One recent line of research in the literature on mind
             wandering has been concerned with examining rates of mind
             wandering in special populations, such as those
             characterized by attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,
             dysphoria, and schizophrenia. To best conceptualize mind
             wandering in studies examining special populations, it has
             recently been suggested that researchers distinguish between
             deliberate and spontaneous subtypes of this experience.
             Extending this line of research on mind wandering in special
             populations, in a large non-clinical sample (N = 2636), we
             examined how rates of deliberate and spontaneous mind
             wandering vary with symptoms of obsessive-compulsive
             disorder (OCD). Results indicate that, whereas deliberate
             mind wandering is not associated with OCD symptomatology,
             spontaneous mind wandering is, with higher reports of
             spontaneous mind wandering being associated with higher
             reports of OCD symptoms. We discuss the implications of
             these results for understanding both mind wandering and
             OCD.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-016-0756-3},
   Key = {fds335725}
}

@article{fds335723,
   Author = {Seli, P and Ralph, BCW and Konishi, M and Smilek, D and Schacter,
             DL},
   Title = {What did you have in mind? Examining the content of
             intentional and unintentional types of mind
             wandering.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {51},
   Pages = {149-156},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.03.007},
   Abstract = {It has recently been argued that researchers should
             distinguish between mind wandering (MW) that is engaged with
             and without intention. Supporting this argument, studies
             have found that intentional and unintentional MW have
             behavioral/neural differences, and that they are
             differentially associated with certain variables of
             theoretical interest. Although there have been considerable
             inroads made into the distinction between
             intentional/unintentional MW, possible differences in their
             content remain unexplored. To determine whether these two
             types of MW differ in content, we had participants complete
             a task during which they categorized their MW as intentional
             or unintentional, and then provided responses to questions
             about the content of their MW. Results indicated that
             intentional MW was more frequently rated as being
             future-oriented and less vague than unintentional MW. These
             findings shed light on the nature of intentional and
             unintentional MW and provide support for the argument that
             researchers should distinguish between intentional and
             unintentional types.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2017.03.007},
   Key = {fds335723}
}

@article{fds335724,
   Author = {Xu, M and Purdon, C and Seli, P and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Mindfulness and mind wandering: The protective effects of
             brief meditation in anxious individuals.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {51},
   Pages = {157-165},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.03.009},
   Abstract = {Mind wandering can be costly, especially when we are engaged
             in attentionally demanding tasks. Preliminary studies
             suggest that mindfulness can be a promising antidote for
             mind wandering, albeit the evidence is mixed. To better
             understand the exact impact of mindfulness on mind
             wandering, we had a sample of highly anxious undergraduate
             students complete a sustained-attention task during which
             off-task thoughts including mind wandering were assessed.
             Participants were randomly assigned to a meditation or
             control condition, after which the sustained-attention task
             was repeated. In general, our results indicate that
             mindfulness training may only have protective effects on
             mind wandering for anxious individuals. Meditation prevented
             the increase of mind wandering over time and ameliorated
             performance disruption during off-task episodes. In
             addition, we found that the meditation intervention appeared
             to promote a switch of attentional focus from the internal
             to present-moment external world, suggesting important
             implications for treating worrying in anxious
             populations.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2017.03.009},
   Key = {fds335724}
}

@article{fds335722,
   Author = {Seli, P and Maillet, D and Smilek, D and Oakman, JM and Schacter,
             DL},
   Title = {Cognitive aging and the distinction between intentional and
             unintentional mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychology and aging},
   Volume = {32},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {315-324},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000172},
   Abstract = {A growing number of studies have reported age-related
             reductions in the frequency of mind wandering. Here, at both
             the trait (Study 1) and state (Study 2) levels, we
             reexamined this association while distinguishing between
             intentional (deliberate) and unintentional (spontaneous)
             mind wandering. Based on research demonstrating
             age-accompanied deficits in executive functioning, we
             expected to observe increases in unintentional mind
             wandering with increasing age. Moreover, because aging is
             associated with increased task motivation, we reasoned that
             older adults might be more engaged in their tasks, and
             hence, show a more pronounced decline in intentional mind
             wandering relative to young adults. In both studies, we
             found that older adults did indeed report lower rates of
             intentional mind wandering compared with young adults.
             However, contrary to our expectations, we also found that
             older adults reported lower rates of unintentional mind
             wandering (Studies 1 and 2). We discuss the implications of
             these findings for theories of age-related declines in mind
             wandering. (PsycINFO Database Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/pag0000172},
   Key = {fds335722}
}

@article{fds335721,
   Author = {Maillet, D and Seli, P and Schacter, DL},
   Title = {Mind-wandering and task stimuli: Stimulus-dependent thoughts
             influence performance on memory tasks and are more often
             past- versus future-oriented.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {52},
   Pages = {55-67},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.04.014},
   Abstract = {Although many studies have indicated that participants
             frequently mind-wander during experimental tasks, relatively
             little research has examined the extent to which such
             thoughts are triggered by task stimuli (stimulus-dependent
             thoughts; SDTs) versus internally triggered
             (stimulus-independent thoughts; SITs). In the current
             experiment, we assessed differences in the frequency and
             characteristics of SDTs and SITs, as well as their
             associations with subsequent memory in young adults. Whereas
             frequency of SDTs (but not SITs) increased in a task with
             more meaningful stimuli, frequency of SITs (but not SDTs)
             increased in an easier task. Furthermore, only SDTs were
             more likely to be past- versus future-oriented. Finally,
             frequency and vividness of SDTs during a shallow, but not a
             deep, incidental encoding task both correlated with later
             memory performance for word stimuli. These results suggest
             that SDTs differ from SITs in several important
             ways.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2017.04.014},
   Key = {fds335721}
}

@article{fds335719,
   Author = {Seli, P and Ralph, BCW and Risko, EF and W Schooler and J and Schacter, DL and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Intentionality and meta-awareness of mind wandering: Are
             they one and the same, or distinct dimensions?},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1808-1818},
   Year = {2017},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1249-0},
   Abstract = {Researchers have recently demonstrated that mind-wandering
             episodes can vary on numerous dimensions, and it has been
             suggested that assessing these dimensions will play an
             important role in our understanding of mind wandering. One
             dimension that has received considerable attention in recent
             work is the intentionality of mind wandering. Although it
             has been claimed that indexing the intentionality of mind
             wandering will be necessary if researchers are to obtain a
             coherent understanding of the wandering mind, one concern is
             that this dimension might be redundant with another,
             longstanding, dimension: namely, meta-awareness. Thus, the
             utility of the argument for assessing intentionality rests
             upon a demonstration that this dimension is distinct from
             the meta-awareness dimension. To shed light on this issue,
             across two studies we compared and contrasted these
             dimensions to determine whether they are redundant or
             distinct. In both studies, we found support for the view
             that these dimensions are distinct.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-017-1249-0},
   Key = {fds335719}
}

@article{fds335717,
   Author = {Seli, P and Smilek, D and Ralph, BCW and Schacter,
             DL},
   Title = {The awakening of the attention: Evidence for a link between
             the monitoring of mind wandering and prospective
             goals.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. General},
   Volume = {147},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {431-443},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000385},
   Abstract = {Across 2 independent samples, we examined the relation
             between individual differences in rates of self-caught mind
             wandering and individual differences in temporal monitoring
             of an unrelated response goal. Rates of self-caught mind
             wandering were assessed during a commonly used
             sustained-attention task, and temporal goal monitoring was
             indexed during a well-established prospective-memory task.
             The results from both samples showed a positive relation
             between rates of self-caught mind wandering during the
             sustained-attention task and rates of checking a clock to
             monitor the amount of time remaining before a response was
             required in the prospective-memory task. This relation held
             even when controlling for overall propensity to mind-wander
             (indexed by intermittent thought probes) and levels of
             motivation (indexed by subjective reports). These results
             suggest the possibility that there is a common monitoring
             system that monitors the contents of consciousness and the
             progress of ongoing goals and tasks. (PsycINFO Database
             Record},
   Doi = {10.1037/xge0000385},
   Key = {fds335717}
}

@article{fds335715,
   Author = {Seli, P and Kane, MJ and Smallwood, J and Schacter, DL and Maillet, D and Schooler, JW and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Mind-Wandering as a Natural Kind: A Family-Resemblances
             View.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {479-490},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010},
   Abstract = {As empirical research on mind-wandering accelerates, we draw
             attention to an emerging trend in how mind-wandering is
             conceptualized. Previously articulated definitions of
             mind-wandering differ from each other in important ways, yet
             they also maintain overlapping characteristics. This
             conceptual structure suggests that mind-wandering is best
             considered from a family-resemblances perspective, which
             entails treating it as a graded, heterogeneous construct and
             clearly measuring and describing the specific aspect(s) of
             mind-wandering that researchers are investigating. We
             believe that adopting this family-resemblances approach will
             increase conceptual and methodological connections among
             related phenomena in the mind-wandering family and encourage
             a more nuanced and precise understanding of the many
             varieties of mind-wandering.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010},
   Key = {fds335715}
}

@article{fds335716,
   Author = {Seli, P and Carriere, JSA and Wammes, JD and Risko, EF and Schacter, DL and Smilek, D},
   Title = {On the Clock: Evidence for the Rapid and Strategic
             Modulation of Mind Wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychological science},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {8},
   Pages = {1247-1256},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797618761039},
   Abstract = {We examined the hypothesis that people can modulate their
             mind wandering on the basis of their expectations of
             upcoming challenges in a task. To this end, we developed a
             novel paradigm in which participants were presented with an
             analog clock, via a computer monitor, and asked to push a
             button every time the clock's hand was pointed at 12:00.
             Importantly, the time at which the clock's hand was pointed
             at 12:00 was completely predictable and occurred at 20-s
             intervals. During some of the 20-s intervals, we presented
             thought probes to index participants' rates of mind
             wandering. Results indicated that participants decreased
             their levels of mind wandering as they approached the
             predictable upcoming target. Critically, these results
             suggest that people can and do modulate their mind wandering
             in anticipation of changes in task demands.},
   Doi = {10.1177/0956797618761039},
   Key = {fds335716}
}

@article{fds335718,
   Author = {Laflamme, P and Seli, P and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Validating a visual version of the metronome response
             task.},
   Journal = {Behavior research methods},
   Volume = {50},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {1503-1514},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1020-0},
   Abstract = {The metronome response task (MRT)-a sustained-attention task
             that requires participants to produce a response in
             synchrony with an audible metronome-was recently developed
             to index response variability in the context of studies on
             mind wandering. In the present studies, we report on the
             development and validation of a visual version of the MRT
             (the visual metronome response task; vMRT), which uses the
             rhythmic presentation of visual, rather than auditory,
             stimuli. Participants completed the vMRT (Studies 1 and 2)
             and the original (auditory-based) MRT (Study 2) while also
             responding to intermittent thought probes asking them to
             report the depth of their mind wandering. The results showed
             that (1) individual differences in response variability
             during the vMRT are highly reliable; (2) prior to thought
             probes, response variability increases with increasing depth
             of mind wandering; (3) response variability is highly
             consistent between the vMRT and the original MRT; and (4)
             both response variability and depth of mind wandering
             increase with increasing time on task. Our results indicate
             that the original MRT findings are consistent across the
             visual and auditory modalities, and that the response
             variability measured in both tasks indexes a
             non-modality-specific tendency toward behavioral
             variability. The vMRT will be useful in the place of the MRT
             in experimental contexts in which researchers' designs
             require a visual-based primary task.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13428-018-1020-0},
   Key = {fds335718}
}

@article{fds338053,
   Author = {Seli, P and Konishi, M and Risko, EF and Smilek, D},
   Title = {The role of task difficulty in theoretical accounts of mind
             wandering.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {65},
   Pages = {255-262},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.08.005},
   Abstract = {Recent research has indicated that reducing the difficulty
             of a task by increasing the predictability of critical
             stimuli produces increases in intentional mind wandering,
             but, contrary to theoretical expectations, decreases in
             unintentional mind wandering. Here, we sought to determine
             whether reducing task difficulty by reducing working-memory
             load would yield similar results. Participants completed an
             easy (Choice Response Time; CRT) task and a relatively
             difficult (Working Memory; WM) task, and intermittently
             responded to thought probes asking about intentional and
             unintentional mind wandering. As in prior studies, we found
             higher rates of intentional mind wandering during the easy
             compared to the more difficult task. However, we also found
             more unintentional mind wandering during the difficult
             compared to the easy task. We discuss these results in the
             context of theoretical accounts of mind wandering.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2018.08.005},
   Key = {fds338053}
}

@article{fds339652,
   Author = {Seli, P and Beaty, RE and Cheyne, JA and Smilek, D and Oakman, J and Schacter, DL},
   Title = {How pervasive is mind wandering, really?},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {66},
   Pages = {74-78},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.10.002},
   Abstract = {Recent claims that people spend 30-50% of their waking lives
             mind wandering (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010; Kane et al.,
             2007) have become widely accepted and frequently cited.
             While acknowledging attention to be inconstant and wavering,
             and mind wandering to be ubiquitous, we argue and present
             evidence that such simple quantitative estimates are
             misleading and potentially meaningless without serious
             qualification. Mind-wandering estimates requiring
             dichotomous judgments of inner experience rely on
             questionable assumptions about how such judgments are made,
             and the resulting data do not permit straightforward
             interpretation. We present evidence that estimates of
             daily-life mind wandering vary dramatically depending on the
             response options provided. Offering participants a range of
             options in estimating task engagement yielded variable
             mind-wandering estimates, from approximately 60% to 10%,
             depending on assumptions made about how observers make
             introspective judgments about their mind-wandering
             experiences and how they understand what it means to be on-
             or off-task.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2018.10.002},
   Key = {fds339652}
}

@article{fds338054,
   Author = {Seli, P and Kane, MJ and Metzinger, T and Smallwood, J and Schacter, DL and Maillet, D and Schooler, JW and Smilek, D},
   Title = {The Family-Resemblances Framework for Mind-Wandering Remains
             Well Clad.},
   Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {11},
   Pages = {959-961},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.007},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.007},
   Key = {fds338054}
}

@article{fds342492,
   Author = {Seli, P and Beaty, RE and Marty-Dugas, J and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Depression, anxiety, and stress and the distinction between
             intentional and unintentional mind wandering},
   Journal = {Psychology of Consciousness: Theory Research, and
             Practice},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {163-170},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000182},
   Abstract = {We examined whether the previously documented association
             between mind wandering and affective dysfunction depends, at
             least to some extent, on whether mind wandering episodes are
             intentional or unintentional. In two large samples, we
             assessed trait-level rates of intentional and unintentional
             mind wandering, as well as three different types of
             affective dysfunction: depression, anxiety, and stress.
             Results indicated that, whereas unintentional mind wandering
             was uniquely positively associated with all three types of
             affective dysfunction, intentional mind wandering was
             uniquely (albeit very weakly) negatively associated with
             stress and anxiety and had no relation to depression. These
             findings indicate that people who more frequently engage in
             unintentional types of mind wandering are more likely to
             report symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress, and that
             intentional mind wandering may buffer against these types of
             affective dysfunction.},
   Doi = {10.1037/cns0000182},
   Key = {fds342492}
}

@article{fds338080,
   Author = {Beaty, RE and Seli, P and Schacter, DL},
   Title = {Network Neuroscience of Creative Cognition: Mapping
             Cognitive Mechanisms and Individual Differences in the
             Creative Brain.},
   Journal = {Current opinion in behavioral sciences},
   Volume = {27},
   Pages = {22-30},
   Publisher = {Elsevier BV},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.013},
   Abstract = {Network neuroscience research is providing increasing
             specificity on the contribution of large-scale brain
             networks to creative cognition. Here, we summarize recent
             experimental work examining cognitive mechanisms of network
             interactions and correlational studies assessing network
             dynamics associated with individual creative abilities. Our
             review identifies three cognitive processes related to
             network interactions during creative performance:
             <i>goal-directed memory retrieval</i>, <i>prepotent-response
             inhibitio</i>n, and <i>internally-focused attention</i>.
             Correlational work using prediction modeling indicates that
             functional connectivity between networks-particularly the
             executive control and default networks-can reliably predict
             an individual's creative thinking ability. We discuss
             potential directions for future network neuroscience,
             including assessing creative performance in specific domains
             and using brain stimulation to test causal hypotheses
             regarding network interactions and cognitive mechanisms of
             creative thought.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.013},
   Key = {fds338080}
}

@article{fds337407,
   Author = {Beaty, RE and Seli, P and Schacter, DL},
   Title = {Thinking about the past and future in daily life: an
             experience sampling study of individual differences in
             mental time travel.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {805-816},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {June},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1075-7},
   Abstract = {Remembering the past and imagining the future are hallmarks
             of mental time travel. We provide evidence that such
             experiences are influenced by individual differences in
             temporal and affective biases in cognitive style,
             particularly brooding rumination (a negative past-oriented
             bias) and optimism (a positive future-oriented bias).
             Participants completed a 7-day, cellphone-based
             experience-sampling study of temporal orientation and mental
             imagery. Multilevel models showed that individual
             differences in brooding rumination predicted less vivid and
             positive past- and future-oriented thoughts, even after
             controlling for depressed mood. People high in brooding
             rumination were also more likely to report thinking about a
             past experience when probed at random during the day.
             Conversely, optimists were more likely to report more vivid
             and positive future-oriented, but not past-oriented
             thoughts, although they did not report thinking more or less
             often about the past and future. The results suggest that
             temporal and affective biases in cognitive style influence
             how people think about the past and future in daily
             life.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-018-1075-7},
   Key = {fds337407}
}

@article{fds335720,
   Author = {Seli, P and Schacter, DL and Risko, EF and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Increasing participant motivation reduces rates of
             intentional and unintentional mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1057-1069},
   Year = {2019},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0914-2},
   Abstract = {We explored the possibility that increasing participants'
             motivation to perform well on a focal task can reduce mind
             wandering. Participants completed a sustained-attention task
             either with standard instructions (normal motivation), or
             with instructions informing them that they could be excused
             from the experiment early if they achieved a certain level
             of performance (higher motivation). Throughout the task, we
             assessed rates of mind wandering (both intentional and
             unintentional types) via thought probes. Results showed that
             the motivation manipulation led to significant reductions in
             both intentional and unintentional mind wandering as well as
             improvements in task performance. Most critically, we found
             that our simple motivation manipulation led to a dramatic
             reduction in probe-caught mind-wandering rates (49%)
             compared to a control condition (67%), which suggests the
             utility of motivation-based methods to reduce people's
             propensity to mind-wander.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-017-0914-2},
   Key = {fds335720}
}

@article{fds353536,
   Author = {Ho, NSP and Baker, D and Karapanagiotidis, T and Seli, P and Wang, HT and Leech, R and Bernhardt, B and Margulies, D and Jefferies, E and Smallwood, J},
   Title = {Missing the forest because of the trees: slower alternations
             during binocular rivalry are associated with lower levels of
             visual detail during ongoing thought.},
   Journal = {Neuroscience of consciousness},
   Volume = {2020},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {niaa020},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/nc/niaa020},
   Abstract = {Conscious awareness of the world fluctuates, either through
             variation in how vividly we perceive the environment, or
             when our attentional focus shifts away from information in
             the external environment towards information that we
             generate via imagination. Our study combined individual
             differences in experience sampling, psychophysical reports
             of perception and neuroimaging descriptions of structural
             connectivity to better understand these changes in conscious
             awareness. In particular, we examined (i) whether aspects of
             ongoing thought-indexed via multi-dimensional experience
             sampling during a sustained attention task-are associated
             with the white matter fibre organization of the cortex as
             reflected by their relative degree of anisotropic diffusion
             and (ii) whether these neurocognitive descriptions of
             ongoing experience are related to a more constrained measure
             of visual consciousness through analysis of bistable
             perception during binocular rivalry. Individuals with
             greater fractional anisotropy in right hemisphere white
             matter regions involving the inferior fronto-occipital
             fasciculus, the superior longitudinal fasciculus and the
             cortico-spinal tract, described their ongoing thoughts as
             lacking external details. Subsequent analysis indicated that
             the combination of low fractional anisotropy in these right
             hemisphere regions, with reports of thoughts with high
             levels of external details, was associated with the shortest
             periods of dominance during binocular rivalry. Since
             variation in binocular rivalry reflects differences between
             bottom-up and top-down influences on vision, our study
             suggests that reports of ongoing thoughts with vivid
             external details may occur when conscious precedence is
             given to bottom-up representation of perceptual
             information.},
   Doi = {10.1093/nc/niaa020},
   Key = {fds353536}
}

@article{fds335714,
   Author = {Ralph, BCW and Seli, P and Wilson, KE and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Volitional media multitasking: awareness of performance
             costs and modulation of media multitasking as a function of
             task demand.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {84},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {404-423},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1056-x},
   Abstract = {In two experiments, we sought to determine whether (a)
             people are aware of the frequently observed performance
             costs associated with engaging in media multitasking
             (Experiment 1), and (b) if so, whether they modulate the
             extent to which they engage in multitasking as a function of
             task demand (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, participants
             completed a high-demand task (2-back) both independently and
             while a video was simultaneously presented. To determine
             whether people were sensitive to the impact that the
             concurrent video had on primary-task performance, subjective
             estimates of performance were collected following both trial
             types (No-Video vs. Video trials), as were explicit beliefs
             about the influence of the video on performance. In
             Experiment 2, we modified our paradigm by allowing
             participants to turn the video on and off at their
             discretion, and had them complete either a high-demand task
             (2-back) or a low-demand task (0-back). Findings from
             Experiment 1 indicated that people are sensitive to the
             magnitude of the decrement that media multitasking has on
             primary-task performance. In addition, findings from
             Experiment 2 indicated that people modulate the extent to
             which they engage in media multitasking in accordance with
             the demands of their primary task. In particular,
             participants completing the high-demand task were more
             likely to turn off the optional video stream compared to
             those completing the low-demand task. The results suggest
             that people media multitask in a strategic manner by
             balancing considerations of task performance with other
             potential concerns.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-018-1056-x},
   Key = {fds335714}
}

@article{fds345891,
   Author = {Ralph, BCW and Smith, AC and Seli, P and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Yearning for distraction: Evidence for a trade-off between
             media multitasking and mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue
             canadienne de psychologie experimentale},
   Volume = {74},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {56-72},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cep0000186},
   Abstract = {We examined whether providing participants with the
             opportunity to media multitask influenced their tendency to
             be 'off-task.' More specifically, we were interested in
             whether providing participants with the opportunity to
             engage with an external media stream during a required
             (researcher-imposed) cognitive task might lead to a
             trade-off between mind wandering and engagement with
             external distractions (such as the media). We also examined
             the extent to which intentionality plays a role in these
             associations. Participants completed 2 phases of a cognitive
             task (1-back). During 1 phase, participants were provided
             the opportunity to concurrently watch a video while they
             performed the cognitive task; during the other, no such
             opportunity was provided. Throughout both phases, thought
             probes asked participants if they were (a) focused on the
             task, (b) attending to external distractions, or (c) mind
             wandering. If options 2 or 3 were selected, participants
             were further asked to report whether these forms of
             distraction were engaged intentionally or unintentionally.
             Our findings indicated that, although the opportunity to
             media multitask increased overall reports of being off-task,
             the tendency to mind-wander was significantly reduced in
             favour of attending to external distractions (such as the
             video). Of interest to the authors, overall reports of being
             unintentionally off-task were equivalent, irrespective of
             whether participants had the opportunity to media multitask
             or not, which suggests that the increased tendency to have
             an off-task locus of attention was because of intentionally
             shifting attention away from the primary task. (PsycINFO
             Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/cep0000186},
   Key = {fds345891}
}

@article{fds366422,
   Author = {Murray, S and Krasich, K and Schooler, JW and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {What's in a Task? Complications in the Study of the
             Task-Unrelated-Thought Variety of Mind Wandering.},
   Journal = {Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the
             Association for Psychological Science},
   Volume = {15},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {572-588},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691619897966},
   Abstract = {In recent years, the number of studies examining mind
             wandering has increased considerably, and research on the
             topic has spread widely across various domains of
             psychological research. Athough the term <i>mind
             wandering</i> has been used to refer to various cognitive
             states, researchers typically operationalize mind wandering
             in terms of <i>task-unrelated thought</i> (TUT). Research on
             TUT has shed light on the various task features that require
             people's attention and on the consequences of task
             inattention. Important methodological and conceptual
             complications do persist, however, in current investigations
             of TUT. We argue that these complications may be dampening
             the development of a more nuanced scientific account of TUT.
             In this article, we outline three of the more prominent
             methodological and conceptual complications in the
             literature on TUT and discuss potential directions for
             researchers to take as they move forward in their
             investigations of TUT.},
   Doi = {10.1177/1745691619897966},
   Key = {fds366422}
}

@article{fds349785,
   Author = {Struk, AA and Scholer, AA and Danckert, J and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Rich environments, dull experiences: how environment can
             exacerbate the effect of constraint on the experience of
             boredom.},
   Journal = {Cognition & emotion},
   Volume = {34},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1517-1523},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1763919},
   Abstract = {We examined the hypothesis that boredom is likely to occur
             when opportunity costs are high; that is, when there is a
             high potential value of engaging in activities other than
             the researcher-assigned activity. To this end, participants
             were either placed in a room with many possible affordances
             (e.g. a laptop, puzzle, etc.; <i>affordances condition;
             n = 121</i>), or they were ushered into an empty room
             (<i>control condition; n = 107</i>). In both conditions
             participants were instructed to entertain themselves with
             only their thoughts (hence, participants in the affordances
             condition were to refrain from engaging with the available
             options). As predicted, participants in the affordances
             condition reported higher levels of boredom compared with
             those in the control condition. Results suggest that under
             some conditions, environments that afford alternative
             activities may be <i>more</i> boring than those that are
             void of such activities.},
   Doi = {10.1080/02699931.2020.1763919},
   Key = {fds349785}
}

@article{fds351440,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Whitehead, PS and Sinnott-Armstrong, W and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Exposure to opposing reasons reduces negative impressions of
             ideological opponents},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {91},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {November},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104030},
   Abstract = {Americans have become increasingly likely to dislike,
             distrust, and derogate their ideological opponents on
             contemporary social and political issues. We hypothesized
             that a lack of exposure to compelling reasons, arguments,
             and evidence from ideological opponents might at least
             partly explain negative views of those opponents. Consistent
             with this hypothesis, we found that participants assume
             their ideological opponents, in comparison to their
             ideological allies, are less likely to have good reasons for
             their positions. Moreover, we found that the more strongly
             participants believe their opponents lack good reasons for
             their positions, the more likely they are to report that
             those opponents lack both intellectual capabilities and
             moral character. Critically, exposure to arguments favoring
             their opponents' position produced more favorable
             impressions of those opponents. We discuss possible
             implications of these results for the role of reasons and
             reasoning in political discourse, and for productive
             disagreement in a functioning democracy.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104030},
   Key = {fds351440}
}

@article{fds350350,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Sinclair, AH and Seli, P},
   Title = {Intellectual humility and perceptions of political
             opponents.},
   Journal = {Journal of personality},
   Volume = {88},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1196-1216},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12566},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objective</h4>Intellectual humility (IH) refers to the
             recognition that personal beliefs might be wrong. We
             investigate possible interpersonal implications of IH for
             how people perceive the intellectual capabilities and moral
             character of their sociopolitical opponents and for their
             willingness to associate with those opponents.<h4>Method</h4>In
             four initial studies (N = 1,926, M<sub>age</sub>  = 38,
             880 females, 1,035 males), we measured IH, intellectual and
             moral derogation of opponents, and willingness to befriend
             opponents. In two additional studies (N = 568,
             M<sub>age</sub>  = 40, 252 females, 314 males), we
             presented participants with a specific opponent on certain
             sociopolitical issues and several social media posts from
             that opponent in which he expressed his views on the issue.
             We then measured IH, intellectual, and moral derogation of
             the opponent, participants' willingness to befriend the
             opponent, participants' willingness to "friend" the opponent
             on social media, and participants' willingness to "follow"
             the opponent on social media.<h4>Results</h4>Low-IH relative
             to high-IH participants were more likely to derogate the
             intellectual capabilities and moral character of their
             opponents, less willing to befriend their opponents, and
             less willing to "friend" and "follow" an opponent on social
             media.<h4>Conclusions</h4>IH may have important
             interpersonal implications for person perception, and for
             understanding social extremism and polarization.},
   Doi = {10.1111/jopy.12566},
   Key = {fds350350}
}

@article{fds351200,
   Author = {Sinclair, AH and Stanley, ML and Seli, P},
   Title = {Closed-minded cognition: Right-wing authoritarianism is
             negatively related to belief updating following prediction
             error.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {6},
   Pages = {1348-1361},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {December},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01767-y},
   Abstract = {When confronted with information that challenges our
             beliefs, we must often learn from error in order to
             successfully navigate the world. Past studies in
             reinforcement learning and educational psychology have
             linked prediction error, a measure of surprise, to
             successful learning from feedback. However, there are
             substantial individual differences in belief-updating
             success, and the psychological factors that influence belief
             updating remain unclear. Here, we identify a novel factor
             that may predict belief updating: right-wing
             authoritarianism (RWA), which is characterized by a desire
             for order, structure, and preservation of social norms. We
             hypothesized that because people who score high on RWA are
             motivated to preserve entrenched beliefs, they may often
             fail to successfully update their beliefs when confronted
             with new information. Using a novel paradigm, we challenged
             participants' false beliefs and misconceptions to elicit
             prediction error. In two studies, we found consistent
             evidence that high-RWA individuals were less successful at
             correcting their false beliefs. Relative to low-RWA
             individuals, high-RWA individuals were less likely to revise
             beliefs in response to prediction error. We argue that RWA
             is associated with a relatively closed-minded cognitive
             style that negatively influences belief updating.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-020-01767-y},
   Key = {fds351200}
}

@article{fds350534,
   Author = {Forrin, ND and Mills, C and D’Mello, SK and Risko, EF and Smilek, D and Seli, P},
   Title = {TL;DR: Longer Sections of Text Increase Rates of
             Unintentional Mind-Wandering},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Education},
   Volume = {89},
   Number = {2},
   Pages = {278-290},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2020.1751578},
   Abstract = {The prevalence of the acronym tl;dr (“too long; didn’t
             read”) suggests that people intentionally disengage their
             attention from long sections of text. We studied this
             real-world phenomenon in an educational context by measuring
             rates of intentional and unintentional mind-wandering while
             undergraduate student participants (n = 80) read academic
             passages that were presented in either short sections of
             text (one sentence per screen) or relatively long sections
             (2–6 sentences per screen). We found that participants
             were significantly more likely to unintentionally disengage
             their attention while reading the longer sections of text,
             whereas intentional mind-wandering rates were equivalent
             across short and long sections of text. The difference in
             unintentional mind-wandering rates suggests that section
             length may serve as a cue that people use to assess the
             cost-benefit tradeoffs involved in attending to (or
             disengaging from) text. We conclude that instructors should
             avoid presenting electronic reading material in long
             sections of text.},
   Doi = {10.1080/00220973.2020.1751578},
   Key = {fds350534}
}

@article{fds352228,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Barr, N and Peters, K and Seli, P},
   Title = {Analytic-thinking predicts hoax beliefs and helping
             behaviors in response to the COVID-19 pandemic},
   Journal = {Thinking and Reasoning},
   Volume = {27},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {464-477},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1813806},
   Abstract = {The COVID-19 outbreak was labeled a global pandemic by the
             WHO in March of 2020. During that same month, the number of
             confirmed cases and the death rate grew exponentially in the
             United States, creating a serious public-health emergency.
             Unfortunately, many Americans dismissed the pandemic as a
             hoax and failed to properly engage in helpful behaviors like
             social-distancing and increased hand-washing. Here, we
             examine a disposition—engagement in analytic-thinking—that
             might predict beliefs that the pandemic is a hoax and
             failures to change behavior in positive ways during that
             critical early period in March. Our results indicate that
             individuals less likely to engage effortful, deliberative,
             and reflective cognitive processes were more likely to
             believe the pandemic was a hoax and less likely to have
             recently engaged in social-distancing and hand-washing in
             March. We discuss possible implications of these results for
             understanding and addressing the COVID-19
             pandemic.},
   Doi = {10.1080/13546783.2020.1813806},
   Key = {fds352228}
}

@article{fds356998,
   Author = {Brosowsky, NP and Van Tilburg and W and Scholer, AA and Boylan, J and Seli,
             P and Danckert, J},
   Title = {Boredom proneness, political orientation and adherence to
             social-distancing in the pandemic.},
   Journal = {Motivation and emotion},
   Volume = {45},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {631-640},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-021-09888-0},
   Abstract = {Research recently showed that boredom proneness was
             associated with increased social distancing rule-breaking in
             a sample collected early in the COVID-19 pandemic. Here we
             explore data collected early in the pandemic to examine what
             factors might drive this relation. We focus on political
             affiliation. Given the functional account of boredom as a
             call to action, we hypothesized that this urge to act may
             drive individuals towards outlets replete with symbolic
             value (e.g., ideology, identity). In addition, given the
             politicization of some social distancing rules (e.g., mask
             wearing), we explored whether those who adhere to strong
             political ideologies-particularly conservative
             ideologies-would be more likely to rule-break. Moderation
             analyses indicated that boredom proneness and social (but
             not fiscal) conservatism were indeed predictive of
             rule-breaking. These results highlight the need for both
             clear messaging emphasizing the strength of communal
             identity and action (i.e., that "We are all in this
             together") and for interventions that emphasize shared
             collective values in contexts that appeal directly to social
             conservatives.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s11031-021-09888-0},
   Key = {fds356998}
}

@article{fds346495,
   Author = {Ralph, BCW and Smith, AC and Seli, P and Smilek, D},
   Title = {The relation between task-unrelated media multitasking and
             task-related motivation.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {408-422},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01246-7},
   Abstract = {In two experiments, we explored the relation between
             participants' (a) levels of motivation to complete a task
             and (b) task-unrelated media multitasking. In Experiment 1,
             we examined the extent to which participants' levels of
             motivation to complete a task influenced their tendency to
             engage in task-unrelated media multitasking. Participants
             completed a 1-back task, while having the opportunity to
             turn on and off an unrelated, optional video. Results showed
             that participants who were told they would finish the
             experiment early if they achieved a sufficient level of
             performance (the motivated group) were significantly less
             likely to play the optional video during the 1-back task
             than those who were not given the opportunity to finish
             early (control condition). In Experiment 2, we examined the
             extent to which engaging in task-unrelated media
             multitasking affected task-related motivation. Three groups
             of participants completed a 1-back task, while (a) no video
             was presented, (b) a video was continuously played, or (c)
             participants could turn on and off a video at their leisure
             (as in Experiment 1). At both the beginning and the end of
             Experiment 2, participants were asked to indicate their
             level of motivation to complete the task. Interestingly,
             results revealed that continuously having the video playing
             helped sustain task-related motivation. Thus, although
             greater motivation to perform a task reduces the likelihood
             of engaging in task-unrelated media multitasking, such media
             multitasking also appears to increase levels of
             motivation.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-019-01246-7},
   Key = {fds346495}
}

@article{fds354515,
   Author = {Gross, ME and Smith, AP and Graveline, YM and Beaty, RE and Schooler,
             JW and Seli, P},
   Title = {Comparing the phenomenological qualities of
             stimulus-independent thought, stimulus-dependent thought and
             dreams using experience sampling.},
   Journal = {Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London.
             Series B, Biological sciences},
   Volume = {376},
   Number = {1817},
   Pages = {20190694},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0694},
   Abstract = {Humans spend a considerable portion of their lives engaged
             in 'stimulus-independent thoughts' (SIT), or mental activity
             that occurs independently of input from the immediate
             external environment. Although such SITs are, by definition,
             different from thoughts that are driven by stimuli in one's
             external environment (i.e. stimulus-dependent thoughts;
             SDTs), at times, the phenomenology of these two types of
             thought appears to be deceptively similar. But how similar
             are they? We address this question by comparing the content
             of two types of SIT (dreaming and waking SITs) with the
             content of SDTs. In this 7 day, smartphone-based
             experience-sampling procedure, participants were
             intermittently probed during the day and night to indicate
             whether their current thoughts were stimulus dependent or
             stimulus independent. They then responded to content-based
             items indexing the qualitative aspects of their experience
             (e.g. My thoughts were jumping from topic to topic). Results
             indicate substantial distinctiveness between these three
             types of thought: significant differences between at least
             two of the three mental states were found across every
             measured variable. Implications are discussed. This article
             is part of the theme issue 'Offline perception: voluntary
             and spontaneous perceptual experiences without matching
             external stimulation'.},
   Doi = {10.1098/rstb.2019.0694},
   Key = {fds354515}
}

@article{fds353330,
   Author = {Boylan, J and Seli, P and Scholer, AA and Danckert,
             J},
   Title = {Boredom in the COVID-19 pandemic: Trait boredom proneness,
             the desire to act, and rule-breaking.},
   Journal = {Personality and individual differences},
   Volume = {171},
   Pages = {110387},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110387},
   Abstract = {The state of boredom presents a conundrum: When bored, we
             want to engage with an activity, but we don't want to engage
             with whatever is currently available. This conflict is
             exacerbated when external factors impose restrictions on the
             range of behaviors we can engage in, which is precisely the
             scenario we are currently facing, at a global level, during
             this period of social isolation in response to the COVID-19
             pandemic. We collected data from 924 North American
             participants (530 Male, Mean age = 37.7 years) using the
             internet-based Mturk platform to examine the relation
             between self-reports of boredom proneness (using the Short
             Boredom-Proneness Scale) and individual responses to
             questions about compliance with social-distancing
             requirements during the COVID-19 pandemic. Our sample
             replicated recent findings in boredom research, including a
             negative correlation between boredom proneness and
             self-control. We also provide novel evidence that highly
             boredom prone people have been more likely to break the
             rules of social isolation in a variety of ways (e.g., fewer
             hours spent in social isolation, poor adherence to social
             distancing as evidenced by increased likelihood of holding a
             social gathering and coming into proximity with more people
             than recommended). We further demonstrated that boredom
             proneness substantially mediates the association between
             self-control and rule-breaking. These results indicate that
             boredom proneness is a critical factor to consider when
             encouraging adherence to social isolation.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.paid.2020.110387},
   Key = {fds353330}
}

@article{fds355487,
   Author = {Smallwood, J and Turnbull, A and Wang, H-T and Ho, NSP and Poerio, GL and Karapanagiotidis, T and Konu, D and Mckeown, B and Zhang, M and Murphy,
             C and Vatansever, D and Bzdok, D and Konishi, M and Leech, R and Seli, P and Schooler, JW and Bernhardt, B and Margulies, DS and Jefferies,
             E},
   Title = {The neural correlates of ongoing conscious
             thought.},
   Journal = {iScience},
   Volume = {24},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {102132},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2021.102132},
   Abstract = {A core goal in cognitive neuroscience is identifying the
             physical substrates of the patterns of thought that occupy
             our daily lives. Contemporary views suggest that the
             landscape of ongoing experience is heterogeneous and can be
             influenced by features of both the person and the context.
             This perspective piece considers recent work that explicitly
             accounts for both the heterogeneity of the experience and
             context dependence of patterns of ongoing thought. These
             studies reveal that systems linked to attention and control
             are important for organizing experience in response to
             changing environmental demands. These studies also establish
             a role of the default mode network beyond task-negative or
             purely episodic content, for example, implicating it in the
             level of vivid detail in experience in both task contexts
             and in spontaneous self-generated experiential states.
             Together, this work demonstrates that the landscape of
             ongoing thought is reflected in the activity of multiple
             neural systems, and it is important to distinguish between
             processes contributing to how the experience unfolds from
             those linked to how these experiences are
             regulated.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.isci.2021.102132},
   Key = {fds355487}
}

@article{fds353535,
   Author = {Frith, E and Elbich, DB and Christensen, AP and Rosenberg, MD and Chen,
             Q and Kane, MJ and Silvia, PJ and Seli, P and Beaty,
             RE},
   Title = {Intelligence and creativity share a common cognitive and
             neural basis.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. General},
   Volume = {150},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {609-632},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000958},
   Abstract = {Are intelligence and creativity distinct abilities, or do
             they rely on the same cognitive and neural systems? We
             sought to quantify the extent to which intelligence and
             creative cognition overlap in brain and behavior by
             combining machine learning of fMRI data and latent variable
             modeling of cognitive ability data in a sample of young
             adults (<i>N</i> = 186) who completed a battery of
             intelligence and creative thinking tasks. The study had 3
             analytic goals: (a) to assess contributions of specific
             facets of intelligence (e.g., fluid and crystallized
             intelligence) and general intelligence to creative ability
             (i.e., divergent thinking originality), (b) to model
             whole-brain functional connectivity networks that predict
             intelligence facets and creative ability, and (c) to
             quantify the degree to which these predictive networks
             overlap in the brain. Using structural equation modeling, we
             found moderate to large correlations between intelligence
             facets and creative ability, as well as a large correlation
             between general intelligence and creative ability (<i>r</i>
             = .63). Using connectome-based predictive modeling, we found
             that functional brain networks that predict intelligence
             facets overlap to varying degrees with a network that
             predicts creative ability, particularly within the
             prefrontal cortex of the executive control network. Notably,
             a network that predicted general intelligence shared 46% of
             its functional connections with a network that predicted
             creative ability-including connections linking executive
             control and salience/ventral attention networks-suggesting
             that intelligence and creative thinking rely on similar
             neural and cognitive systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c)
             2021 APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xge0000958},
   Key = {fds353535}
}

@article{fds354593,
   Author = {Brosowsky, NP and Murray, S and Schooler, JW and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Attention need not always apply: Mind wandering impedes
             explicit but not implicit sequence learning.},
   Journal = {Cognition},
   Volume = {209},
   Pages = {104530},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {April},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104530},
   Abstract = {According to the attentional resources account, mind
             wandering (or "task-unrelated thought") is thought to
             compete with a focal task for attentional resources. Here,
             we tested two key predictions of this account: First, that
             mind wandering should not interfere with performance on a
             task that does not require attentional resources; second,
             that as task requirements become automatized, performance
             should improve and depth of mind wandering should increase.
             Here, we used a serial reaction time task with implicit- and
             explicit-learning groups to test these predictions.
             Providing novel evidence for the attentional resource
             account's first prediction, results indicated that depth of
             mind wandering was negatively associated with learning in
             the explicit, but not the implicit, group, indicating that
             mind wandering is associated with impaired explicit, but not
             implicit, learning. Corroborating the attention resource
             account's second prediction, we also found that, overall,
             performance improved while at the same time depth of mind
             wandering increased. From an implicit-learning perspective,
             these results are consistent with the claim that explicit
             learning is impaired under attentional load, but implicit
             learning is not. Data, analysis code, manuscript preparation
             code, and pre-print available at osf.io/qzry7/.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104530},
   Key = {fds354593}
}

@article{fds349003,
   Author = {Seli, P and O'Neill, K and Carriere, JSA and Smilek, D and Beaty, RE and Schacter, DL},
   Title = {Mind-Wandering Across the Age Gap: Age-Related Differences
             in Mind-Wandering Are Partially Attributable to Age-Related
             Differences in Motivation.},
   Journal = {The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological
             sciences and social sciences},
   Volume = {76},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {1264-1271},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {August},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa031},
   Abstract = {<h4>Objectives</h4>A common finding in the mind-wandering
             literature is that older adults (OAs) tend to mind-wander
             less frequently than young adults (YAs). Here, we sought to
             determine whether this age-related difference in
             mind-wandering is attributable to age-related differences in
             motivation.<h4>Method</h4>YAs and OAs completed an attention
             task during which they responded to thought probes that
             assessed rates of mind-wandering, and they provided
             self-reports of task-based motivation before and after
             completion of the attention task.<h4>Results</h4>Age-related
             differences in mind-wandering are partially explained by
             differences in motivation, and motivating YAs via incentive
             diminishes mind-wandering differences across these
             groups.<h4>Discussion</h4>We consider these results in the
             context of theories on age-related differences in mind
             wandering, with a specific focus on their relevance to the
             recently proposed motivational account of such age-related
             differences.},
   Doi = {10.1093/geronb/gbaa031},
   Key = {fds349003}
}

@article{fds359684,
   Author = {Brosowsky, NP and Murray, S and Schooler, JW and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Thought dynamics under task demands: Evaluating the
             influence of task difficulty on unconstrained
             thought.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and
             performance},
   Volume = {47},
   Number = {9},
   Pages = {1298-1312},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000944},
   Abstract = {As research on mind wandering has accelerated, the
             construct's defining features have expanded and researchers
             have begun to examine different dimensions of mind
             wandering. Recently, Christoff and colleagues have argued
             for the importance of investigating a hitherto neglected
             variety of mind wandering: "unconstrained thought," or,
             thought that is relatively unguided by executive-control
             processes. To date, with only a few studies investigating
             unconstrained thought, little is known about this intriguing
             type of mind wandering. Across 2 experiments, we examined,
             for the first time, whether changes in task demand influence
             rates of constrained versus unconstrained thoughts. In both
             experiments, participants completed either an easy (0-back)
             or hard (2-back) task and responded to intermittently
             presented thought probes that gauged thought constraint
             throughout the task. In Experiment 1, we found that
             participants completing the easy task engaged in
             unconstrained thoughts more frequently than those completing
             the difficult task. In Experiment 2, we replicated this
             result and further demonstrated manipulations of
             unconstrained thought while also measuring task-relatedness
             (a common dimension of mind wandering). Finally, exploratory
             analyses showed associations between constrained thought and
             age, verbal intelligence, and an assessment of flow ('deep
             effortless concentration'), thereby adding further evidence
             to indicate a dissociation between task-relatedness and
             constraint. We discuss the methodological and theoretical
             applications of our findings to the burgeoning field of
             research on unconstrained thought. All data, analysis,
             article, and experiment code can be found at
             https://osf.io/wr2vk/ (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021
             APA, all rights reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xhp0000944},
   Key = {fds359684}
}

@article{fds365320,
   Author = {Murray, S and Liang, N and Brosowsky, N and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {What are the benefits of mind wandering to
             creativity?},
   Journal = {Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the
             Arts},
   Publisher = {American Psychological Association (APA)},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {September},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000420},
   Doi = {10.1037/aca0000420},
   Key = {fds365320}
}

@article{fds358357,
   Author = {Whitehead, PS and Mahmoud, Y and Seli, P and Egner,
             T},
   Title = {Mind wandering at encoding, but not at retrieval, disrupts
             one-shot stimulus-control learning.},
   Journal = {Attention, perception & psychophysics},
   Volume = {83},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {2968-2982},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-021-02343-9},
   Abstract = {The one-shot pairing of a stimulus with a specific cognitive
             control process, such as task switching, can bind the two
             together in memory. The episodic control-binding hypothesis
             posits that the formation of temporary stimulus-control
             bindings, which are held in event-files supported by
             episodic memory, can guide the contextually appropriate
             application of cognitive control. Across two experiments, we
             sought to examine the role of task-focused attention in the
             encoding and implementation of stimulus-control bindings in
             episodic event-files. In Experiment 1, we obtained
             self-reports of mind wandering during encoding and
             implementation of stimulus-control bindings. Results
             indicated that, whereas mind wandering during the
             implementation of stimulus-control bindings does not
             decrease their efficacy, mind wandering during the encoding
             of these control-state associations interferes with their
             successful deployment at a later point. In Experiment 2, we
             complemented these results by using trial-by-trial
             pupillometry to measure attention, again demonstrating that
             attention levels at encoding predict the subsequent
             implementation of stimulus-control bindings better than
             attention levels at implementation. These results suggest
             that, although encoding stimulus-control bindings in
             episodic memory requires active attention and engagement,
             once encoded, these bindings are automatically deployed to
             guide behavior when the stimulus recurs. These findings
             expand our understanding of how cognitive control processes
             are integrated into episodic event files.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13414-021-02343-9},
   Key = {fds358357}
}

@article{fds352390,
   Author = {O'Neill, K and Smith, AP and Smilek, D and Seli, P},
   Title = {Dissociating the freely-moving thought dimension of
             mind-wandering from the intentionality and task-unrelated
             thought dimensions.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {85},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {2599-2609},
   Year = {2021},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01419-9},
   Abstract = {The recently forwarded family-resemblances framework of
             mind-wandering argues that mind-wandering is a
             multidimensional construct consisting of a variety of
             exemplars. On this view, membership in the mind-wandering
             family is graded along various dimensions that define more
             or less prototypical instances of mind-wandering. In recent
             work, three dimensions that have played a prominent role in
             defining prototypicality within the mind-wandering family
             include: (a) task-relatedness (i.e., how related the content
             of a thought is to an ongoing task), (b) intentionality
             (i.e., whether thought is deliberately or spontaneously
             engaged), and (c) thought constraint (i.e., how much
             attention constrains thought dynamics). One concern,
             however, is that these dimensions may be redundant with each
             other. The utility of distinguishing among these different
             dimensions of mind-wandering rests upon a demonstration that
             they are dissociable. To shed light on this issue, we
             indexed the task-relatedness, intentionality, and constraint
             dimensions of thought during the completion of a laboratory
             task to evaluate how these dimensions relate to each other.
             We found that 56% of unconstrained thoughts were "on-task"
             and that 23% of constrained thoughts were "off-task."
             Moreover, we found that rates of off-task thought, but not
             "freely-moving" (i.e., unconstrained) thought, varied as a
             function of expected changes in task demands, confirming
             that task-relatedness and thought constraint are separable
             dimensions. Participants also reported 21% of intentional
             off-task thoughts that were freely moving and 9% of
             unintentional off-task thoughts that were constrained.
             Finally, off-task thoughts were more likely to be
             freely-moving than unintentional. Taken together, the
             results suggest that these three dimensions of
             mind-wandering are not redundant with one
             another.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-020-01419-9},
   Key = {fds352390}
}

@article{fds363784,
   Author = {Lopata, JA and Barr, N and Slayton, M and Seli, P},
   Title = {Dual-Modes of Creative Thought in the Classroom:
             Implications of Network Neuroscience for Creativity
             Education},
   Journal = {Translational Issues in Psychological Science},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {79-89},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tps0000317},
   Abstract = {Students experience varying engagement levels and modes of
             thought in educational contexts, and educators have
             substantial influence on those attributes of student
             engagement. When designing lessons, educators typically
             utilize Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives;
             however, there are calls for education planning to be more
             empirically grounded and supportive of creativity. Previous
             to the past decade, creativity neuroscientists have focused
             on small-scale regional attributions of cognitive processes
             to specific brain areas. The most recent decade, however,
             has seen a trend toward a large-scale view attributing
             cognitive processes to whole-brain networks. Several
             networks have been identified, and two—the default mode
             network and executive control network—have been implicated
             in creative cognition. These networks appear to interact,
             and neuroscientists have associated them with dual-process
             models delineating generative and evaluative phases of the
             creative process, type I and type II cognitive processes,
             and associative and analytic modes of thought. In the
             current article, we draw on cognitive science and creativity
             neuroscience research to advance the notion that modes of
             creative thought are legitimate and important
             creativitysupportive learning objectives. Given that
             creativity is a 21st century educational priority, it is
             imperative for educators to begin considering how to design
             creativitysupportive learning experiences with these modes
             at the front of their minds. Recommendations for educational
             practice and future research directions are
             provided.},
   Doi = {10.1037/tps0000317},
   Key = {fds363784}
}

@article{fds355374,
   Author = {Smith, AC and Brosowsky, NP and Ralph, BCW and Smilek, D and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Re-examining the effect of motivation on intentional and
             unintentional task-unrelated thought: accounting for thought
             constraint produces novel results.},
   Journal = {Psychological research},
   Volume = {86},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {87-97},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {February},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01487-5},
   Abstract = {It has been proposed that motivating participants to perform
             well on a cognitive task ought to lead to decreases in rates
             of intentional, but not unintentional, task-unrelated
             thought (TUT; a commonly studied variety of mind wandering).
             However, at odds with this prediction, research has found
             that increasing motivation results in decreases in both
             intentional and unintentional TUTs. One possible explanation
             for this surprising finding is that standard assessments of
             TUT may inadvertently conflate TUTs with another variety of
             mind wandering: unconstrained thought. If so, then
             deconfounding task-unrelated and unconstrained varieties of
             mind wandering might produce the predicted effect of a
             decrease in intentional, but not unintentional, TUT when
             motivation is increased. To explore this possibility, in the
             present study, participants completed a sustained-attention
             task after receiving standard instructions
             (normal-motivation condition) or instructions informing them
             that they could leave the study early if they achieved a
             certain level of performance (motivated condition).
             Throughout the task, we assessed rates of TUT (both
             intentional and unintentional) and unconstrained thoughts.
             Consistent with prior work, the results indicated that
             motivated participants reported being on-task significantly
             more frequently than non-motivated participants. However,
             unlike previous work, we found that when deconfounding TUTs
             and unconstrained thoughts, participants in the motivation
             condition reported significantly fewer bouts of intentional
             TUT than those in the non-motivation condition, but no
             differences in rates of unintentional TUT were observed
             between groups. These results suggest that (a) motivation
             specifically targets intentional TUT and (b) standard
             assessments of TUT conflate task-relatedness and thought
             constraint.},
   Doi = {10.1007/s00426-021-01487-5},
   Key = {fds355374}
}

@article{fds362204,
   Author = {Brosowsky, NP and Smith, AC and Smilek, D and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {On the relation between mind wandering, PTSD symptomology,
             and self-control.},
   Journal = {Consciousness and cognition},
   Volume = {99},
   Pages = {103288},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2022.103288},
   Abstract = {Here we examined the association between mind wandering,
             post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomology, and
             self-control. In a large undergraduate sample (N = 5,387),
             we assessed trait-levels of spontaneous and deliberate mind
             wandering, self-control, and PTSD symptomology. Results
             indicated that, while PTSD symptomology was uniquely
             positively associated with spontaneous mind wandering, it
             was negatively associated with deliberate mind wandering and
             self-control. These findings suggest that the mechanism(s)
             underlying everyday mind wandering may also underlie PTSD
             symptomology and traumatic intrusions. Moreover, the unique
             negative association between PTSD symptomatology and
             self-control suggests that PTSD is characterized not only by
             impairments in inhibiting unwanted thoughts (as indexed by
             mind wandering), but also by impairments in inhibiting other
             unwanted behaviors.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.concog.2022.103288},
   Key = {fds362204}
}

@article{fds362484,
   Author = {Brosowsky, NP and Barr, N and Mugon, J and Scholer, AA and Seli, P and Danckert, J},
   Title = {Creativity, Boredom Proneness and Well-Being in the
             Pandemic.},
   Journal = {Behavioral sciences (Basel, Switzerland)},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {68},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/bs12030068},
   Abstract = {Throughout the course of the pandemic, it has become clear
             that the strictures of social isolation and various levels
             of lockdown constraints have impacted people's well-being.
             Here, our aim was to explore relations between trait
             dispositions associated with boredom proneness,
             self-regulation and well-being using data collected early in
             the pandemic. Specifically, we explored whether the tendency
             to engage in everyday creative pursuits (e.g., making your
             own greeting cards) would act as a prophylactic against poor
             well-being. Results showed that well-being was higher for
             those individuals who increased engagement with creative
             pursuits during the early stages of the pandemic. That is,
             people who engaged more in everyday creative activities also
             reported higher levels of self-esteem, optimism, and
             positive affect. In contrast, those who pursued fewer
             creative outlets had higher levels of depression and
             anxiety, were higher in boredom proneness, and reported
             experiencing more negative affect. As we emerge from the
             pandemic, these data provide a clue as to how people might
             plan to cope adaptively with the restrictive circumstances
             this extreme world event engendered. More generally, these
             data provide support for the notion that everyday creativity
             (and not necessarily creative expertise) has positive
             associations for well-being.},
   Doi = {10.3390/bs12030068},
   Key = {fds362484}
}

@article{fds362784,
   Author = {Ragnhildstveit, A and Slayton, M and Jackson, LK and Brendle, M and Ahuja, S and Holle, W and Moore, C and Sollars, K and Seli, P and Robison,
             R},
   Title = {Ketamine as a Novel Psychopharmacotherapy for Eating
             Disorders: Evidence and Future Directions.},
   Journal = {Brain sciences},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {382},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12030382},
   Abstract = {Eating disorders (EDs) are serious, life-threatening
             psychiatric conditions associated with physical and
             psychosocial impairment, as well as high morbidity and
             mortality. Given the chronic refractory nature of EDs and
             the paucity of evidence-based treatments, there is a
             pressing need to identify novel approaches for this
             population. The noncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor
             (NMDAr) antagonist, ketamine, has recently been approved for
             treatment-resistant depression, exerting rapid and robust
             antidepressant effects. It is now being investigated for
             several new indications, including obsessive-compulsive,
             post-traumatic, and substance use disorder, and shows
             transdiagnostic potential for EDs, particularly among
             clinical nonresponders. Hence, the aim of this review is to
             examine contemporary findings on the treatment of EDs with
             ketamine, whether used as a primary, adjunctive, or
             combination psychopharmacotherapy. Avenues for future
             research are also discussed. Overall, results are
             encouraging and point to therapeutic value; however, are
             limited to case series and reports on anorexia nervosa.
             Further empirical research is thus needed to explore
             ketamine efficacy across ED subgroups, establish safety
             profiles and optimize dosing, and develop theory-driven,
             targeted treatment strategies at the individual patient
             level.},
   Doi = {10.3390/brainsci12030382},
   Key = {fds362784}
}

@article{fds363702,
   Author = {Smith, AP and Brosowsky, N and Murray, S and Daniel, R and Meier, ME and Seli, P},
   Title = {Fixation, flexibility, and creativity: The dynamics of mind
             wandering.},
   Journal = {Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and
             performance},
   Volume = {48},
   Number = {7},
   Pages = {689-710},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001012},
   Abstract = {The mind-wandering literature is long on results and short
             on theory. One notable exception is the <i>Dynamic
             Framework,</i> a theoretical framework that characterizes
             mind wandering as thoughts that are relatively unconstrained
             from deliberate and automatic sources, or "freely moving."
             Critically, this framework makes numerous testable
             predictions, including (a) a positive association between
             freely moving thought and ADHD, (b) negative associations
             between freely moving thought and depression, anxiety, and
             OCD, and (c) a positive association between freely moving
             thought and divergent thinking ability. In Study 1, to test
             these predictions, we measured participants' reports of
             freely moving thoughts during a cognitive task and assessed
             divergent thinking and various psychopathological symptoms.
             Results failed to support any of the Dynamic Framework's
             predictions. In Study 2, we assessed the predicted relations
             between freely moving thought and divergent-thinking
             performance by manipulating thought constraint during a
             creative-incubation interval that preceded a
             divergent-thinking task. Here, we found some evidence
             (albeit very weak) to support the Dynamic Framework's
             prediction. Finally, in Study 3, we examined the possibility
             that indexing freely moving thought <i>during</i> a
             divergent-thinking task would yield the predicted
             associations but failed to find support for these
             associations. These results, most of which are at odds with
             the predictions of the Dynamic Framework, suggest either the
             need to revise the framework and/or that current methods are
             inadequate to properly test these predictions. (PsycInfo
             Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights
             reserved).},
   Doi = {10.1037/xhp0001012},
   Key = {fds363702}
}

@article{fds363196,
   Author = {Stanley, ML and Whitehead, PS and Marsh, EJ and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Prior exposure increases judged truth even during periods of
             mind wandering.},
   Journal = {Psychonomic bulletin & review},
   Volume = {29},
   Number = {5},
   Pages = {1997-2007},
   Year = {2022},
   Month = {October},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02101-4},
   Abstract = {Much of our day is spent mind-wandering-periods of
             inattention characterized by a lack of awareness of external
             stimuli and information. Whether we are paying attention or
             not, information surrounds us constantly-some true and some
             false. The proliferation of false information in news and
             social media highlights the critical need to understand the
             psychological mechanisms underlying our beliefs about what
             is true. People often rely on heuristics to judge the truth
             of information. For example, repeated information is more
             likely to be judged as true than new information (i.e., the
             illusory truth effect). However, despite the prevalence of
             mind wandering in our daily lives, current research on the
             contributing factors to the illusory truth effect have
             largely ignored periods of inattention as experimentally
             informative. Here, we aim to address this gap in our
             knowledge, investigating whether mind wandering during
             initial exposure to information has an effect on later
             belief in the truth of that information. That is, does the
             illusory truth effect occur even when people report not
             paying attention to the information at hand. Across three
             studies we demonstrate that even during periods of mind
             wandering, the repetition of information increases truth
             judgments. Further, our results suggest that the severity of
             mind wandering moderated truth ratings, such that greater
             levels of mind wandering decreased truth judgements for
             previously presented information.},
   Doi = {10.3758/s13423-022-02101-4},
   Key = {fds363196}
}

@article{fds352862,
   Author = {Brosowsky, NP and DeGutis, J and Esterman, M and Smilek, D and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Mind Wandering, Motivation, and Task Performance Over Time:
             Evidence That Motivation Insulates People From the Negative
             Effects of Mind Wandering},
   Journal = {Psychology of Consciousness: Theory Research, and
             Practice},
   Volume = {10},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {475-486},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000263},
   Abstract = {In the current study, we examined whether participant
             motivation was associated with fluctuations of attentional
             engagement and performance over time. We gauged
             participants’ motivation and depth of mind wandering as
             they completed the metronome response task to determine
             whether fluctuations in inattention (indexed by task
             performance and depth of mind wandering) would be related to
             fluctuations in motivation. As in prior work, we found that,
             with increasing time on task, (a) self-reported depth of
             mind wandering increased, (b) task performance decreased,
             and (c) motivation waned. Extending this work, we found an
             interaction between motivation and mind wandering such that
             mind wandering was negatively associated with task
             performance when motivation was low, but unrelated to
             performance when motivation was high. These results suggest
             that motivation may help improve task performance by
             reducing the depth of mind wandering, while also providing
             insulation from the negative effects of mind wandering, when
             it does occur.},
   Doi = {10.1037/cns0000263},
   Key = {fds352862}
}

@article{fds362396,
   Author = {Jansen, EJ and Danckert, J and Seli, P and Scholer,
             AA},
   Title = {Under pressure: Locomotion and assessment in the COVID-19
             pandemic},
   Journal = {Self and Identity},
   Volume = {22},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {1-18},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2022.2036635},
   Abstract = {The COVID-19 pandemic poses unique opportunities to explore
             how fundamental self-regulatory variables affect responses
             to the pandemic. We examine how two critical self-regulatory
             orientations, locomotion and assessment, relate to
             psychological distress and obeying public health guidelines
             using secondary data analysis. In the initial pandemic
             stages (April and May, 2020), North American participants (N
             = 924) completed measures of chronic locomotion and
             assessment, pandemic behaviors and feelings, and various
             individual-differences. Analyses revealed that assessment,
             but not locomotion, was indirectly associated with greater
             pandemic rule-breaking and psychological distress through
             the fear of missing out, difficulty engaging in activities,
             and engagement in negative activities. We discuss why the
             vulnerabilities of assessment, and not locomotion, may be
             particularly sensitive to pandemic-related
             constraints.},
   Doi = {10.1080/15298868.2022.2036635},
   Key = {fds362396}
}

@article{fds367264,
   Author = {Smith, AC and Brosowsky, NP and Caron, EE and Seli, P and Smilek,
             D},
   Title = {Examining the relation between mind wandering and unhealthy
             eating behaviours},
   Journal = {Personality and Individual Differences},
   Volume = {200},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111908},
   Abstract = {In the present study, we explored how individual differences
             in the tendency to mind-wander are related to unhealthy
             eating behaviours (i.e., eating habits and eating-disorder
             symptoms). Given that eating-disorders are associated with
             inhibition (extreme control) and impulsivity (a lack of
             control), we were interested in how unhealthy eating
             behaviours might relate to both spontaneous mind-wandering,
             which is often construed as a failure of executive control,
             and deliberate mind-wandering, which is thought to occur via
             controlled processes. To ensure that any observed relations
             were not driven by self-control, we also measured and
             statistically controlled for this variable. In a large,
             non-clinical sample (N = 2328), regression analyses
             predicting each of the eating measures with self-control,
             spontaneous mind-wandering, and deliberate mind-wandering
             revealed that self-control and spontaneous mind-wandering
             were significantly positively predictive of unhealthy eating
             behaviours, whereas deliberate mind-wandering did not
             significantly predict these measures. These findings suggest
             that spontaneous, but not deliberate, mind-wandering has a
             robust unique relation with unhealthy eating behaviours,
             even when controlling for self-control.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.paid.2022.111908},
   Key = {fds367264}
}

@article{fds369859,
   Author = {Ragnhildstveit, A and Kaiyo, M and Snyder, MB and Jackson, LK and Lopez,
             A and Mayo, C and Miranda, AC and August, RJ and Seli, P and Robison, R and Averill, LA},
   Title = {Cannabis-assisted psychotherapy for complex dissociative
             posttraumatic stress disorder: A case report.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychiatry},
   Volume = {14},
   Pages = {1051542},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1051542},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>A dissociative subtype of posttraumatic
             stress disorder, known as "D-PTSD", has been included in the
             Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth
             Edition. In addition to meeting criteria for PTSD, patients
             endorse prominent dissociative symptoms, namely
             depersonalization and derealization, or detachment from
             one's self and surroundings. At present, this population is
             supported by a highly heterogeneous and undeveloped
             literature. Targeted interventions are therefore lacking,
             and those indicated for PTSD are limited by poor efficacy,
             delayed onset of action, and low patient engagement. Here,
             we introduce cannabis-assisted psychotherapy (CAP) as a
             novel treatment for D-PTSD, drawing parallels to psychedelic
             therapy.<h4>Case presentation</h4>A 28-year-old female
             presented with complex D-PTSD. In a naturalistic setting,
             she underwent 10 sessions of CAP, scheduled twice monthly
             over 5 months, coupled with integrative cognitive behavioral
             therapy. An autonomic and relational approach to CAP was
             leveraged, specifically psychedelic somatic interactional
             psychotherapy. Acute effects included oceanic boundlessness,
             ego dissolution, and emotional breakthrough. From baseline
             to post-treatment, the patient showed a 98.5% reduction in
             pathological dissociation, as measured by the
             Multidimensional Inventory of Dissociation, no longer
             meeting criteria for D-PTSD. This was accompanied by
             decreased cognitive distractibility and emotional suffering,
             as well as increased psychosocial functioning. Anecdotally,
             the patient has sustained improvements for over 2 years to
             date.<h4>Conclusions</h4>There is urgency to identify
             treatments for D-PTSD. The present case, while inherently
             limited, underscores the potential of CAP as a therapeutic
             option, leading to robust and sustained improvement.
             Subjective effects were comparable to those produced by
             classic and non-classic psychedelics, such as psilocybin and
             ketamine. Further research is warranted to explore,
             establish, and optimize CAP in D-PTSD, and to characterize
             its role in the pharmacological landscape.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1051542},
   Key = {fds369859}
}

@article{fds372414,
   Author = {Bellaiche, L and Smith, AP and Barr, N and Christensen, A and Williams,
             C and Ragnhildstveit, A and Schooler, J and Beaty, R and Chatterjee, A and Seli, P},
   Title = {Back to the basics: Abstract painting as an index of
             creativity},
   Journal = {Creativity Research Journal},
   Volume = {35},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {698-713},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2023.2243100},
   Abstract = {Researchers have invested a great deal in creating reliable,
             “gold-standard” creativity assessments that can be
             administered in controlled laboratory settings, though these
             efforts have come at the cost of not using ecologically and
             face-valid tasks. To help fill this critical gap, we
             developed and implemented a novel, face-valid paradigm that
             required participants to paint abstract pieces of art, which
             were later rated for creative quality. We first sought to
             evaluate whether there was good convergence among creativity
             ratings provided by independent raters. Next, we examined
             whether its measure of creativity correlated with (a)
             existing creativity measures and (b) individual traits (e.g.
             openness, fluid intelligence) that are typically correlated
             with indices of creativity. Our findings indicate that our
             abstract-painting paradigm is feasible to implement
             (independent ratings of the creativity of the paintings
             converged well), and that its measure of creativity
             significantly correlated with some of the gold-standard
             indices of creativity (thereby providing convergent
             validity). These findings suggest that having participants
             engage in abstract painting provides a valid index of
             creativity, thereby opening new opportunities for future
             research to index a more-face-valid measure of
             creativity.},
   Doi = {10.1080/10400419.2023.2243100},
   Key = {fds372414}
}

@article{fds374573,
   Author = {Ragnhildstveit, A and Khan, R and Seli, P and Bass, LC and August, RJ and Kaiyo, M and Barr, N and Jackson, LK and Gaffrey, MS and Barsuglia, JP and Averill, LA},
   Title = {5-MeO-DMT for post-traumatic stress disorder: a real-world
             longitudinal case study.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychiatry},
   Volume = {14},
   Pages = {1271152},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1271152},
   Abstract = {Psychedelic therapy is, arguably, the next frontier in
             psychiatry. It offers a radical alternative to longstanding,
             mainstays of treatment, while exciting a paradigm shift in
             translational science and drug discovery. There is
             particular interest in 5-methoxy-<i>N,N</i>-dimethyltryptamine
             (5-MeO-DMT)-a serotonergic psychedelic-as a novel,
             fast-acting therapeutic. Yet, few studies have directly
             examined 5-MeO-DMT for trauma- or stress-related
             psychopathology, including post-traumatic stress disorder
             (PTSD). Herein, we present the first longitudinal case study
             on 5-MeO-DMT for chronic refractory PTSD, in a 23-year-old
             female. A single dose of vaporized bufotoxin of the Sonoran
             Desert Toad (<i>Incilius alvarius</i>), containing an
             estimated 10-15 mg of 5-MeO-DMT, led to clinically
             significant improvements in PTSD, with next-day effects.
             This was accompanied by marked reductions in hopelessness
             and related suicide risk. Improvements, across all
             constructs, were sustained at 1-, 3-, 6-, and 12-months
             follow-up, as monitored by a supporting clinician. The
             subject further endorsed a complete mystical experience,
             hypothesized to underly 5-MeO-DMT's therapeutic activity. No
             drug-related, serious adverse events occurred. Together,
             results showed that 5-MeO-DMT was generally tolerable, safe
             to administer, and effective for PTSD; however, this was not
             without risk. The subject reported acute nausea,
             overwhelming subjective effects, and late onset of night
             terrors. Further research is warranted to replicate and
             extend these findings, which are inherently limited,
             non-generalizable, and rely on methods not clinically
             accepted.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1271152},
   Key = {fds374573}
}

@article{fds369746,
   Author = {Brendle, M and Ragnhildstveit, A and Slayton, M and Smart, L and Cunningham, S and Zimmerman, MH and Seli, P and Gaffrey, MS and Averill,
             LA and Robison, R},
   Title = {Registered clinical trials investigating ketamine and
             esketamine for treatment-resistant depression: A systematic
             review},
   Journal = {Journal of Psychedelic Studies},
   Volume = {6},
   Number = {3},
   Pages = {176-187},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/2054.2022.00234},
   Abstract = {Background and Aims: Ketamine and esketamine have garnered
             interest in both psychiatric research and clinical practice
             for treatment-resistant depression (TRD). In this review, we
             examined registered trials investigating the therapeutic use
             of ketamine or esketamine for TRD, with the aim of
             characterizing emerging trends and knowledge gaps. Methods:
             The ClinicalTrials.gov electronic registry and results
             database was queried from inception to February 5, 2022,
             adhering to elements of the PRISMA guideline, we evaluated
             trial eligibility in the qualitative synthesis. Data
             regarding study design, drug regimens, and measures were
             subsequently abstracted and descriptively analyzed. Results:
             The search returned 86 records, of which 56 trials were
             included in the final review. The number of trials
             investigating ketamine and esketamine for TRD increased
             since 2008, with higher peaks observed in 2015 (n = 9) and
             2021 (n = 9). Most trials were Phase 2 (13, 23.2%) or Phase
             3 (11, 19.6%), gathering preliminary data on efficacy and/or
             further data on safety and efficacy with variant dosing and
             pharmacological approaches. By and large, trials examined
             ketamine and esketamine as individual versus combination
             treatments (45% and 25%, respectively). The
             Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) was most
             commonly used to assess clinical outcomes (75%).
             Conclusions: There are increasingly large-scale and
             late-phase trials of esketamine over ketamine for TRD,
             coupled with efforts to centralize evidence on these
             medications. Yet several trials do not assess patient
             characteristics that may affect treatment response, such as
             age, sex, and race. By understanding these design
             limitations, scientists and clinicians can avoid research
             waste and funding bodies can judiciously direct support
             towards high priority research.},
   Doi = {10.1556/2054.2022.00234},
   Key = {fds369746}
}

@article{fds364337,
   Author = {Landry, AP and Schooler, JW and Willer, R and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Reducing Explicit Blatant Dehumanization by Correcting
             Exaggerated Meta-Perceptions},
   Journal = {Social Psychological and Personality Science},
   Volume = {14},
   Number = {4},
   Pages = {407-418},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {May},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/19485506221099146},
   Abstract = {If explicitly, blatantly dehumanizing a group of
             people—overtly characterizing them as less than
             human—facilitates harming them, then reversing this
             process is paramount. Addressing dehumanization among
             American political partisans appears especially crucial,
             given that it has been linked to their anti-democratic
             hostility. Perhaps because of its overt nature, partisans
             recognize—and greatly exaggerate—the extent to which
             out-partisans explicitly, blatantly dehumanize them. Past
             research has found that when people perceive they are
             dehumanized by an outgroup (i.e., meta-dehumanization), they
             respond with reciprocal dehumanization. Therefore, we
             reasoned that partisans’ dehumanization could be reduced
             by correcting their exaggerated meta-dehumanization. Indeed,
             across three preregistered studies (N = 4,154), an
             intervention correcting American partisans’ exaggerated
             meta-dehumanization reduced their own dehumanization of
             out-partisans. This decreased dehumanization persisted at a
             1-week follow-up and predicted downstream reductions in
             partisans’ anti-democratic hostility, suggesting that
             correcting exaggerated meta-dehumanization can durably
             mitigate the dark specter of dehumanization.},
   Doi = {10.1177/19485506221099146},
   Key = {fds364337}
}

@article{fds371795,
   Author = {Bellaiche, L and Shahi, R and Turpin, MH and Ragnhildstveit, A and Sprockett, S and Barr, N and Christensen, A and Seli,
             P},
   Title = {Humans versus AI: whether and why we prefer human-created
             compared to AI-created artwork.},
   Journal = {Cognitive research: principles and implications},
   Volume = {8},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {42},
   Year = {2023},
   Month = {July},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-023-00499-6},
   Abstract = {With the recent proliferation of advanced artificial
             intelligence (AI) models capable of mimicking human
             artworks, AI creations might soon replace products of human
             creativity, although skeptics argue that this outcome is
             unlikely. One possible reason this may be unlikely is that,
             independent of the physical properties of art, we place
             great value on the imbuement of the human experience in art.
             An interesting question, then, is whether and why people
             might prefer human-compared to AI-created artworks. To
             explore these questions, we manipulated the purported
             creator of pieces of art by randomly assigning a
             "Human-created" or "AI-created" label to paintings actually
             created by AI, and then assessed participants' judgements of
             the artworks across four rating criteria (Liking, Beauty,
             Profundity, and Worth). Study 1 found increased positive
             judgements for human- compared to AI-labelled art across
             all criteria. Study 2 aimed to replicate and extend Study 1
             with additional ratings (Emotion, Story, Meaningful, Effort,
             and Time to create) intended to elucidate why people
             more-positively appraise Human-labelled artworks. The main
             findings from Study 1 were replicated, with narrativity
             (Story) and perceived effort behind artworks (Effort)
             moderating the label effects ("Human-created" vs.
             "AI-created"), but only for the sensory-level judgements
             (Liking, Beauty). Positive personal attitudes toward
             AI moderated label effects for more-communicative
             judgements (Profundity, Worth). These studies demonstrate
             that people tend to be negatively biased against AI-created
             artworks relative to purportedly human-created artwork, and
             suggest that knowledge of human engagement in the artistic
             process contributes positively to appraisals of
             art.},
   Doi = {10.1186/s41235-023-00499-6},
   Key = {fds371795}
}

@article{fds376292,
   Author = {Petranker, R and Anderson, T and Fewster, EC and Aberman, Y and Hazan,
             M and Gaffrey, M and Seli, P},
   Title = {Keeping the promise: a critique of the current state of
             microdosing research.},
   Journal = {Frontiers in psychiatry},
   Volume = {15},
   Pages = {1217102},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1217102},
   Abstract = {<h4>Introduction</h4>The practice of taking small,
             sub-hallucinogenic doses of psychedelics, known as
             microdosing, has exploded in popularity over the last
             decade. Users claim benefits ranging from improved mood and
             enhanced creativity to an increased sense of meaning and
             connectedness in life. While research on microdosing is
             still lagging behind the shift in public opinion, several
             papers have been published in the last five years which
             attempted to assess the effects of microdosing.<h4>Methods</h4>This
             review paper aimed to critically analyze the research
             practices used in the recent wave of microdosing research:
             We reviewed 15 papers published before the closing date of
             this review in March 2022.<h4>Results</h4>Our review
             concludes that it is premature to draw any conclusions about
             the efficacy or safety of microdosing since the research
             quality cannot be considered confirmatory.<h4>Discussion</h4>We
             propose some potential causes for the current state of the
             literature and some suggestions for how these causes may be
             ameliorated.},
   Doi = {10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1217102},
   Key = {fds376292}
}

@article{fds376092,
   Author = {Landry, AP and Seli, P},
   Title = {A family-resemblances framework for dehumanization
             research},
   Journal = {Current Research in Ecological and Social
             Psychology},
   Volume = {6},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2024.100185},
   Abstract = {Dehumanization has figured prominently in intergroup
             discrimination and violence, which has inspired sustained
             social-psychological inquiry. Over two decades, researchers
             have brought an abundance of theories and methods to bear on
             the empirical study of dehumanization. Collectively, this
             work has painted an expansive portrait of the many ways we
             can overlook or deny the humanity of others. At the same
             time, these diverse conceptual and measurement approaches
             have progressed in relative isolation, which has created
             confusion about what, precisely, is meant by
             “dehumanization” and cast uncertainty on fundamental
             conclusions drawn from this research. To stimulate
             theoretical development and more-productive exchanges across
             the field, we offer a Family-Resemblances perspective on
             dehumanization research. Specifically, we conceptualize
             dehumanization as a multifaceted construct that encompasses
             a family of related processes with both overlapping and
             unique features. Thus, the diverse theoretical and
             methodological approaches to studying dehumanization are
             complementary means of capturing a fundamentally
             heterogeneous phenomenon. Further, we argue that this
             perspective can catalyze a more nuanced and precise
             understanding of dehumanization's many facets: by specifying
             the different varieties of dehumanization under
             investigation, the field can more precisely map them onto
             specific targets, causes, consequences, and intervention
             strategies.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.cresp.2024.100185},
   Key = {fds376092}
}

@article{fds376908,
   Author = {Cárdenas-Egúsquiza, AL and Seli, P and Berntsen,
             D},
   Title = {Associations Between Autobiographical Memory and Dreaming:
             An Individual-Differences Approach},
   Journal = {Dreaming},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {January},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/drm0000264},
   Abstract = {Autobiographical memory and dreaming are ubiquitous in
             everyday life. The study of their relation has largely been
             assessed using experimental approaches, abstracting from
             individual differences, despite evidence of stable
             individual differences in both mental processes. Here, we
             examined, for the first time, whether individual differences
             in the recollective experience of autobiographical memory
             (measured by the Autobiographical Recollection Test [ART];
             Berntsen et al., 2019) are associated with individual
             differences in dreaming (measured by the Inventory of Dream
             Experiences and Attitudes, BeaulieuPrévost et al., 2009) in
             a sample of 246 participants. The ART showed consistent and
             robust associations with five out of seven aspects of
             dreaming, demonstrating that the way people generally
             remember their personal past is reliably related to the way
             they experience their dreams. The findings provide new
             perspectives on the role of autobiographical memory in
             dreaming as well as on the continuity hypothesis of
             dreaming.},
   Doi = {10.1037/drm0000264},
   Key = {fds376908}
}

@article{fds374608,
   Author = {Landry, AP and Fincher, K and Barr, N and Brosowsky, NP and Protzko, J and Ariely, D and Seli, P},
   Title = {Harnessing dehumanization theory, modern media, and an
             intervention tournament to reduce support for retributive
             war crimes},
   Journal = {Journal of Experimental Social Psychology},
   Volume = {111},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2023.104567},
   Abstract = {We demonstrate how psychological scientists can curate
             rich-yet-accessible media to intervene on
             conflict-escalating attitudes during the earliest stages of
             violent conflicts. Although wartime atrocities all-too-often
             ignite destructive cycles of tit-for-tat war crimes,
             powerful third parties can de-escalate the bloodshed.
             Therefore, following Russia's illegal invasion of Ukraine,
             we aimed to reduce Americans' support for committing
             retributive war crimes against Russian soldiers. To
             intervene during the earliest stages of the invasion, we
             drew on theories of dehumanization and “parasocial”
             intergroup contact to curate publicly available media
             expected to humanize Russian soldiers. We then identified
             the most effective materials by simultaneously evaluating
             all of them with an intervention tournament. This allowed us
             to quickly implement a psychological intervention that
             reliably reduced support for war crimes during the first
             days of a momentous land war. Our work provides a practical,
             result-driven model for developing psychological
             interventions with the potential to de-escalate incipient
             conflicts.},
   Doi = {10.1016/j.jesp.2023.104567},
   Key = {fds374608}
}

@article{fds376752,
   Author = {Ragnhildstveit, A and Tuteja, N and Seli, P and Smart, L and Uzun, N and Bass, LC and Miranda, AC and Ford, TJ and Neufeld,
             SAS},
   Title = {Transitions from child and adolescent to adult mental health
             services for eating disorders: an in-depth systematic review
             and development of a transition framework.},
   Journal = {Journal of eating disorders},
   Volume = {12},
   Number = {1},
   Pages = {36},
   Year = {2024},
   Month = {March},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40337-024-00984-3},
   Abstract = {<h4>Background</h4>Eating disorders (EDs) peak in
             mid-to-late adolescence and often persist into adulthood.
             Given their early onset and chronicity, many patients
             transition from child and adolescent mental health services
             (CAMHS) to adult mental health services (AMHS) for ongoing,
             speciality ED care. This transition typically occurs at
             18 years of age, when important biological, psychosocial,
             and vocational changes take place. Thus, smooth and
             effective transitions are paramount for ensuring service
             continuity, as well as reducing the risk of ED relapse and
             premature death. Here, we synthesized evidence on
             transitions from CAMHS to AMHS for young people with EDs,
             aiming to inform future research, clinical practice, and
             healthcare policy.<h4>Methods</h4>A systematic review of the
             literature was conducted. This adhered to PRISMA guidelines.
             PubMed, Embase, and Scopus electronic databases were queried
             from inception to December 3, 2023. Leveraging the PICOS
             framework, study eligibility was evaluated in the
             qualitative synthesis. Data regarding methodology, analytic
             approach, and associated outcomes were then extracted. The
             quality of evidence was examined using critical appraisal
             tools. Finally, concept mapping was applied to organize
             findings into a transition framework.<h4>Results</h4>The
             search returned 76 articles. Of these, 14 were included in
             the final review. Articles were grouped into 'qualitative'
             (n = 10), 'cross-sectional' (n = 2), and
             'longitudinal cohort' (n = 2) studies based on
             research design. Overall, ED transitions were complex,
             multifaceted, and challenging for patients, caregivers, and
             providers alike. This resulted from an interplay of
             temporal- (e.g., timing of ED onset and transition),
             stakeholder- (e.g., patient ambivalence towards recovery)
             and systemic- (e.g., differences between services) related
             factors. Most studies were of moderate-to-high quality.
             Findings informed the development of five
             transition strategies designed to facilitate effective
             transfers across ED care: Timely talks, Readiness,
             Inclusion, Preparation, and Synergy (TRIPS).<h4>Conclusions</h4>Transitions
             from CAMHS to AMHS appear problematic for young people with
             EDs and other involved stakeholders. The field stands to
             benefit from TRIPS, an actionable, evidence-based framework
             that aims to alleviate challenges of transitioning and
             subsequently improve ED trajectories. As a logical next
             step, future work should empirically test the TRIPS
             framework, exploring its predictive utility and clinical
             value.},
   Doi = {10.1186/s40337-024-00984-3},
   Key = {fds376752}
}


%% Chapters in Books   
@misc{fds337196,
   Author = {Wammes, JD and Seli, P and Smilek, D},
   Title = {Mind-wandering in educational settings},
   Pages = {259-272},
   Booktitle = {The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering,
             Creativity, and Dreaming},
   Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
   Year = {2018},
   Month = {April},
   ISBN = {9780190464745},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.15},
   Abstract = {Recently, there has been a growing interest in exploring the
             influence of mind- wandering on learning in educational
             settings. In considering the available research on the
             topic, one might draw the following conclusions: the
             prevalence of unintentional mind- wandering in classroom
             settings is high; mindwandering rates increase over time in
             lectures; and mind- wandering interferes with learning.
             Although research in the extant literature provides ample
             support for these conclusions, much of this research was
             conducted in the laboratory, while participants viewed
             video- recorded lectures. More recently, however,
             researchers have examined the effects of intentional and
             unintentional mind- wandering in live- classroom settings,
             and, as this chapter reveals, such research has produced
             some results that are at odds with those produced in
             laboratory- based studies. The chapter discusses these
             recent findings in the context of the aforementioned
             potential conclusions, and concludes that findings from the
             laboratory do not readily generalize to real- world
             educational settings.},
   Doi = {10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.15},
   Key = {fds337196}
}

@misc{fds362205,
   Author = {Barr, N and Beaty, R and Seli, P},
   Title = {Autonomy and control across cognition: Insights from
             creativity, memory, mind wandering, and reasoning
             research},
   Pages = {25-54},
   Booktitle = {Creativity and the Wandering Mind: Spontaneous and
             Controlled Cognition},
   Year = {2020},
   Month = {January},
   ISBN = {9780128164006},
   url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-816400-6.00002-X},
   Abstract = {Increasing amounts of behavioral and neuroscientific
             evidence support a view in which creativity arises as a
             result of an interaction between associative and executive
             processes (Beaty, Benedek, Silvia, & Schacter, 2016; Beaty,
             Silvia, Nusbaum, Jauk, & Benedek, 2014). Although much
             progress has been made in this area, the precise nature of
             the interplay among different modes of thought in creative
             thinking requires further specification (see Sowden,
             Pringle, & Gabora, 2014). In this chapter, it is argued that
             advances in this area can be facilitated by connecting
             creativity research to other cognitive literatures that make
             similar delineations among types of thought, but whose
             evolution has progressed relatively independently.
             Theoreticians in the areas of mind wandering, memory, and
             reasoning have all independently argued for the utility of
             distinguishing processing and thinking that is
             unintentional, spontaneous, and autonomous from that which
             is intentional, deliberate, and controlled (Evans and
             Stanovich, 2013; Hintzman, 2011; Seli, Risko, Smilek, &
             Schacter, 2016). By connecting creativity research to these
             literatures, this chapter aids theoretical refinement and
             integration, makes suggestions for future empirical
             research, and helps to further ground the study of creation
             in cognition.},
   Doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-816400-6.00002-X},
   Key = {fds362205}
}


Duke University * Arts & Sciences * Faculty * Staff * Grad * Postdocs * Reload * Login